Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

English possessive ’s: clitic and affix

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The English possessive marker displays properties of both clitic and affix. I argue that synchronically it is, in fact, both, rather than only one or the other or something halfway between. I show that it is possible to model the dual clitic/affix status of the possessive in the framework of Lexical-Functional Grammar, and that this model is capable of accounting for the full range of constructions in which the possessive marker appears. This also has consequences for questions of English noun phrase syntax, and issues of categoriality and degrammaticalization.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
€34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (Germany)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Explore related subjects

Discover the latest articles and news from researchers in related subjects, suggested using machine learning.

Notes

  1. Besides the literature cited in this paper, see also the works referenced in Allen (2008), Anderson (2008), Payne (2009).

  2. The distinction is discussed in detail by Anderson et al. (2006).

  3. Phrasal affixes were in fact proposed by Luís and Sadler (2003) to account for European Portuguese clitics in an LFG framework, but their model was quickly modified, and the phrasal affix element dropped (Luís and Otoguro 2004, 2005).

  4. On what is actually found in spoken PDE, see Scott et al. (2007) and Denison et al. (2010). The latter find only five examples of the phrasal possessive in the British National Corpus (BNC), i.e. 0.04 % of all possessive constructions; they argue this gap cannot be ascribed to a general lack of postmodification with nouns. They argue that its infrequency is due to the use of avoidance strategies: the of-construction, and the previously unidentified ‘split genitive’, discussed below (Sect. 6.2).

  5. I return to this issue in a slightly different context in Sect. 3.1.1 below.

  6. As pointed out to me by the editor, in at least some North American dialects (5a) is in fact more acceptable than (4a), whereas (5b) is consistently bad.

  7. For example, Börjars et al. (2013) argue that the clitic-affix distinction is multi-dimensional and that the mental grammar has access to probabilities, statistical preferences, in relation to different phrases occurring in different environments, so that graded/statistical grammaticality judgements result from interaction between competing constraints, in a stochastic model. They attempt no formal modeling, but it is clear that possessive ’s still occupies a single position within this framework of probabilities and constraints.

  8. Besides the work of Wescoat, Lexical Sharing is also utilized in LFG by Broadwell (2007, 2008) and Alsina (2010). Broadwell (2008) also notes that the approach of Bresnan and Mugane (2006) to mixed categories is in some ways similar to LS, permitting a single word to instantiate two nodes, but only in the case of morphologically ‘mixed’ forms.

  9. LS therefore represents an instantiation in LFG terms of ‘multidominance’ in syntactic representation, i.e. the idea that one word or node may be dominated by more than one mother node. Multidominance is found also in other syntactic theories, such as Minimalism (cf. Citko and Gračanin-Yuksek 2013:5ff., with references). In LFG it is absolutely restricted to adjacent mother nodes; this is not necessarily the case in other theories, but it is, for example, in the Representation Theory of Williams (2003) and the ‘Spanning’ theory within Nanosyntax (Ramchand 2008; Svenonius 2011).

  10. The evidence provided by Wescoat (2005), which I am able only to summarize here, is unambiguous within the lexicalist and modular architectural assumptions of LFG, but it may be noted that in other frameworks it could be possible to derive some of these features without assuming an affix.

  11. LS is relevant only to the analysis of portmanteau words that pattern with two-word sequences. Given LFG’s lexicalism, where portmanteau words pattern with single words of greater morphological complexity, as with English went beside regular past tense forms like walk-ed, no special analysis is required since both the morphologically complex and the portmanteau words are treated as single lexical items.

  12. As pointed out by a reviewer, this ‘constrained’ approach to LS renders impossible Wescoat’s (2009) analysis of Udi ‘endoclitic’ person markers, since no allomorphy whatsoever is involved.

  13. The confusion between the two arises partly because of this diachronic origin of (at least some) LS phenomena, and also partly because in both cases one is dealing with single phonological words, so again the correct analysis is not always immediately obvious.

  14. For recent discussion of the s-string, its representation and place in the grammar, see Dalrymple and Mycock (2011) and Mycock and Lowe (2013).

  15. The one possible question mark over this reformulation is that \(\pi\) becomes a relation, not a function, which threatens the overall concept of a function from form to meaning (cf. Asudeh 2006). This potential problem can be avoided, however, as long as each annotation on a ‘shared’ lexical entry is explicitly assigned to only one of the c-structure exponents. In this way the ‘path’ through the relation \(\pi\) is specified, such that it still effectively works as a function.

  16. For an overview of the DP hypothesis, see Zlatic (1997:Chap. 1) and Bernstein (2001).

  17. Cf. also Zribi-Hertz (1997).

  18. This difference from the movement-based tradition partly stems from the fact that in LFG features like agreement and discourse function are captured not in the constituent structure but at other levels of the grammar. More widely on the formalization of noun phrases in LFG, see Laczkó (1995).

  19. I restrict discussion purely to contexts involving the possessive marker; more widely on the arguments for and against the DP in PDE, see Payne (1993).

  20. There are semantic restrictions on this construction, the details of which are not universally agreed upon and which are not relevant to the syntactic analysis; cf. Payne (2013:179) with references.

  21. In more complex analyses of noun phrase syntax only the definite determiner is assumed to appear in D, but in the original form of the DP hypothesis, where there is only one functional head in the noun phrase, all determiners must be assumed to appear there.

  22. Barker (1998) among others argues that this is the correct analysis of the construction, on the grounds that the prepositional phrase here is partitive, not possessive. Such an analysis would be entirely consistent with the model proposed here, though it is not necessary, as shown below.

  23. And is, of course, implied by affixal analyses of the possessive such as those of Zwicky (1987), Lapointe (1990), Miller (1991) and Payne (2009). The most recent clitic-based analysis that reflects this intuition is that of Peters and Westerståhl (2013).

  24. The DP hypothesis has more widely been subject to theoretical criticism, for example by Payne (1993), Meyers (1995), Bruening (2009), and cf. further Spencer (1992), Hudson (1987), Payne and Huddleston (2002).

  25. Furthermore, they do not exclude the possibility that the nominal syntax of English is more complicated, perhaps involving functional projections (other than DP) above NP, when it comes to dealing with, for example, quantified noun phrases, which I make no attempt to deal with here.

  26. For non-projecting categories, cf. Toivonen (2003); the circumflex representation for non-projecting categories comes originally from Asudeh (2002).

  27. I assume the functional attribute poss for possessors, but nothing would in principle prevent an alternative functional analysis, for example subj, as proposed by Chisarik and Payne (2001).

  28. For the sake of avoiding a lengthy digression I pass over the details of the semantic structure here, but it is worth noting that I assume throughout that c-structure possessors and ‘of’ phrases always appear in the f-structure as poss and adj respectively, and that more specific thematic relations are dealt with in the semantic structure. That is, for example, in the NP Napoleon’s destruction of the city, Napoleon is functionally a possessor and only semantically a subject (i.e. an agent), and likewise the city is functionally the obj within an adjunct headed by of, and only semantically the object (i.e. the patient) of the event noun.

  29. It would be possible to assume an f-structure attribute such as possform, in order to ensure that the ’s were a functional co-head, parallel to compform with CPs, but I do not consider this necessary.

  30. A technical consequence of Kaplan’s s-string is that the arrow ↑ commonly used in lexical entries must be interpreted somewhat differently from when it appears as an annotation on c-structure nodes. Specifically, ↑ in a lexical entry is not \(\hat{\ast}_{\phi}\) but \(\bullet_{\pi\phi}\), where • refers to the current s-string element (cf. Mycock and Lowe 2013:453–454). Really, then, ↑ (and ↓) should be avoided in lexical entries, and alternative abbreviations used, but for the sake of simplicity I retain the traditional notation.

  31. The specification in the lexical entry here is essentially equivalent to Wescoat’s lexical instantiation rules.

  32. The lexical entry specifies the c-structure categories with which the word is associated, and the relative order in which they must appear. The hierarchical relations between the two nodes are not specified, but will be determined by the phrase structure rules of the language (including the rules given above, 16–20).

  33. For my own treatment of this phenomenon, see below.

  34. In the approach presented in this paper, we must account for examples like (36) by assuming that 3sg. present tense verb forms parallel regular plural nouns and words like species in having an affixal (lexically shared) possessive form. Admittedly, this requires a certain amount of lexical duplication, but not as much as analyses in which every verb form (and preposition, etc.) must have a lexically specified possessive form.

  35. Cf. the similar criticism of Anderson’s proposals by Payne (2009:328).

  36. Or we may not: Plank (1985) considers the zero genitive with regular plurals to be the regular outcome of the inherited genitive plural marker -a; this zero marker became an allomorph of ’s when the latter was extended to irregular plurals (having been originally exclusively singular).

  37. Given that the head N of an NP cannot regularly be omitted in English.

  38. Plank (1992) notes that things are more complex than simply an either/or determiner/modifier analysis; cf. also Haspelmath (1999).

  39. On the alternation in the form of possessive pronouns see also Barker (1998) and Bernstein and Tortora (2005).

  40. In phrases such as my old one(s), the pronoun one will contribute a pred pro feature to the possessed f-structure (i.e. the f-structure of one). *Mine old one(s) is then impossible because pred values are uniquely instantiated; so although both mine and one specify the same value (i.e. pro) for the possessed f-structure’s pred, a pred clash results, since it is only permitted for a pred value to be specified once.

  41. Anderson (2013) discusses coordinated possessive phrases like Rachel’s and Alice’s husbands, arguing that these are problematic for an analysis in which possessive ’s has its own node in the syntactic structure. However, even under the traditional DP hypothesis that Anderson assumes, a coordinated structure similar (mutatis mutandis) to that in (49) could account for such phrases, and under the analysis of noun phrase syntax assumed here can be analysed without difficulty by assuming coordination of possessive DPs in Spec, NP.

  42. I know of no examples in which both reanalyses occur, e.g. “the man’s with the ducks’s car.” This may simply be due to the rarity of the double marking construction; there is nothing in the analysis proposed here that would exclude its existence.

  43. Interestingly, Allen (2013) shows that ordinary genitive case constructions in Old English cannot underlie the split possessive in Middle English and, by implication, PDE, which indirectly supports an analysis whereby the Middle English/PDE split possessive is not a pure affixal construction.

References

  • Abney, Steven. 1987. The English noun phrase in its sentential aspect. PhD diss, MIT Press.

  • Alexiadou, Artemis. 2004. On the development of possessive determiners. In Diachronic clues to synchronic grammar, eds. Eric Fußand Carola Trips, 31–58. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, Cynthia L. 2008. Genitives in early English: typology and evidence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, Cynthia L. 2013. Dealing with postmodified possessors in early English: split and group genitives. In Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession, eds. Kersti Börjars, David Denison, and Alan Scott, 1–34. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Alsina, Alex. 2010. The Catalan definite article as lexical sharing. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 10, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 5–25. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Stephen R. 2005. Aspects of the theory of clitics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Stephen R. 2008. The English “group genitive” is a special clitic. English Linguistics 25: 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Stephen R. 2013. The marker of the English “group genitive” is a special clitic, not an inflection. In Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession, eds. Kersti Börjars, David Denison, and Alan Scott, 193–218. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Stephen R., Lea Brown, Alice Gaby, and Jacqueline Lecarme. 2006. Life on the edge: there’s morphology there after all! Lingue e linguaggio 5(1): 33–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asudeh, Ash. 2002. The syntax of preverbal particles and adjunction in Irish. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 02, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asudeh, Ash. 2006. Direct compositionality and the architecture of LFG. In Intelligent linguistic architectures: variations on themes by Ronald M. Kaplan, eds. Miriam Butt, Mary Dalrymple, and Tracy Holloway King, 363–387. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asudeh, Ash. 2012. The logic of pronominal resumption. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, Chris. 1998. Partitives, double genitives and anti-uniqueness. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 16: 679–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, Judy B. 2001. The DP hypothesis: identifying clausal properties in the nominal domain. In The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory, eds. Mark Baltin and Chris Collins, 536–561. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, Judy B., and Christina Tortora. 2005. Two types of possessive forms in English. Lingua 115(9): 1221–1242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Börjars, Kersti. 2003. Morphological status and (de)grammaticalization: the Swedish possessive. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 26(2): 133–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Börjars, Kersti, David Denison, and Alan Scott, eds. 2013. Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Börjars, Kersti, John Payne, and Erika Chisarik. 1999. On the justification for functional categories in LFG. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 99, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Börjars, Kersti, David Denison, Grzegorz Krajewski, and Alan Scott. 2013. Expression of possession in English. In Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession, eds. Kersti Börjars, David Denison, and Alan Scott, 123–148. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brame, M. 1982. The head-selector theory of lexical specifications and the nonexistence of coarse categories. Linguistic Analysis 10(4): 321–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, Joan. 2001. Lexical-functional syntax. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, Joan, and John Mugane. 2006. Agentive nominalizations in Gĩkũyũ and the theory of mixed categories. In Intelligent linguistic architectures: variations on themes by Ronald M. Kaplan, eds. Miriam Butt, Mary Dalrymple, and Tracy Holloway King, 201–234. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadwell, George Aaron. 2007. Lexical sharing and non-projecting words: the syntax of Zapotec adjectives. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 07, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 87–106. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadwell, George Aaron. 2008. Turkish suspended affixation is lexical sharing. In Lexical functional grammar (LFG) 08, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 198–213. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruening, Benjamin. 2009. Selectional asymmetries between CP and DP suggest that the DP hypothesis is wrong. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 15(1): 27–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carstairs, Andrew. 1987. Diachronic evidence and the affix-clitic distinction. In International conference on historical linguistics (ICHL) 7, eds. Anna Giacalone Ramat, Onofrio Carruba, and Giuliano Bernini, 151–162. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chisarik, Erika, and John Payne. 2001. Modeling possessor constructions in LFG: English and Hungarian. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 01, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Citko, Barbara, and Martina Gračanin-Yuksek. 2013. Towards a new typology of coordinated wh-questions. Journal of Linguistics 49(1): 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalrymple, Mary. 2001. Lexical functional grammar. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dalrymple, Mary, and Louise Mycock. 2011. The prosody-semantics interface. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 11, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 173–193. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • den Dikken, Marcel. 1998. (Anti-)agreement in DP. In Linguistics in the Netherlands 1998, eds. Renée van Bezooijen and René Kager, 95–107. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denison, David, Alan K. Scott, and Kersti Börjars. 2010. The real distribution of the English “group genitive”. Studies in Language 34(3): 532–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, Yehuda N. 2001. Lexical-functional grammar: an introduction to parallel constraint-based syntax. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haspelmath, Martin. 1999. Explaining article-possessor complementarity: economic motivation in noun phrase syntax. Language 75(2): 227–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hristov, Božhil P. 2013. Pronominal case assignment in English. Journal of Linguistics 49(3): 567–611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, Richard A. 1987. Zwicky on heads. Journal of Linguistics 23: 109–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, Richard A. 1990. English word grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, Richard A. 2007. Language networks: the new word grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, Richard A. 2010. An introduction to word grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, Richard A. 2013. A cognitive analysis of John’s hat. In Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession, eds. Kersti Börjars, David Denison, and Alan Scott, 149–176. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jahr-Sørheim, Mette-Catherine. 1980. The s-genitive in present-day English. PhD diss, University of Oslo.

  • Jespersen, Otto. 1914. A Modern English grammar on historical principles. Part II: syntax. Heidelberg: Winter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juvonen, Teo. 2013. Variation in the form and function of the possessive morpheme in late middle and early modern English. In Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession, eds. Kersti Börjars, David Denison, and Alan Scott, 35–58. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, Ronald M. 1987. Three seductions of computational psycholinguistics. In Linguistic theory and computer applications, eds. Peter Whitelock, Mary McGee Wood, Harold L. Somers, Rod Johnson, and Paul Bennett, 149–181. London: Academic Press. Also in Formal issues in lexical-functional grammar, ed. Mary Dalrymple, Ronald M. Kaplan, John T. Maxwell III, and Annie Zaenen, CSLI Publications, 1995, pp. 339–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, Ronald M. 1989. The formal architecture of lexical-functional grammar. In Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing (ROCLING) II, eds. Chu-Ren Huang and Keh-Jiann Chen, 3–18. Tapei: The Association for Computational Linguistics and Chinese Language Processing (ACLCLP). Also published in Journal of Information Science and Engineering 5 (1989), pp. 305–322, and in Formal issues in lexical-functional grammar, ed. Mary Dalrymple, Ronald M. Kaplan, John T. Maxwell III and Annie Zaenen, CSLI Publications, 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, Ronald M., and Joan Bresnan. 1982. Lexical-functional grammar: a formal system for grammatical representation. In The mental representation of grammatical relations, ed. Joan Bresnan, 173–281. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard S. 1993. Toward a modular theory of auxiliary selection. Studia Linguistica 47(1): 3–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard S. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroeger. 1993. Phrase structure and grammatical relations in Tagalog. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruisinga, E. 1932. A handbook of present-day English, Part II: English accidence and syntax. Groningen: Noordhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laczkó, Tibor. 1995. The syntax of Hungarian noun phrases – a lexical-functional approach. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laczkó, Tibor. 2000. Derived nominals, possessors, and lexical mapping theory in Hungarian DPs. In Argument realization, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 189–227. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lapointe, Steven G. 1990. Edge features in GPSG. In Chicago Linguistic Society (CLS) 26, vol. 1, eds. Michael Ziolkowski, Manuela Noske, and Kren Deaton, 221–235. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luís, Ana, and Ryo Otoguro. 2004. Proclitic contexts in European Portuguese and their effect on clitic placement. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 04, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 334–352. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luís, Ana, and Ryo Otoguro. 2005. Morphological and syntactic well-formedness: the case of European Portuguese clitics. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 05, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 253–270. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luís, Ana, and Louisa Sadler. 2003. Object clitic and marked morphology. In Empirical issues in formal syntax and semantics 4, eds. Claire Beyssade, Oliver Bonami, Patricia Cabredo Hofherr, and Francis Corblin, 133–153. Paris: Presses de l’Université de Paris-Sorbonne.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, William D., and Lorraine K. Tyler. 2007. Morphology, language and the brain: the decompositional substrate for language comprehension. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences 362: 823–836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyers, Adam. 1995. The NP analysis of NP. In Chicago Linguistics Society (CLS) 31, 329–342. Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, Philip H. 1991. Clitics and constituents in phrase-structure grammar. PhD diss, Rijksuniversiteit te Utrecht.

  • Miller, Philip H., and Aaron L. Halpern. 1993. English possessives and the syntax of morphological features. In Formal Linguistics Society of Mid-America (FLSM) III, ed. Laurel Smith Stvan, 219–234. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Linguistics Club.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mycock, Louise. 2010. Prominence in Hungarian: the prosody-syntax connection. Transactions of the Philological Society 108(3): 265–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mycock, Louise, and John J. Lowe. 2013. The prosodic marking of discourse functions. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 13, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 440–460. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norde, Muriel. 1997. The history of the genitive in Swedish: a case study in degrammaticalization. PhD diss, University of Amsterdam.

  • Norde, Muriel. 2001. Deflexion as a counterdirectional factor in grammatical change. Language Sciences 23(2–3): 231–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norde, Muriel. 2013. Tracing the origins of the Swedish group genitive. In The genitive, eds. Anne Carlier and Jean-Christophe Verstraete, 299–332. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nordlinger, Rachel, and Joan Bresnan. 2011. Lexical-functional grammar: interactions between morphology and syntax. In Non-transformational syntax: formal and explicit models of grammar, eds. Robert D. Borsley and Kersti Börjars, 112–140. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John. 1993. The headedness of noun phrases: slaying the nominal hydra. In Heads in grammatical theory, eds. Greville G. Corbett, Norman M. Fraser, and Scott McGlashan, 114–139. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John. 2009. The English genitive and double case. Transactions of the Philological Society 107(3): 322–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John. 2011. Genitive coordinations with personal pronouns. English Language and Linguistics 15(2): 363–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John. 2013. The oblique genitive in English. In Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession, eds. Kersti Börjars, David Denison, and Alan Scott, 177–192. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, John, and Rodney Huddleston. 2002. Nouns and noun phrases. In The Cambridge grammar of the English language, eds. Rodney Huddleston and Geoffrey K. Pullum, 323–523. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, Stanley, and Dag Westerståhl. 2013. The semantics of possessives. Language 89(4): 713–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plank, Frans. 1985. The interpretation and development of form alternations conditioned across word boundaries. The case of wife’s, wives, and wives’. In International Conference on English Historical Linguistics (ICEHL) 4, Amsterdam, 10–13 April, 1985, eds. Roger Eaton, Olga Fischer, Willem Koopman, and Frederike van der Leek, 205–233. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plank, Frans. 1992. Possessives and the distinction between determiners and modifiers (with special reference to German). Journal of Linguistics 28: 453–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plank, Frans. 1995. Entgrammatisierung — Spiegelbild der Grammatisierung? In Beiträge zum internationalen Symposium über “Natürlichkeitstheorie und Sprachwandel” an der Universität Maribor vom 13.5.–15.5.1993, eds. Norbert Boretzky, Wolfgang Dressler, Janez Orešnik, Karmen Teržan, and Wolfgang Wurzel, 199–219. Bochum: Brockmeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Post, Brechtje, William D. Marslen-Wilson, Billi Randall, and Lorraine K. Tyler. 2008. The processing of English regular inflections: phonological cues to morphological structure. Cognition 109: 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul. 1966. On so-called pronouns in English. In Modern studies in English, eds. David Reibel and Sanford Schane, 201–223. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quirk, Randolf, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech, and Jan Svartvik. 1985. A comprehensive grammar of the English language. Harlow: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramchand, Gillian Catriona. 2008. Verb meaning and the lexicon: a first-phase syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, Alan K., David Denison, and Kersti Börjars. 2007. Is the English possessive ’s truly a right edge phenomenon. Paper presented at the International Conference on the Linguistics of Contemporary English (ICLCE) 2, Toulouse, 2 July 2007.

  • Spencer, Andrew. 1992. Nominal inflection and the nature of functional categories. Journal of Linguistics 28: 313–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, Andrew. 2005. Case in Hindi. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 05, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 429–446. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Svenonius, Peter. 2011. Spanning. MS, University of Tromsø.

  • Toivonen, Ida. 2003. Non-projecting words: a case study of Swedish verbal particles. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wescoat, Michael Thomas. 2002. On lexical sharing. Ph.D. thesis, Stanford University.

  • Wescoat, Michael Thomas. 2005. English nonsyllabic auxiliary contractions: an analysis in LFG with lexical sharing. In Lexical functional grammar (LFG) 05, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 468–486. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wescoat, Michael Thomas. 2007. Preposition-determiner contractions: an analysis in optimality-theoretic lexical-functional grammar with lexical sharing. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 07, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 439–459. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wescoat, Michael Thomas. 2009. Udi person markers and lexical integrity. In Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) 09, eds. Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King, 604–622. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, Edwin. 2003. Representation theory. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zlatic, Larisa. 1997. The structure of the Serbian noun phrase. PhD diss, The University of Texas at Austin.

  • Zribi-Hertz, Anne. 1997. On the dual nature of the ‘possessive’ marker in modern English. Journal of Linguistics 33(2): 511–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwicky, Arnold M. 1977. On clitics. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwicky, Arnold M. 1987. Suppressing the zs. Journal of Linguistics 23: 133–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwicky, Arnold M. 1988. Direct reference to heads. Folia Linguistica 22(3–4): 397–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwicky, Arnold M., and Geoffrey K. Pullum. 1983. Cliticization vs. inflection: English n’t. Language 59(3): 502–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am very grateful to Mary Dalrymple, Louise Mycock, Liselotte Snijders, two anonymous reviewers and the editor-in-chief Marcel den Dikken for comments on earlier versions. I would like to thank the audiences at LSA 2013, SE-LFG 10, the University of Oxford General Linguistics Seminar, and the 21st International Conference on Historical Linguistics, where earlier versions of this work were presented, for their attention and helpful comments. All errors are my own. This work was undertaken while in receipt of an Early Career Research Fellowship from the Leverhulme Trust.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John J. Lowe.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lowe, J.J. English possessive ’s: clitic and affix. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 34, 157–195 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-015-9300-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-015-9300-1

Keywords