Human Agression and Violence
Human Agression and Violence
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
11
by
th
e
Am
er
ic
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
and Violence
al
As
Human Aggression
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:27 AM
Am
er
Page ii
ic
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
A)
P
(A
n
Human Aggression
io
at
ci
so
and Violence
As
al
ic
Causes, Manifestations, and Consequences
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
Copyright © 2011 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved. Except as
permitted under the United States Copyright Act of 1976, no part of this publication may
be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means, including, but not limited to, the
process of scanning and digitization, or stored in a database or retrieval system, without the
prior written permission of the publisher.
A)
Published by To order
P
American Psychological Association APA Order Department
(A
750 First Street, NE P.O. Box 92984
Washington, DC 20002 Washington, DC 20090-2984
n
www.apa.org Tel: (800) 374-2721; Direct: (202) 336-5510
io
Fax: (202) 336-5502; TDD/TTY: (202) 336-6123
at
Online: www.apa.org/pubs/books
E-mail: [email protected]
ci
so
In the U.K., Europe, Africa, and the Middle East, copies may be ordered from
American Psychological Association
As
3 Henrietta Street
Covent Garden, London
al
WC2E 8LU England
ic
og
Typeset in Goudy by Circle Graphics, Inc., Columbia, MD
opinions and statements do not necessarily represent the policies of the American
Psychological Association.
an
BF575.A3H867 2011
20
155.2'32—dc22
2010005100
©
First Edition
C
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page v
A)
Mario Mikulincer and Phillip R. Shaver, Series Editors
P
(A
Series Titles
n
io
Prosocial Motives, Emotions, and Behavior: The Better Angels of Our Nature
at
Edited by Mario Mikulincer and Phillip R. Shaver
ci
so
Human Aggression and Violence: Causes, Manifestations, and Consequences
As
Edited by Phillip R. Shaver and Mario Mikulincer
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:27 AM
Am
er
Page vi
ic
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page vii
CONTENTS
PA)
(A
n
io
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
Contributors ............................................................................................ xi
ic
er
Preface .................................................................................................... xv
Am
Introduction ............................................................................................. 3
Phillip R. Shaver and Mario Mikulincer
e
th
Aaron Sell
C
vii
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page viii
A)
Chapter 5. Dispositional Influences on Human Aggression ........ 89
Jennifer L. Tackett and Robert F. Krueger
P
(A
Chapter 6. A Social Neuroscience Perspective on the
n
Neurobiological Bases of Aggression ........................ 105
io
Thomas F. Denson
at
ci
so
II. Genetic and Environmental Determinants ................................. 121
As
Chapter 7. The Transmission of Aggressiveness Across
Generations: Biological, Contextual,
al
and Social Learning Processes .................................. 123
ic
L. Rowell Huesmann, Eric F. Dubow, and Paul Boxer
og
Chapter 8. Genetic and Environmental Influences ol
ch
on Aggression ........................................................... 143
y
Kenneth A. Dodge
Chapter 12. Evolved Mechanisms for Revenge and Forgiveness .... 221
op
viii CONTENTS
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page ix
PA)
Chapter 15. Respecting Others and Being Respected
(A
Can Reduce Aggression in Parent–Child
Relations and in Schools .......................................... 277
n
io
Ofra Mayseless and Miri Scharf
at
ci
IV. Aggression at the Societal Level ................................................ 295
so
Chapter 16. An Existential Perspective on Violent Solutions to
As
Ethno–Political Conflict .......................................... 297
al
Gilad Hirschberger and Tom Pyszczynski
ic
og
Chapter 17. The Emotional Roots of Intergroup Aggression:
The Distinct Roles of Anger and Hatred ................. 315
ol
Eran Halperin
y ch
CONTENTS ix
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:27 AM
Am
Page x
er
ic
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page xi
CONTRIBUTORS
PA)
(A
n
io
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
Ames
er
Champaign
ig
Lexington
xi
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page xii
Kenneth A. Dodge, PhD, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke Univer-
sity, Durham, NC
John F. Dovidio, PhD, Department of Psychology, Yale University, New
Haven, CT
A)
Eric F. Dubow, PhD, Department of Psychology, Bowling Green State Uni-
versity, Bowling Green, OH
P
(A
Donald G. Dutton, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of British
Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
n
Eli J. Finkel, PhD, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University,
io
Evanston, IL
at
Karni Ginzburg, PhD, The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv
ci
University, Tel Aviv, Israel
so
Gail S. Goodman, PhD, Department of Psychology and Center for Public
As
Policy Research, University of California, Davis
Eran Halperin, PhD, School of Government, Interdisciplinary Center
al
Herzliya, Herzliya, Israel
ic
LaTonya S. Harris, EdM, Department of Psychology and Center for Public
og
Policy Research, University of California, Davis
ol
Gilad Hirschberger, PhD, School of Psychology, Interdisciplinary Center
ch
Herzliya, Herzliya, Israel
y
nia, Philadelphia
Am
Colorado Springs
ig
Soo Hyun Rhee, PhD, Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Col-
yr
orado, Boulder
op
xii CONTRIBUTORS
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page xiii
A)
Phillip R. Shaver, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of California,
Davis
P
(A
Purnima Singh, PhD, Department of Psychology, Women’s College, Aligarh
Muslim University, Aligarh, India
n
Erica B. Slotter, MA, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University,
io
Evanston, IL
at
Zahava Solomon, PhD, The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv
ci
University, Tel Aviv, Israel
so
Benjamin A. Tabak, MSc, Department of Psychology, University of Miami,
As
Coral Gables, FL
Jennifer L. Tackett, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of
al
Toronto, Toronto, Canada
ic
Nicole Tausch, PhD, Department of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff,
og
Wales, United Kingdom
ol
Sander Thomaes, PhD, Department of Developmental Psychology, Utrecht
ch
University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
y
Rochester, NY
Irwin D. Waldman, PhD, Department of Psychology, Emory University,
an
Atlanta, GA
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
CONTRIBUTORS xiii
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:27 AM
Am
er
Page xiv
ic
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page xv
PREFACE
PA)
(A
n
io
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
We live in a violent world. Aggression, intimidation, and cruelty are
ch
part of daily life almost everywhere. Homicides, violent robberies, gang
y
warfare, political violence, terrorism, and international conflicts fill the daily
Ps
military recruits.
ic
The media? Alcohol and drugs that impair self-control and disrupt rational
problem solving? Access to guns?
e
Within the social and behavioral sciences, and now within the brain sci-
th
ences as well, investigators agree that human aggression is complex and multi-
by
faceted and that many kinds of research are necessary to understand its causes,
manifestations, and consequences. Some writers, for example, have considered
11
aggression and violence from evolutionary and genetic perspectives and have
20
lence. The timeliness and social relevance of the topic led us to dedicate the
xv
12051-00_FM-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:27 AM Page xvi
A)
are invited to visit the Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya to engage in con-
structively critical but collaborative conversations and then to contribute to
P
(A
a focused, gracefully written volume that can be shared with other members
of the discipline and understood by a wide range of educated readers outside
n
the discipline. The first volume in the series, Prosocial Motives, Emotions, and
io
Behavior (published by the American Psychological Association [APA] in
at
2009), was dedicated to prosocial behavior. The present volume is the second
ci
in the series.
so
The chapter authors generously agreed to deliver lectures, participate in
As
hours of discussion of the lectures, and then return home and prepare chap-
ters based on these lectures and discussions. The meeting was cohosted by the
al
two of us, and we worked together with the chapter authors to make the book
ic
as accessible, coherent, and readable as possible, so that it would be suitable
og
for researchers and application-oriented professionals as well as university
ol
classes and the educated public. The book provides a state-of-the-art review
ch
of the major theoretical perspectives on human aggression and violence; the
y
group of scholars and admirable human beings who care about both their sci-
Am
ences and the betterment of human lives. We especially wish to thank Pro-
fessor Uriel Reichmann, president of the Interdisciplinary Center Herzliya,
e
who provided financial and staff support for an annual series of conferences
th
the many on-site details, and coped masterfully with the inevitable glitches
20
and emergencies. We would also like to thank Maureen Adams, senior acqui-
sitions editor at APA Books, for seeing the value of this book and the series
©
friend during this book’s preparation. Finally, we thank Tyler Aune and Katie
ig
xvi PREFACE
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
11
by
th
e
Am
er
ic
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
and Violence
al
As
Human Aggression
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
11
12051-01_Introduction-rev2.qxd
by
th
e
5/25/10
Am
er
11:28 AM
ic
an
Page 2
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-01_Introduction-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:28 AM Page 3
INTRODUCTION
PHILLIP R. SHAVER AND MARIO MIKULINCER
PA)
(A
n
io
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
urgent need to understand and control these destructive forces. Within the
er
social and behavioral sciences, and recently within genetics and neuroscience
Am
as well, there are deep and empirically supported insights into aggression and
violence, but they are rarely included in a single conversation. We know more
e
than ever about the evolutionary history and functions of aggression; several
th
and cultural causes. But the resulting knowledge has not been integrated and
effectively applied in real life. It is time for an open, collaborative, and creative
11
ing another person who wants to avoid the harm (i.e., a victim), and violence is
yr
examples and discussions of violence at the dyadic, familial, and societal levels,
C
see Chapters 14, 16, 17, and 19–21). In psychological science, aggression and
3
12051-01_Introduction-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:28 AM Page 4
violence are viewed as different from assertiveness and power. Although aggres-
sion can be used to assert one’s authority and control over precious resources,
there are many cases in which power can be attained without inflicting any
harm to another person (for discussions of the relations among aggression, vio-
A)
lence, and power, see Chapters 3, 4, and 13). Researchers also make distinc-
tions between subtypes of aggression (e.g., proactive vs. reactive aggression,
P
(A
thoughtful vs. thoughtless aggression), but they realize that aggressive acts in
the real world are often complex combinations of these subtypes that can
n
change over time and as a function of the victim’s responses (for discussions of
io
dyadic-level effects on aggressive behavior, see Chapters 1, 2, and 20).
at
Historically, aggressive behavior was viewed either as the result of an
ci
inborn instinct aimed at the destruction of life (e.g., Freud’s, 1920/1961, the-
so
ory of the death drive) or as a learned response to the frustration of one’s needs
As
(e.g., Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer, & Sears’s, 1939, well-known frustration–
aggression hypothesis). With the advance of psychological research, several
al
theories of aggression and its causes have emerged. For example, neoassociation-
ic
ist theory (Berkowitz, 1993) posits that aversive events (e.g., frustration, provo-
og
cation) can automatically trigger aggressive behavior, whereas a cognitive
ol
revision of this theory emphasizes that higher level cognitive processes (e.g.,
ch
attributions, appraisals) moderate these automatic aggressive responses. Social
y
world around them, including the mass media. Social interaction theory (Tedeschi
Am
social identity.
th
theory of aggression, the general aggression model (GAM; for a detailed discus-
sion of this theory, see Chapter 1), encompassing the common features of
11
analyzed at two different levels of causation, distal and proximal, and within
each level there are personal and situational risk factors as well as affective,
©
motivational, and cognitive mechanisms through which the risk factors influ-
ht
ence aggression. (These factors are discussed in detail throughout this vol-
ig
ume.) For example, at the distal level, the GAM emphasizes genetic risk
yr
childhood (Chapters 7 and 9), cultural norms and values (Chapter 10),
socioeconomic risk factors, and child abuse and neglect (Chapters 13, 14, and
C
19). The GAM also emphasizes personality predispositions (see Chapter 5),
A)
the GAM emphasizes the role of self-regulatory processes in triggering and
resisting aggression (see Chapters 2, 6, and 12).
P
(A
Theory and research on aggression have also informed efforts to prevent
and treat aggression. The methods include reducing exposure to events that
n
teach or reward aggressive behavior or hostile appraisals and attributions (see
io
Chapters 7 and 9), diminishing personality and situational forces that can trig-
at
ger aggression such as narcissism and alcohol abuse (see Chapters 2 and 11),
ci
strengthening prosocial virtues and action tendencies (Chapters 12 and 15),
so
and increasing the quality of parental care and the resulting sense of emotional
As
security (Chapters 13 and 14).
The need for understanding and reducing aggression, violence, and their
al
destructive consequences inspired us to dedicate the second volume of The
ic
Herzliya Series on Personality and Social Psychology to the psychology of aggres-
og
sive behavior, viewed from various levels of analysis, from the neural to the
ol
societal. We invited experts on the science of aggression and violence to
ch
explain their ideas and research findings, and encouraged them to trace the
y
where it might lead in the future. Readers of this volume will gain a deeper and
broader understanding of aggression, violence, and their social and psycholog-
an
ical causes and effects, whether the readers are anchored in the disciplines of
ic
personality, social, and clinical psychology; the close relationships field; devel-
er
learn about methods used to study aggression and violence, and about prom-
ising psychological and social interventions that can reduce violence and deal
e
The volume is organized into five sections. The first, focusing on major
by
aggression and violence. The second section, focusing on research into the
20
siveness, and the genetic, social information processing, and cultural factors
ht
that mediate the development of aggression and its transmission across gener-
ig
ations. The third section includes five chapters dealing with psychological and
yr
tion includes three chapters on the existential, emotional, and structural roots
of intergroup aggression. The final section considers the consequences of aggres-
C
sion viewed from the perspective of its victims: the abused child, the battered
INTRODUCTION 5
12051-01_Introduction-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:28 AM Page 6
romantic partner, and citizens and soldiers exposed to political violence, acts
of terrorism, and wars.
In Chapter 1, Nathan DeWall and Craig Anderson present a current
account of the influential GAM. They conceptualize aggression in terms of
A)
cycles—the single-episode cycle, the developmental cycle, the violence esca-
lation cycle—and specify causes and processes, both biological and environ-
P
(A
mental, that contribute to aggressive behavior. They also discuss ways to
reduce violence at the individual, community, national, and international lev-
n
els. This overview of the classic research on aggression provides a foundation
io
for the following chapters.
at
In Chapter 2, Erica Slotter and Eli Finkel expand on the GAM by pro-
ci
viding a new theoretical framework, the I3 (I-Cubed) theory, which organizes
so
aggression risk factors into three categories: (a) instigating triggers—discrete
As
events that arouse action tendencies conducive to aggression; (b) impelling
forces—factors that increase the likelihood of an aggressive impulse follow-
al
ing an instigating trigger; and (c) inhibiting forces—factors that increase the
ic
likelihood that aggression will be contained or held in check rather than
og
being expressed.
ol
In Chapter 3, Aaron Sell explains how anger and aggression may have
ch
evolved biologically to solve certain adaptive problems. Applying the meth-
y
anger to treat the angry person better. Using this framework, Sell reviews
er
recent studies of anger and aggression and shows that their findings can be
Am
parses human motivation into several life domains and considers the biological
function of the behavioral systems that evolved to solve adaptive problems
11
within each of these domains. Shaver et al. propose that a “power” behavioral
20
Scale that measures hyperactivation and deactivation of the power system, and
ht
present new research findings concerning how these two dimensions provide a
ig
A)
factors are best understood in relation to particular environmental contexts.
In Chapter 6, Thomas Denson moves to the neural level of analysis and
P
(A
considers how some of the processes discussed by other chapter authors are
manifested in the brain. He shows that the limbic system and the dorsal ante-
n
rior cingulate are involved in the arousal of anger, and that regions associated
io
with emotion regulation are involved in rumination and displaced aggression
at
(i.e., harming people who are not responsible for the aggressor’s problems).
ci
In addition, neural regions involved in reward processing are active during
so
acts of aggression, helping to explain the difficulty of down-regulating aggres-
As
sion. Many provocative exciting ideas for future research are proposed in this
chapter.
al
The second section of the volume begins with a chapter by Rowell Hues-
ic
mann, Eric Dubow, and Paul Boxer (Chapter 7), who consider the prevalence
og
of aggressiveness from childhood to adulthood and the extent to which this
ol
trait is transmitted across generations. They also provide a road map for under-
ch
standing the relative importance of genetic factors, continuity in environ-
y
In Chapter 8, Soo Hyun Rhee and Irwin Waldman explore genetic influ-
er
ences on aggression and compare the patterns and magnitudes of genetic and
Am
conclude that aggression is partially attributable to genes and that the heritabil-
by
(b) these patterns account for the effects of genetic and environmental factors
ig
aggressive behavior. Dodge evaluates his model with data from the ongoing lon-
op
gitudinal Child Development Project. His work has important implications for
the design of interventions to impede the development of aggressive and vio-
C
lent behavior.
INTRODUCTION 7
12051-01_Introduction-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:28 AM Page 8
A)
als position themselves in alignment with the dominant value system of their
culture. They review recent studies of people from honor, dignity, and face
P
(A
cultures, showing that both within- and between-culture forms of variation
are important in explaining a person’s propensity to behave aggressively.
n
The third section of the volume begins with Sander Thomaes and Brad
io
Bushman’s chapter (Chapter 11) on the relations among narcissism, self-
at
esteem, and aggression. In the first section of the chapter, the authors present
ci
evidence against the familiar hypothesis that low self-esteem leads to aggres-
so
sion. In the second section, they argue that aggressive individuals are typi-
As
cally self-absorbed, believing they are better than others and overestimating
their own valuable qualities; in other words, they are narcissistic. Thomaes
al
and Bushman review recent findings showing that the combination of narcis-
ic
sism and insult leads to exceptionally high levels of aggression. Their work is
og
important for designers of school programs that foster self-views that deter
aggression. ol
ch
In Chapter 12, Michael McCullough, Robert Kurzban, and Benjamin
y
humans’ capacities for revenge and forgiveness. They argue against character-
izations of revenge as something “gone wrong” in human nature, and against
an
They review evidence suggesting that both revenge and forgiveness are behav-
er
ioral adaptations that helped our human ancestors solve prevalent social prob-
Am
lems. McCullough et al. enumerate the selection pressures that probably gave
rise to both revenge and forgiveness and describe the psychological processes
e
Chapters 13 (by Mario Mikulincer & Phillip Shaver) and Chapter 14 (by
by
tional forms of aggression. We have included two chapters about this perspec-
20
tive because there are two different lines of work combining attachment
theory with (a) personality and social psychological research on social motives,
©
(b) issues in clinical and forensic psychology (covered by Dutton). In the first
ig
research on the links between these insecurities and anger, domestic violence,
antisocial behavior, and intergroup hostility. They also consider the main
C
A)
domestic violence) or oneself (e.g., self-cutting or suicide) can be caused by
attachment-related threats (e.g., threats of separation, rejection, or abandon-
P
(A
ment). Dutton also discusses symbolic attachment, the promise of reunion with
and high praise from loved ones after death, and considers its role in religion-
n
based aggression, military violence, and other forms of violence against mem-
io
bers of outgroups.
at
In Chapter 15, Ofra Mayseless and Miri Scharf discuss the importance
ci
of respect in social relationships as a buffer against aggression. They consider
so
various definitions of respect and draw distinctions between respect and other
As
emotions or attitudes thought to be buffers against aggression, such as trust,
empathy, and acceptance. They review studies that illuminate the role of
al
respect in reducing aggression in unequal relationships, such as the respect
ic
shown by parents for their children and by children for their parents, and the
og
respect shown by teachers for their students and by students for their teach-
ol
ers. The chapter provides important insights into the role of respect in inter-
ch
ventions aimed at reducing aggression in homes and schools.
y
In the first chapter in the fourth section of the volume (see Chapter 16),
Ps
that basic existential fears and awareness of one’s own mortality affect the
ic
extent to which people rely on aggression and violence to solve intergroup con-
er
flicts. They review studies from their own and other laboratories showing that
Am
contextual factors that inhibit the effects of existential concerns on the escala-
th
tion of violence. The chapter ends with an integrative model showing how
by
patriotism, vengeance, and pacifism are all rooted in basic existential concerns.
In Chapter 17, Eran Halperin probes the emotional roots of intergroup
11
roles. Equipped with an integration of theories from social psychology and the
field of conflict resolution, Halperin reviews studies focused mainly on the
©
Israeli–Palestinian conflict, showing that neither anger nor hatred alone is suf-
ht
chapter is that the link between anger and escalating cycles of violence is
op
INTRODUCTION 9
12051-01_Introduction-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:28 AM Page 10
discounting the basic premise of contact theory, which proposes that inter-
group aggression can be reduced by bringing members of opposing groups
together under conditions that stress commonalities. Instead, Saguy et al.
focus on power-related dynamics and review findings from both laboratory
A)
and real-world studies showing that stressing commonalities between strong
and weak groups can create tension rather than harmony. The authors show
P
(A
that well-intentioned interventions aimed at reducing intergroup hostility
may paradoxically reduce the likelihood of reconciliation.
n
The final section of the volume deals with the consequences of aggres-
io
sion viewed from the perspectives of its victims. In Chapter 19, Sheree Toth,
at
LaTonya Harris, Gail Goodman, and Dante Cicchetti discuss the long-term
ci
consequences of aggression against children (child abuse and maltreatment).
so
The authors focus on emotion regulation, attachment insecurity, and mental
As
health, as well as effects of maltreatment on memories of abusive experiences
and events. They suggest policies and practices that can benefit children, ado-
al
lescents, and adults who were abused, neglected, or maltreated during infancy
ic
and childhood. They also suggest practices that may increase the accuracy of
og
such people’s memories and reports in forensic contexts.
ol
In Chapter 20, Ximena Arriaga and Nicole Capezza focus on intimate
ch
partner violence and consider the paradox of being a victim of a partner’s
y
aggression while still being committed to the aggressive partner and the trou-
Ps
authors then present findings from several studies showing that a victim’s well-
er
being and the maintenance of the victim’s relationship are sometimes at odds.
Am
The chapter emphasizes the need for special interventions to protect the vic-
tims of relational aggression.
e
In Chapter 21, Zahava Solomon and Karni Ginzburg discuss the long-
th
findings from 3-decade longitudinal studies of (a) veterans of the First Lebanon
20
War in 1982 and (b) ex-POWs from the Yom Kippur War in 1973. The stud-
ies reveal that for many soldiers and civilians the war does not end when the
©
shooting stops but continues to plague them in diverse and complicated ways.
ht
In some cases, the violence is brought home from the battlefield and adversely
ig
As can be seen from these brief thumbnail sketches, the chapters in this
volume cover a broad array of ideas and research on aggression and violence,
C
harmful effects. The book is realistic, recognizing that aggression arises partly
from human evolution, genes, and natural interpersonal and intergroup con-
flict and is therefore not likely to be completely eliminated. But the book is
also hopeful, showing that scientific insight and a variety of research-based
A)
interventions can reduce the development, expression, and detrimental con-
sequences of aggression and violence. The authors have provided a com-
P
(A
mendable service to readers by writing clearly and compellingly about their
areas of expertise, discussing their ideas in person with each other at a vibrant
n
and creative series of meetings in Israel, and cross-referencing their chapters
io
to help readers pursue useful connections. Together they offer a deep, mature
at
portrait of human aggression and violence and suggest concrete ways in which
ci
it can be reduced in future generations.
so
As
REFERENCES
al
ic
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psy-
og
chology, 53, 27–51. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231
ol
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
ch
Prentice-Hall.
y
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York, NY:
Ps
McGraw-Hill.
Dollard, J., Doob, L., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration
an
Freud, S. (1961). Beyond the pleasure principle (J. Strachey, Ed. & Trans.). New York,
er
implications for social policy (pp. 73–109). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
by
Tedeschi, J. T., & Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence, aggression, and coercive actions.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10160-000
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
INTRODUCTION 11
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
11
12051-01_Introduction-rev2.qxd
by
th
e
5/25/10
Am
er
11:28 AM
ic
an
Page 12
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-02_PT1-CH01-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 13
A)
P
(A
MAJOR THEORETICAL
n
io
PERSPECTIVES
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
12051-02_PT1-CH01-rev2.qxd
11
by
5/25/10
th
e
Am
11:29 AM
er
ic
an
Page 14
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-02_PT1-CH01-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 15
PA)
THE GENERAL AGGRESSION MODEL
(A
n
io
C. NATHAN DEWALL AND CRAIG A. ANDERSON
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
Marwan outlined a two-step process (Ghosh, 2005). First, he will ask Allah to
th
bless his holy mission with a high rate of American casualties. Second, he will
by
ask for a pure soul that is suitable to see Allah and his mujahideen brothers
who are already in paradise. Marwan’s final wishes are both chilling and puz-
11
zling, suggesting, as they do, that Allah will approve and assist Marwan’s mur-
20
der of many Americans. How does such a privileged youth become a suicide
bomber?
©
joined the American National Guard not to inflict pain on others but to pro-
ig
vide a means to pay for her education. She has a son toward whom she behaves
yr
with love and kindness. As a guard in the Abu Ghraib prison in Iraq, however,
op
she wreaked havoc on Iraqi prisoners through the use of cruel and humiliating
torture practices. Her acts gained worldwide attention through the publication
C
of pictures showing her apparent glee over inflicting pain and humiliation on
15
12051-02_PT1-CH01-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 16
A)
Scholars, politicians, and the general public are often perplexed not only
by aggressive acts committed by individuals but also by aggression between
P
groups large and small. The escalating conflict between Israelis and Palestini-
(A
ans, for example, has claimed the lives of thousands of people, including many
n
unarmed civilians. Both Israeli and Palestinian leaders have offered logical sug-
io
gestions for ways to end the conflict, and other governments have done the
at
same. Yet the end of the conflict is nowhere in sight. The dispute appears
ci
intractable (see Chapters 16 and 17). On December 29, 2008, Israel launched
so
a major military offensive designed to stop Hamas militants from firing mis-
As
siles into the Jewish state. Over 1,000 people died, with more than 4,500 addi-
tional people wounded. After the assault ended, the Hamas rockets continued
al
to land in southern Israel. Why does the Israeli–Palestinian conflict continue
ic
to escalate despite recurrent efforts to end it peacefully?
og
The purpose of this chapter is to demonstrate how the general aggression
ol
model (GAM; Anderson & Bushman, 2002; Anderson & Huesmann, 2003)
ch
helps to answer these perplexing questions (and many others) regarding the
y
and social learning theories that have been developed over the past 40 years
er
dura, 1977; Berkowitz, 1989; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1980; Huesmann,
1988; Mischel, 1973; Mischel & Shoda, 1995; Chapters 7 and 9, this volume).
e
These perspectives paved the way for understanding the learning and devel-
th
tion of the GAM. Third, we discuss the dynamic process by which persono-
ht
logical and situational factors establish and sustain aggression: the violence
ig
escalation cycle. Fourth, we use the GAM to understand how seemingly ordinary
yr
citizens become terrorists, suicide bombers, torturers, and other doers of aggres-
op
sion and violence. Fifth, we discuss the implications of the GAM for aggression
between groups of people. Sixth, we apply the GAM to show how certain gov-
C
ernment actions designed to promote peace can increase aggression and violent
A)
ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR, AGGRESSION, AND VIOLENCE
P
(A
Much has changed since the Mesolithic period, during which human
evolution presumably formed many of our current innate tendencies. Agri-
n
culture now dominates people’s access to food, allowing people to settle in
io
communities instead of hunting and gathering in nomadic groups. Cultural
at
progress enables people to depend on others for food, clothing, and shelter
ci
instead of having to provide for themselves. Technological advances provide
so
people with the means to travel easily and to transmit knowledge to each
As
other quickly. Despite these revolutionary changes in human lives, aggression
and violence remain important topics in modern society, just as they must
al
have been to our evolutionary ancestors.
ic
Archeological and historical evidence indicates that aggression and vio-
og
lence were prevalent among our hunter/gatherer ancestors 25,000 years ago.
ol
Aggression and violence was widespread among Greek, Egyptian, and Roman
ch
societies 2,000 to 3,000 years ago. Just as modern citizens of the world ingest
y
violent media, ancient Romans had their own form of “media violence” in
Ps
which gladiators inflicted physical injury and death on each other in the pres-
ence of thousands of viewers. Aggression and violence continue to be wide-
an
spread. In short, they remain ubiquitous parts of human life. Before we can
ic
to discuss what we mean when we say that a person or group acts antisocially,
Am
aggressively, or violently.
e
Antisocial Behavior
th
by
sion and violence, but not always. In societies with norms prohibiting physical
20
often engage in aggressive and violent actions, but they also violate standards
yr
breaking other laws. Thus, antisocial behavior can involve aggression, violence,
or any other type of response that defies cultural standards for desirable behav-
C
Aggression refers to behavior carried out with the proximal (i.e., immedi-
ate) intention to inflict harm on another person who is motivated to avoid the
A)
harm (e.g., Anderson & Bushman, 2002; Baron & Richardson, 1994). Harm-
ful behavior that is accidental or an incidental by-product of helpful actions is
P
not aggressive. From a social psychological perspective, violence usually refers
(A
to the most severe types of physical aggression, those that are likely to cause
n
serious bodily injury. Occasionally, researchers in this domain use “emotional”
io
or “psychological” violence to indicate severe forms of nonphysical aggression.
at
All acts of violence fit our definition of aggression, but not all aggressive acts
ci
are violent. By our definition, violent actions need not involve illegal behav-
so
ior. Note, however, that other behavioral sciences (e.g., criminology) define
As
violence in somewhat different ways, such as by requiring the act to be illegal
(Neuilly, 2007).
al
ic
og
GENERAL AGGRESSION MODEL
ol
Several dichotomous distinctions among various forms of aggression have
ch
been proposed. Although these distinctions (i.e., proactive vs. reactive, instru-
y
mental vs. hostile, impulsive vs. premeditated) have yielded important insights,
Ps
structure approach (Bushman & Anderson, 2001). We also argue that differ-
ent forms of aggression can be distinguished in terms of proximate and ultimate
ic
aggressive behavior according to four dimensions, each of which fits well with
Am
act) can be characterized along each of the following dimensions: degree of hos-
th
tile or agitated affect present, automaticity, degree to which the primary (i.e.,
by
ultimate) goal is to harm the victim versus benefit the perpetrator, and degree
to which consequences are considered. Because many aggressive acts involve
11
sion along these four dimensions rather than relying on dichotomous category
systems provides researchers with a better means of understanding aggression
©
Basic Model
op
The GAM takes into account how aggression depends on cognitive fac-
C
and interprets his or her environment and the people therein, expectations
regarding the likelihood of various outcomes, knowledge and beliefs about
how people typically respond in certain situations, and how much people
believe they have the abilities to respond to a variety of events (see Chap-
A)
ters 7 and 9). By understanding these cognitions, researchers have a basis for
understanding both within-person and situation-specific stability in aggres-
P
(A
sion because people show similarity in how they respond to similar events
over time, and because situations frequently impose realistic demands that
n
limit the number of options regarding how people can construe the situation.
io
Furthermore, such social–cognitive models also account for variability in
at
aggression across time, people, and contexts, as different knowledge struc-
ci
tures develop and change, and as different situational contexts prime differ-
so
ent knowledge structures.
As
The GAM also focuses heavily on how the development and use of
knowledge structures influence both early (e.g., basic visual perception) and
al
downstream (e.g., judgments, decisions behaviors) psychological processes
ic
(e.g., Bargh, 1996; Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Wegner & Bargh, 1998). People
og
develop knowledge structures from their experience. Within the context of
ol
aggression, knowledge structures can influence toward whom a person directs
ch
visual attention as a function of possible threat, affective responses to provo-
y
ical threat have been shown to affect decisions to shoot unarmed African
20
(Plant & Peruche, 2005). The effect that knowledge structures can have on
ht
American man who was shot 19 times by New York City police officers as he
yr
reached for his wallet (Cooper, 1999). Thus, knowledge structures set the stage
op
for understanding how people identify objects, people, and complex social
events as relevant or irrelevant to aggression; how beliefs about specific peo-
C
ple (e.g., Osama bin Laden) or groups (e.g., Nazis, Hutus) shape perceptions
of relevance to aggression; and how people use behavioral scripts to guide their
behavior under various circumstances (e.g., responding with retaliation to an
insult when that insult increases hostile affect).
A)
Single Episode Cycle
P
(A
At the most basic level, the GAM focuses primarily on how aggression
unfolds within one cycle of an ongoing social interaction. At this level the
n
model emphasizes three main issues: person and situation inputs, present
io
internal state (i.e., cognition, arousal, affect), and outcomes of appraisal and
at
decision-making processes (see Anderson & Bushman, 2002, Figure 2).
ci
so
Person and Situation Inputs
As
The GAM considers both situation and person factors—relatively
al
enduring traits, motivations, attitudes, beliefs, and other chronic knowledge
ic
structures and less enduring cognitive, affective, and arousal states that arise
og
in particular contexts (see Chapters 2, 7, and 9). Person and situation inputs
ol
are proximate causes in that they provide the most direct guiding force
ch
behind aggression behavior, although the behavior may also serve an ulti-
mate goal (see Tooby & Cosmides, 1992). Social psychologists have identi-
y
Ps
loud noises, violent media, and physical pain (for a review, see Anderson &
ic
are hostile attribution bias, narcissism, being male, and a host of beliefs, atti-
Am
tudes, values, and behavioral scripts (see Chapters 2, 5, 9, 10, 11, 13, and 14,
this volume).
Situation and person factors are not mutually exclusive. Some situa-
e
th
tional factors give rise to states that closely resemble person variables; for
example, social rejection or playing violent video games can strengthen hos-
by
tile cognitive biases (Bushman & Anderson, 2002; DeWall, Twenge, Gitter,
11
Internal States
Person and situation variables influence aggression through the internal
states they create. That is, internal states serve as mechanisms underlying the
relationship between person and situation variables and outcomes of appraisal
A)
and decision-making processes. Affect, arousal, and cognition represent the
P
three most significant internal states. A specific person variable (e.g., high
(A
trait hostility) or situational variable (e.g., viewing violent media) may influ-
ence one, two, or all three types of internal states. Violent media, for exam-
n
io
ple, affect all three states. Moreover, the three internal states can influence
at
each other.
ci
so
Outcomes
As
A large body of literature within social psychology suggests that com-
plex information processes can involve reliance on the automatic system or
al
the controlled system (Smith & Lazarus, 1993). In the GAM (see Anderson
ic
& Bushman, 2002, Figure 3), the third stage includes complex appraisal and
og
decision processes that range from automatic to heavily controlled (Strack &
ol
Deutsch, 2004; Chapter 6, this volume). Therefore, inputs (Stage 1) affect
ch
internal states (Stage 2), which in turn influence appraisal and decision
y
processes (Stage 3). The appraisal and decision processes include automatic
Ps
sive ways. These actions enter a feedback loop that becomes part of the input
er
threat, for example, occurs effortlessly and without conscious awareness (e.g.,
th
Öhman, Lundqvist, & Esteves, 2001). This immediate appraisal process may
by
include fear and anger-related affect, goals related to aggression, and the for-
mation of intentions to carry out aggression-related acts. Person and situation
11
inputs guide immediate appraisals in ways that are congruent with a person’s
20
resources, some aggressive acts occur so fast that it may seem that appraisal
ht
has not even occurred, and indeed some behavioral scripts may be so closely
ig
suggests, for example, that the ability to override unwanted impulses depends
on a limited energy resource that becomes depleted after prior exertion (Gail-
liot et al., 2007; Chapter 2, this volume). If a person has recently engaged in
an act involving the expenditure of self-regulatory energy, that person will be
A)
less likely to engage in reappraisal (DeWall, Baumeister, Stillman, & Gail-
liot, 2007; Finkel, DeWall, Slotter, Oaten, & Foshee, 2009; Chapter 6, this
P
(A
volume). Likewise, if a person’s immediate appraisal indicates that the prob-
able outcome is either satisfying or unimportant, then the person will be less
n
likely to engage in reappraisal. Other resource limitations, such as time and
io
cognitive capacity, may also preclude reappraisal.
at
Thus, aggression results from the proximate convergence of situations
ci
and personological inputs. Situations can impel or inhibit aggression, whereas
so
personological factors enhance or diminish a person’s propensity to behave
As
aggressively. These situational and personological inputs activate affective,
arousal, and cognitive internal states, which in turn influence aggression by
al
means of appraisal and decision processes. Once the individual has performed
ic
the impulsive or thoughtful action, the behavior feeds back to the situation
og
and personological inputs to guide the next episodic cycle.
ol
ch
Aggression Before and After the Single Episode Cycle
y
Is the GAM stuck in the present? At first glance, the GAM appears to
Ps
focus most of its attention on how current internal states determine aggres-
an
sion, neglecting the importance of the past and future. However, the persono-
logical input factors bring the past to the present in the form of knowledge
ic
structures and well-rehearsed cognitive and affective processes that have been
er
influenced by biological factors (e.g., genes, hormones) and past history (see
Am
by the future through the knowledge structures used to perceive, react, and
th
learn. The present influences the future in at least two different ways: by
by
tudes, beliefs, expectations other people have of the person). Chronic exposure
20
to violent media, for example, can increase aggressive attitudes, beliefs, expec-
tations of others as hostile, and desensitization to future violence (Anderson,
©
Carnagey, & Eubanks, 2003; Bartholow, Bushman, & Sestir, 2006; Carnagey,
ht
Anderson, & Bushman, 2007). The present is influenced by the future through
ig
the person’s beliefs and expectations about how others will act, their goals,
yr
and other plans. Thus, the GAM focuses on internal states as they relate to
op
what people bring with them to the present episode from the past and also
shows how the present episode can influence future personological and situ-
C
ational factors that will influence future internal states and subsequent appraisal
and decision processes that guide aggression. It even provides a simple process
by which personality influences situations.
A)
VIOLENCE ESCALATION CYCLE
P
(A
Most incidents involving aggression and violence occur after a series of
conflict-based interactions in which the two parties trade retaliatory behav-
n
iors back and forth in an escalating cycle. Such escalating cycles include what
io
some refer to as “ordinary” violent crimes between individuals (e.g., assault
at
and murder) and between larger groups and nations. Figure 1.1 illustrates the
ci
violence escalation cycle.
so
The violence escalation cycle begins with an initial triggering event
As
that may be serious or relatively benign. The triggering event can influence
any kind of dyad, including two people, two groups, two religions, or two
al
nations. Whereas person or group A considers retaliation to the event to be
ic
justified and relatively mild, person or group B considers the retaliation to be
og
unjustified and severe, leading to retaliation toward person or group A. The
ol
cycle persists through several iterations of violent actions in which one unit
ch
perceives its retaliation to be appropriate and justified, whereas the second
y
ered to be gang B’s turf. Gang B perceives this lack of respect for gang bound-
er
eral members of gang A who try to stop the destruction of the cars. Gang B’s
th
retaliation therefore becomes gang A’s provocation, leading them to shoot and
by
kill several members of gang B. The escalation cycle continues over the course
of several weeks or months, with dozens of members of both gangs experienc-
11
ing serious physical injury or death. Real-world examples of the violence esca-
20
neutral third parties can make accurate causal inferences regarding violence
op
between two parties, the parties themselves usually cannot. In a version of the
fundamental attribution error, people tend to explain the causes of others’
C
Inappropriate Appropriate
over-retaliation retaliation
B harms A
A)
P
Appropriate Inappropriate
(A
retaliation over-retaliation
n
io
A harms B
at
ci
Inappropriate Appropriate
so
over-retaliation retaliation
As
B harms A
al
ic
Unintentional Intentional
og
Justified Unjustified
Relatively mild
ol Relatively harmful
y ch
Ps
A harms B
an
ic
er
Figure 1.1. Violence escalation cycle. Adapted from “Creating Your Own Hostile
e
forces (Anderson, Krull, & Weiner, 1996); people become caught in a web
in which members perceive the other party as acting out of malice or evil and
©
Outgroup homogeneity effects may also prevent members of both parties from
ig
making accurate attributions. U.S. citizens, for example, may perceive all
yr
members of the Islamic faith as jihadists, when in reality this term represents
op
ing one person turns into shooting many people, and so on. Such escalation
is often an attempt to signal to the other side that it should back down (see
Chapters 3 and 12, this volume). Third, increased levels of retaliatory vio-
lence stem from perspective biases in which the most recent perpetrator views
the harmful behavior as appropriate and justified, whereas the most recent
A)
victim perceives the perpetrator’s act as an inappropriate overreaction.
The violence escalation cycle corresponds to the GAM’s assertion that
P
(A
personological and situational factors can influence each other in a dynamic
manner. Social psychology frequently demonstrates that powerful situational
n
factors can override personality traits. But personality traits (including beliefs,
io
attitudes, and insecurities) sometimes dramatically influence the situation
at
(see Chapters 4 and 5). People who characteristically perceive the world as
ci
hostile and who resolve conflict through the use of aggression can turn a sit-
so
uation that involves potentially mild conflict into a severely hostile one that
As
gives rises to escalating violence (Anderson, Buckley, & Carnagey, 2008;
Chapters 11, 13, and 14, this volume).
al
ic
og
USING THE GENERAL AGGRESSION MODEL TO UNDERSTAND
HOW PERPETRATORS OF VIOLENCE ARE CREATED ol
y ch
is the prototype of the former, whereas various genocidal events (e.g., the
er
not fit this dichotomy so well. For example, some “terrorists,” such as Okla-
homa City bomber Timothy McVeigh, do not seem to fit either. Larger and
e
more persistent terrorist groups seem more institutional (e.g., Irish Republican
th
against others, it may be best to avoid the political labels whenever possible,
20
GAM can be used to organize them all. One point to keep in mind, however,
ht
ley Milgram’s (1963) obedience to authority studies and Haney, Banks, and
A)
suicide bombers in the Middle East; and recent instances of waterboarding
and other brutal tactics used by members of the U.S. military continue to raise
P
(A
the question: How are terrorists, suicide bombers, and torturers created?
Subsequent chapters in this volume provide many more specific details
n
about the precursors of specific categories of violence. Many can be summarized
io
in a few statements. Many precursors can be seen as factors that promote the
at
development of individuals who are capable of and predisposed to use aggres-
ci
sion and violence to solve conflicts (e.g., Chapters 4, 5, and 7). Other precur-
so
sors are immediate situational factors (in the case of individual violence) or the
As
current social milieu, serving in some cases as triggers (i.e., precipitating causes)
or as factors that support ongoing violence (e.g., Chapters 9 and 16).
al
Beliefs, attitudes, and expectations supportive of violence must come from
ic
somewhere. And they do. If you want to create people who are predisposed to
og
aggression and violence, begin by depriving them of resources necessary to meet
ol
basic needs—physical, emotional, psychological, and social. Provide them with
ch
multiple examples of aggression and violence, examples in which such behav-
y
ior appears to work. Desensitize them to the images, sounds, smells, and, in gen-
Ps
eral, to the horrors of violence by exposing them to these stimuli, both live and
in electronic media form. Then provide them with a belief system that serves
an
to dehumanize potential targets, that justifies on moral grounds any and all
ic
to oneself and one’s social group while maximizing positive consequences in the
Am
near future and/or in an afterlife. Finally, if you want specific forms of violence
to emerge, provide training (i.e., the behavioral scripts) in those specific forms
e
of violence. Link these violence scripts to the social support system and the
th
belief systems that you have already provided, and you will have a group of peo-
by
ple who are quite prepared to behave violently. Put these people into the right
situation, and the desired violence will occur (Miller, 2004).
11
20
The GAM makes specific predictions about aggression not only between
yr
two people but also between groups of people large (e.g., nations) and small
op
(e.g., two or more people with a defined identity and common goal). The
majority of evidence supporting GAM, however, is derived from correlational,
C
A)
In a recent review, Meier, Hinsz, and Heimerdinger (2007) argued that
groups commit and receive more aggression than individuals. Although the
P
(A
aggression literature is dominated by research on aggression between individ-
uals, the available evidence on aggression between small groups supports this
n
view (Jaffe, Shapir, & Yinon, 1981; Meier & Hinsz, 2004; Wildschut, Pinter,
io
Vevea, Insko, & Schopler, 2003). The findings suggest that group size func-
at
tions as a situational factor that produces increased levels of aggressive behav-
ci
ior from both the initial perpetrator and the initial target.
so
According to the GAM, heightened aggression between groups (rela-
As
tive to individuals) results from increased levels of aggressive affect, arousal,
or cognition. Indeed, expecting to interact with an unfriendly group increases
al
hostile expectations (Hoyle, Pinkley, & Insko, 1989), and the presence of
ic
others increases arousal (Zajonc, 1965). Finally, terror management theory
og
suggests additional ways in which groups will become more embedded in esca-
ol
lating cycles of violence (Chapter 16, this volume).
y ch
Ps
aggression and violence from perpetrators within and outside the society.
Despite the good intentions behind many government actions, governmental
e
efforts to reduce aggression and violence often fail. Even worse, some govern-
th
people will respond to the program by behaving more aggressively and violently.
20
including not only traditional natural resources (e.g., land, water) but psycho-
yr
logical ones as well (e.g., access to religious sites, traditional homeland bound-
op
aries; Avalos, 2005). One nation responds in a manner that its citizens believe
is justified, whereas the other nation perceives the action to be unjustified and
C
A)
ations and which change the nature of situations they will encounter in the
future. As a result, it is difficult for citizens to understand how their nation’s
P
(A
actions can be considered “evil” by members of the enemy nation and how
the enemy nation can feel justified in its retaliatory actions.
n
Consider how two ongoing conflicts—the U.S. War on Terrorism in
io
Afghanistan and Iraq and the Israeli–Palestinian conflict—fit the GAM’s
at
explanation of how governmental action can influence aggression and vio-
ci
lence. The U.S. War on Terrorism in Afghanistan and the war in Iraq are, in
so
large part, governmental responses to the terrorist attacks on the United
As
States on September 11, 2001, by members of al-Qaeda. Many U.S. citizens
perceive the actions taken by the United States as justified responses to the
al
terrorist attacks, but these same U.S. citizens appear baffled that Arab groups
ic
and other members of the Gulf region perceive U.S. governmental actions as
og
evil or at least unjustified overreactions. There is also evidence that the 9/11
ol
attacks changed aggression-related knowledge structures in a manner consis-
ch
tent with the GAM. After the 9/11 attacks, college students showed increased
y
positive attitudes toward war and more aggressive personalities (Carnagey &
Ps
Anderson, 2007). The increased aggressive attitudes about the war and the
increase in trait physical aggression persisted even a year after the terrorist
an
attacks. These findings provide at least indirect evidence that the U.S. gov-
ic
ernment’s actions in Afghanistan and Iraq affected attitudes toward war and
er
aggressive personality traits. The broader implication is that the U.S. govern-
Am
ment’s actions created not only hostile attitudes toward Arabs and Muslims
among U.S. citizens but also had the unplanned effect of pushing many people
e
who suffered from the U.S. military actions to join terrorist factions to retal-
th
iate against what they perceived as unwarranted and harsh attacks on their
by
nations. The U.S. War on Terrorism may therefore have created more U.S.
enemies than it killed.
11
Israeli and Arab residents of the region alternately called Israel or Palestine,
a strip of land that Jews claim as their birthright and Palestinians claim as
©
their own. Persistent fighting between Israelis and Palestinians over the
ht
course of the past 60 years shows little sign of waning (see Chapters 16, 17,
ig
and 21). Although most members of each group perceive their own leaders’
yr
lence may actually increase the level of violence between the groups. For
example, Israel’s top generals and intelligence officers have admitted that
their military actions in response to Palestinian suicide bombings have had
the effect of creating additional Palestinian terrorist cells (Moore, 2003).
A)
Thus, the GAM provides a useful framework for understanding how govern-
mental actions can produce an escalating cycle of violence between groups
P
(A
and even nations.
These two examples illustrate another key point about escalatory vio-
n
lence. When the two parties in a conflict have vastly different resources, their
io
forms of violence and of escalation will vary. Al-Qaeda cannot launch a con-
at
ventional war against the West, so its attacks include unconventional forms
ci
of violence, such as roadside or suicide bombings. Similarly, the Palestinians
so
cannot win a conventional war with Israel, so they resort to terrorist tactics.
As
al
USING THE GENERAL AGGRESSION MODEL TO REDUCE
ic
AGGRESSION AND VIOLENCE
og
ol
Thus far, we have dwelled on how the GAM can help to describe, pre-
ch
dict, and explain aggressive behavior. The GAM can also help researchers,
y
aggression. The most likely points for intervention will vary from case to case,
but several stand out. In the case of groups or individuals already in conflict,
an
the first step should be to stop the violence cycle. Interventions by third par-
ic
ties may be necessary. A second step should be to ensure that people’s basic
er
needs are met. A third step would be to address people’s symbolic needs (e.g.,
Am
effective in the case of individuals who have not already become aggression-
by
chological. This includes the learning of basic attitudes, beliefs, and values
20
Although retraining people who are already violence prone is more dif-
ht
ficult, research in the violent crime domain has found that intensive inter-
ig
Health and Human Services, 2001). This is a different conclusion from the
op
one that most scholars and public policy makers in the United States held as
recently as the late 1980s and early 1990s.
C
CONCLUSIONS
A)
aggressively. It identifies a wide range of factors that influence the develop-
ment of aggressive tendencies over time. It explains how highly aggressive
P
(A
contexts are created and maintained through violence escalation cycles. It
clarifies why government actions designed to bring about peace often fail, cre-
n
ating even more violent conflict. And perhaps most important, it offers pos-
io
sible solutions for preventing and reducing aggression and violence both
at
between individuals and groups.
ci
so
As
REFERENCES
al
Anderson, C. A., Buckley, K. E., & Carnagey, N. L. (2008). Creating your own hostile
ic
environment: A laboratory examination of trait aggression and the violence esca-
og
lation cycle. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 462–473. doi:10.1177/
0146167207311282 ol
ch
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of
y
Anderson, C. A., Carnagey, N. L., & Eubanks, J. (2003). Exposure to violent media:
The effects of songs with violent lyrics on aggressive thoughts and feelings. Journal
an
Handbook of basic principles (pp. 271–296). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
by
Avalos, H. (2005). Fighting words: The origins of religious violence. Buffalo, NY:
Prometheus Books.
11
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New York, NY: Prentice Hall.
20
Bargh, J. A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information processing and social
ig
Bartholow, B. D., Bushman, B. J., & Sestir, M. A. (2006). Chronic violent video
game exposure and desensitization: Behavioral and event-related brain poten-
tial data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 532–539. doi:10.1016/
j.jesp.2005.08.006
A)
Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration–aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformu-
lation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.106.1.59
P
(A
Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile
versus instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychological Review, 108, 273–279.
n
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.273
io
Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2002). Violent video games and hostile expec-
at
tations: A test of the general aggression model. Personality and Social Psychology
ci
Bulletin, 28, 1679–1686. doi:10.1177/014616702237649
so
Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-
As
esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-hate lead to
violence? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 219–229. doi:10.1037/
al
0022-3514.75.1.219
ic
Carnagey, N. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2005). The effects of reward and punishment
og
in violent video games on aggressive affect, cognition, and behavior. Psycho-
ol
logical Science, 16, 882–889. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01632.x
ch
Carnagey, N. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2007). Changes in attitudes towards war and
y
violence after September 11, 2001. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 118–129. doi:10.1002/
Ps
ab.20173
Carnagey, N. L., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2007). The effect of video game
an
Cooper, M. (1999, February 5). Officers in Bronx fire 41 shots, and an unarmed man
Am
killed.html
th
Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police officer’s
by
0022-3514.83.6.1314
20
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social informa-
tion processing mechanisms in children’s adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115,
©
74–101. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74
ht
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Stillman, T. F., & Gailliot, M. T. (2007). Violence
ig
j.jesp.2005.12.005
DeWall, C. N., Twenge, J. M., Gitter, S. A., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). It’s the
C
thought that counts: The role of hostile cognition in shaping aggressive responses
to social exclusion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 45–59. doi:
10.1037/a0013196
Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children’s aggressive behavior. Child
Development, 51, 162–170. doi:10.2307/1129603
A)
Finkel, E. J., DeWall, C. N., Slotter, E. B., Oaten, M., & Foshee, V. A. (2009). Self-
regulatory failure and intimate partner violence perpetration. Journal of Person-
P
ality and Social Psychology, 97, 483–499. doi:10.1037/a0015433
(A
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
n
McGraw-Hill.
io
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice,
at
D. M., . . . Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited
ci
energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and
so
Social Psychology, 92, 325–336. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.325
As
Ghosh, B. (2005, June 28). Inside the mind of an Iraqi suicide bomber. Time. Retrieved
from http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1077288,00.html
al
Haney, C., Banks, W. C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1973). Interpersonal dynamics in a
ic
simulated prison. International Journal of Criminology and Penology, 1, 69–97.
og
Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy: A clinical construct whose time has come. Criminal
ol
Justice and Behavior, 23, 25–54. doi:10.1177/0093854896023001004
ch
Hoyle, R. H., Pinkley, R. L., & Insko, C. A. (1989). Perceptions of social behav-
y
1<13::AID-AB2480140104>3.0.CO;2-J
Am
Jaffe, Y., Shapir, N., & Yinon, Y. (1981). Aggression and its escalation. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 12, 21–36. doi:10.1177/0022022181121002
e
Miller, A. (Ed.). (2004). The social psychology of good and evil (pp. 168–192). New
ig
A)
Neuilly, M.-A. (2007). When murder is not enough: Toward a new definition of com-
munity violence. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 12, 598–610. doi:10.1016/
P
(A
j.avb.2007.02.004
Öhman, A., Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd effect: An
n
anger superiority effect with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social
io
Psychology, 80, 381–396.
at
Plant, E. A., & Peruche, B. M. (2005). The consequences of race for police officers’
ci
responses to criminal suspects. Psychological Science, 16, 180–183. doi:10.1111/
so
j.0956-7976.2005.00800.x
As
Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational
themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 233–269. doi:10.1080/
al
02699939308409189
ic
og
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social
behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. doi:10.1207/
s15327957pspr0803_1 ol
ch
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In
y
J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 19–136). New
Ps
Wegner, D. M., & Bargh, J. A. (1998). Control and automaticity in social life. In
er
Zajonc, R. B. (1965, July 16). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274. doi:10.1126/
science.149.3681.269
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
11
by
5/25/10
th
e
Am
11:29 AM
er
ic
an
Page 34
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 35
PA)
I3 THEORY: INSTIGATING,
(A
IMPELLING, AND INHIBITING
n
io
FACTORS IN AGGRESSION
at
ci
so
ERICA B. SLOTTER AND ELI J. FINKEL
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ical coherence on the massive number of established risk factors for aggression
Am
and (b) to use the tools of statistical (and conceptual) moderation to gain new
insights into the processes by which a previously nonaggressive interaction
e
escalates into an aggressive one (see Finkel, 2007, 2008). I3 theory (pronounced
th
“I-cubed theory”) does not advance one key variable (or even a specific set of
by
given risk factor promotes aggression and (b) how multiple risk factors inter-
20
main and interactive effects of the instigating triggers, impelling forces, and
ig
sion, which in this chapter refers to any behavior carried out with the primary
proximal goal of inflicting physical harm on a target who is motivated to
C
avoid being harmed (Baron & Richardson, 1994). (We do not examine in
35
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 36
A)
I3 theory, the GAM focuses less on a particular variable or process than on
general classes of aggression risk factors and processes. The GAM consists
P
(A
of three main foci. The first emphasizes person and situation inputs, or risk
factors, for aggression. Person inputs include personality traits, gender, beliefs,
n
attitudes, values, long-term goals, and scripts; situation inputs include aggressive
io
cues (e.g., presence of guns), provocation, frustration, pain and discomfort,
at
drugs, and incentives (determined by a cost/benefit analysis). The second focus
ci
is the interconnected affective, arousal, and cognitive routes, or mechanisms,
so
through which the inputs influence aggressive behavior. Affective routes include
As
mood and emotion and expressive motor tendencies; arousal routes include
the strengthening of a dominant action tendency or certain misattribution
al
processes; cognitive routes include hostile thoughts and scripts. Finally, the
ic
third focus is the outcomes of the underlying appraisal and decision processes.
og
Individuals are likely to act impulsively when they lack the resources and
ol
motivation to alter their immediate appraisal of the situation. If they possess
ch
the resources and motivation, however, they may reappraise the situation and
y
I3 THEORY
ic
er
regulation as a core emphasis of the model, and it specifies the novel ways in
which aggression risk factors interact to predict aggressive behavior.
11
The theory begins by posing three questions. First, does at least one indi-
20
override the aggressive impulses? Each affirmative answer increases the like-
ig
lihood of aggressive behavior via both a main effect and interactive effects
yr
with variables relevant to one or both of the other questions. Whereas the
op
the collective power of the variables that cause the individual to override this
aggressive urge.
In addition to these three initial questions, I3 theory poses a fourth: How
do effects of variables in one category (i.e., instigating triggers, impelling forces,
A)
or inhibiting forces) interact with effects of one or more variables from the other
categories to predict aggressive behavior? As presented in Table 2.1, answering
P
(A
these four questions enables scholars to identify seven key I3 theory effects. Fig-
ure 2.1 (which builds on work by Fals-Stewart, Leonard, & Birchler, 2005) illus-
n
trates how these seven effects can work together to increase or decrease the
io
likelihood of aggressive behavior.
at
I3 theory diverges from the aggression theories mentioned earlier in its
ci
central emphasis on inhibitory processes. The theory recognizes the impor-
so
tance of instigating triggers and impelling forces, but it argues that such fac-
As
tors cause individuals to enact aggressive behavior only when their collective
power is stronger than the collective power of inhibitory processes. Although
al
other theories address the importance of inhibitory processes in aggression
ic
(e.g., Chapters 1, 6, 9, and 15), the emphasis on such processes gains new
og
prominence with I3 theory.
ol
ch
Instigating Triggers
y
Ps
The first stage of I3 theory concerns the presence of one or more instigat-
ing triggers, which are discrete situational events or circumstances that induce
an
lower left of Figure 2.1, impelling and inhibiting forces are irrelevant when
er
instigating triggers are absent. Even the world’s angriest, least controlled per-
Am
son is not aggressive all the time; some situational variable (even if it only
serves to activate a long-standing goal or grievance) is required before the per-
e
son becomes aggressive. Aversive events can trigger (via automatic associa-
th
physiological, and even motor tendencies that prime the individual to aggress
(Berkowitz, 1993; Chapters 1 and 9, this volume). I3 theory suggests that cer-
11
tain events can also trigger aggressive tendencies driven by instrumental goals
20
(Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer, & Sears, 1939), and social rejection (Leary,
op
nated in somebody other than the target. The same kinds of triggers that lead
I3 THEORY 37
C
op
yr
ig
ht
38
©
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd
20
11
5/25/10
by
TABLE 2.1
3 The Seven Effects
th I Theory of Aggression:
I3 Effect
e
11:29 AM
4 Stage 1 × Stage 2 Instigating Trigger × Impelling Ego Threat × Narcissism Bushman &
an
Forces Interaction Effect Baumeister, 1998
5 Stage 1 × Stage 3 Instigating Trigger × Inhibiting Provocation × Self-Regulatory Finkel et al., 2009
Forces Interaction Effect Strength
Ps
6 Stage 2 × Stage 3 Impelling Forces × Inhibiting
yc Physical Proclivity × Negative Finkel & Foshee,
Forces Interaction Effect Outcome Expectancies 2009
7 Stage 1 × Stage 2 Instigating Trigger × Impelling [No Known Example] [No Known Example]
ho
× Stage 3 Forces × Inhibiting Forces lo
Interaction Effect gi
ca
lA
s so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 39
Violence Threshold
Given Strong
A)
Inhibiting Forces
P
(A
n
io
Violence Threshold
at
Given Weak
Inhibiting Forces
ci
so
Absent Moderate Extreme
As
Intensity of Instigating Triggers
al
Figure 2.1. How the three components of I3 theory interrelate to predict aggressive
behavior. For ease of illustration, impelling forces and inhibiting forces are depicted
ic
as if they are binary—either weak or strong. In reality, the intensity of each type of
og
force varies continuously from weak to strong.
ol
ch
y
lead to this tendency with respect to a third party. For example, an individ-
ual who feels provoked or rejected may experience an instigation to aggress
an
not only against the source of the provocation or rejection but also (or alter-
ic
nately) toward another target whom the potential perpetrator believes would
er
make a more acceptable or desirable target (e.g., somebody who is less likely
Am
to fight back).
e
Impelling Forces
th
by
The second stage of I3 theory concerns risk factors that determine the
strength of the aggressive impulse experienced by the individual, through
11
main effects and through interactions with instigating triggers. In some situ-
20
ations, individuals may effortlessly shrug off (or perhaps not even notice; see
Chapter 9) an instigating trigger, experiencing virtually no impulse toward
©
powerful aggressive impulses when impelling forces are strong than when they
are weak (see dashed vs. dotted lines in Figure 2.1), especially to the degree
C
I3 THEORY 39
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 40
Impelling forces fall into one of four categories: evolutionary and cul-
tural, personal, dyadic, and situational. Evolutionary and cultural impellors refer
to features of the potentially aggressive individual’s biological or cultural her-
itage, including evolutionary adaptations and social norms (see Chapters 3
A)
and 10). Examples include adaptations resulting from evolutionary pressures
that provided ancestral men and women with a survival advantage for expe-
P
(A
riencing violent impulses in certain situations (Lorenz, 1966) and social
norms delineating the extent to which certain instigating triggers provoke
n
strong aggressive impulses (Nisbett & Cohen, 1996). Personal impellors refer
io
to relatively stable characteristics of a given individual that differ from
at
those of many other individuals, including personality characteristics, atti-
ci
so
tudes, beliefs, interpersonal interaction styles, or biological factors. Exam-
ples include dispositional hostility (Norlander & Eckhardt, 2005; Chapters
As
5 and 8, this volume), narcissism (Twenge & Campbell, 2003; Chapter 11,
this volume), and testosterone (Dabbs, Frady, Carr, & Besch, 1987; Van
al
ic
Goozen, Frijda, & Van de Poll, 1994). Dyadic impellors refer to characteris-
og
tics of the relationship between the potential aggressor and the potential
target. Examples include dissatisfaction with the amount of power one has
ol
in a relationship (Ronfeldt, Kimerling, & Arias, 1998), target-specific jeal-
ch
ousy (Dutton, van Ginkel, & Landolt, 1996; Holtzworth-Munroe, Stuart,
y
Inhibiting Forces
th
by
The third stage of I3 theory concerns risk factors that determine whether
11
individuals will override the aggressive impulses that emerge from the instigat-
ing triggers, impelling factors, and their interaction. In some situations, indi-
20
aggressive behavior. If the inhibiting forces are weak (i.e., the lower horizontal
op
line in Figure 2.1), then aggressive impulses need not be especially strong to
C
result in aggressive behavior. If the inhibiting forces are strong (i.e., the upper
horizontal line in Figure 2.1), then aggressive impulses must be strong to result
in aggressive behavior.
As with impelling forces, inhibiting forces fall into one of four categories:
evolutionary and cultural, personal, dyadic, and situational. Examples of evo-
A)
lutionary and cultural inhibitors include adaptations resulting from evolutionary
pressures that provided ancestral men and women with a survival advantage
P
(A
for overriding aggressive impulses in certain situations (Baumeister, 2005;
Chapter 3, this volume) and social norms or institutions that decrease the like-
n
lihood that individuals will act on aggressive impulses (Guerra, Huesmann, &
io
Spindler, 2003; Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997; Chapter 10, this vol-
at
ume). Examples of personal inhibitors include dispositional self-control (Finkel,
ci
DeWall, Slotter, Oaten, & Foshee, 2009), executive functioning (Giancola,
so
2000; Chapter 6, this volume), and beliefs that enacting aggressive behavior
As
will lead to poor outcomes for the self (Slaby & Guerra, 1988). Examples of
dyadic inhibitors include partner empathy or perspective taking (Richardson,
al
Green, & Lago, 1998; Van Baardewijk, Stegge, Bushman, & Vermeiren, in
ic
press), relationship commitment (Gaertner & Foshee, 1999; Slotter, Finkel,
og
& Bodenhausen, 2009), and relative physical size (Archer & Benson, 2008;
ol
Felson, 1996; Chapter 3, this volume). Finally, examples of situational
ch
inhibitors include sobriety (i.e., vs. alcohol intoxication; Bushman & Cooper,
y
Baumeister, Stillman, & Gailliot, 2007; Finkel et al., 2009; Chapter 6, this
volume), and plentiful cognitive processing time (Finkel et al., 2009).
an
ic
I3 theory encompasses seven key effects: three main effects (i.e., insti-
11
gating triggers, impelling forces, and inhibiting forces), three two-way inter-
action effects (i.e., instigating triggers × impelling forces, instigating triggers
20
× inhibiting forces, and impelling forces × inhibiting forces), and one three-
way interaction effect (instigating triggers × impelling forces × inhibiting
©
forces). Table 2.1 lists these seven effects and provides an example of each.
ht
I3 THEORY 41
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 42
A)
administered substantially louder, more painful sound blasts to an unknown
stranger than did participants who had been unanimously accepted (Twenge,
P
(A
Baumeister, Tice, & Stucke, 2001, Study 5). In this study, social rejection
served as a displaced instigating trigger because the target of the aggression
n
was not a member of the group who had previously rejected the participant.
io
Another study, which provided an in-depth analysis of all well-documented
at
school shootings in the United States between 1995 and 2001, yielded com-
ci
patible conclusions, with acute or chronic rejection preceding the shootings
so
in 87% of the cases (Leary, Kowalski, Smith, & Phillips, 2003).
As
2. An Illustrative Main Effect of Impelling Forces: Testosterone
al
ic
As shown in the second row of Table 2.1, an illustrative impelling force
og
is the androgen testosterone. Although testosterone is higher in men than in
ol
women, its level predicts aggression in both sexes (Archer, Birring, & Wu,
ch
1998; Dabbs & Hargrove, 1997; see Sapolsky, 1998). In a study of male prison
inmates, testosterone levels correlated positively with crime severity; indeed,
y
Ps
nine of the 11 inmates with the lowest testosterone levels had committed
nonviolent crimes, whereas 10 of the 11 inmates with the highest testos-
an
terone levels had committed violent crimes (Dabbs et al., 1987). In another
study, female-to-male transsexuals became considerably more aggressive in
ic
er
self-regulation (Baumeister, Gailliot, DeWall, & Oaten, 2006), all such acts
20
in the short term but it also can be bolstered over time by adherence to a
ig
men strengthens a muscle over time. In one study, hungry participants who
op
ner to eat a snack with plentiful hot sauce despite his or her distaste for spicy
foods) than were participants who had previously resisted eating a less tempt-
ing food (radishes; low ego depletion condition), even though participants in
the two conditions did not differ in how angry they were in response to the
A)
provocation (DeWall et al., 2007, Study 1).
Complementing this evidence that short-term self-regulatory exertions
P
(A
can deplete self-regulatory resources and thereby predict elevated aggression
is evidence that longer term self-regulatory exertion regimens can bolster self-
n
regulatory strength and thereby predict reduced aggression. A recent study
io
demonstrated that individuals who had adhered to a 2-week self-regulatory
at
strength-bolstering regimen declined significantly from before to after the reg-
ci
imen in their aggressive tendencies toward their romantic partner (Finkel
so
et al., 2009, Study 5). In this study, participants who deliberately regulated
As
either their physical behavior (e.g., brushing their teeth with their non-
dominant hand) or their verbal behavior (e.g., making sure not to begin sen-
al
tences with “I”) reported a reduced likelihood of being physically aggressive
ic
in response to various partner provocations (e.g., “I walk in and catch my part-
og
ner having sex with someone”), whereas participants in a no-intervention
ol
condition exhibited no change from pretest to posttest.
ch
4. An Illustrative Instigating Trigger × Impelling Forces Interaction Effect:
y
Ps
Although many scholars have suggested that low self-esteem causes aggres-
Am
sion, others have increasingly argued that a form of high self-esteem is fre-
quently more likely to do so. In particular, individuals whose self-views are
e
not only favorable but also unstable, inflated, or uncertain are especially
th
prone toward aggressive behavior when their favorable self-views are socially
by
threatened (Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996; Chapter 11, this volume).
One series of studies demonstrated that participants who had experienced an
11
ego threat in the form of insulting feedback about an essay they had written
20
(an instigating trigger) were more aggressive toward the same-sex provoca-
teur (subjecting him or her to painfully loud noise blasts) than were partic-
©
ipants who had not experienced an ego threat (Bushman & Baumeister,
ht
1998). The key finding, however, was that this main effect of ego threat was
ig
substantially larger for participants who were high in narcissism (an impelling
yr
study demonstrated that the link between social rejection and displaced
I3 THEORY 43
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 44
aggression (painful noise blasts) was substantially stronger for individuals who
were high in narcissism than for those who were low in narcissism (Twenge &
Campbell, 2003, Study 4), and another demonstrated that the link between
social rejection and aggression (aversive hot sauce) was substantially stronger
A)
for individuals who were high in rejection sensitivity (those who anxiously
expect, readily perceive, and overreact to rejection) than for those who were
P
(A
low in rejection sensitivity (Ayduk, Gyurak, & Luerssen, 2008).
n
5. An Illustrative Instigating Trigger × Inhibiting Forces Interaction Effect:
io
Provocation × Self-Regulatory Strength
at
ci
As shown in the fifth row of Table 2.1, an illustrative instigating trigger
so
× inhibiting forces interaction effect is provocation × self-regulatory strength. As
discussed previously, both provocation and self-regulatory strength predict
As
aggression via main effects. Recent studies confirm the I3 theory prediction that
al
incorporating their interaction effect yields a richer story (e.g., Chapter 6). Two
ic
recent experiments, one in which the aggression was directed at strangers (aver-
og
sive sound blasts; DeWall et al., 2007, Study 2) and one in which the aggres-
ol
sion was directed at one’s romantic partner (forcing him or her to maintain
ch
body poses for painfully long durations; Finkel et al., 2009, Study 4), demon-
strated that participants were especially aggressive when they experienced both
y
Ps
action effect is provocation severity × frontal lobe functioning (Lau, Pihl, &
ic
underlies the ability to control one’s impulses (Hecaen & Albert, 1978).
Consistent with I3 theory, participants were more aggressive (administering
painful electric shocks) to the degree that the opponent had previously pro-
e
th
voked them severely rather than mildly (i.e., had administered painful electric
shocks to them), but this provocation main effect was substantially stronger
by
among individuals with weak rather than strong frontal lobe functioning (Lau
11
et al., 1995).
Yet another example of an instigating trigger × inhibiting forces inter-
20
In this study, participants who had just consumed four alcoholic or placebo
beverages were provoked in either a salient or a subtle way and then had the
ht
the provocateur would have to keep his or her hand immersed in iced water.
yr
in the subtle provocation condition, and this effect was significantly stronger
C
A)
provoked them, and this provocation main effect was especially strong among
participants who were low in relationship commitment. Extending work sug-
P
(A
gesting that commitment promotes prorelationship behaviors in other con-
flictual relationship domains, such as forgiveness (Finkel, Rusbult, Kumashiro,
n
& Hannon, 2002), it appears that individuals who are highly committed to
io
their romantic relationships are able to override aggressive impulses when their
at
partner provokes them.
ci
so
6. An Illustrative Impelling Forces × Inhibiting Forces Interaction Effect:
As
Physical Proclivity × Negative Outcome Expectancies
al
As shown in the sixth row of Table 2.1, an illustrative impelling forces
ic
× inhibiting forces interaction effect is physical proclivity × negative outcome
og
expectancies. Individuals vary in the degree to which they prefer physical ver-
sus cognitive tasks, and a relative preference for the former predicts increased
ol
tendencies toward aggression and criminal behavior (Gottfredson & Hirschi,
ch
1990). Individuals also vary in the degree to which they believe that engaging
y
Ps
predict decreased tendencies toward aggression (Slaby & Guerra, 1988). One
recent study examined whether physical proclivity (an impelling factor)
ic
er
expectancies were low. It seems that the tendencies to prefer physical to cog-
nitive tasks predicts greater aggression among individuals whose aggression is
by
not restrained by beliefs that being aggressive will bring about negative effects
11
Although the three main effects and the three two-way interaction effects
reviewed previously are key components of I3 theory, the instigating triggers ×
ig
most important component of the theory. Indeed, the theory suggests that all
op
three of the preceding two-way interaction effects are moderated by third vari-
C
I3 THEORY 45
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 46
A)
strength (instigating trigger × inhibiting factor) interaction effect is moderated
P
(A
by an impelling factor (e.g., testosterone, physical proclivity, dispositional
anger), and (c) whether the physical proclivity × negative outcome expectancy
n
(impelling factor × inhibiting factor) interaction effect is moderated by an insti-
io
gating trigger (e.g., social rejection, ego threat, provocation). Testing for such
at
interaction effects is an important direction for future research.
ci
so
As
DISCUSSION
al
I3 theory seeks (a) to impose enhanced theoretical coherence on the
ic
vast array of aggression risk factors by identifying how each of them increases
og
the likelihood of aggression (via instigating triggers, impelling forces, and/or
ol
inhibiting forces) and (b) to examine the manner in which risk factors from
ch
one category interact with those from one or both of the other categories to
y
predict aggressive behavior. In the preceding section (also see Table 2.1 and
Ps
Figure 2.1), we reviewed specific examples of how certain risk factors fit
into I3 theory and how they interface with variables from the other I3 theory
an
categories.
ic
One important direction for future research, aside from providing the
er
into more than one category if a given variable both increases aggressive
by
cation are instigating triggers; (b) that testosterone, narcissism, and physi-
20
cal proclivity are impelling factors; and (c) that self-regulatory strength, lack
of alcohol consumption, and negative outcome expectancies are inhibiting
©
the inhibition threshold (see the horizontal lines in Figure 2.1), thereby
increasing the likelihood that individuals will override aggressive impulses.
C
That said, perhaps such dispositional self-control also predicts reduced aggres-
A)
decreasing restraint? One promising approach is to adapt recent developments
in process dissociation paradigms used by social cognition researchers. Schol-
P
(A
ars have recently modeled behavior on laboratory tasks to discern the degree
to which participants exhibit certain automatic tendencies (e.g., toward preju-
n
dice or discrimination) and also controlled tendencies that override these
io
automatic tendencies (Payne, 2001; Sherman et al., 2008; see also Jacoby,
at
1991). After developing empirical procedures for distinguishing impulses
ci
toward aggressive behavior from self-controlled processes that override those
so
impulses, scholars will be able to examine the association of a given risk fac-
As
tor with both (a) individuals’ tendencies to experience impulses to aggress and
(b) their tendencies to override those impulses (Chapter 6, this volume). We
al
predict that variables such as testosterone, narcissism, and physical proclivity
ic
will correlate positively with the automatic aggressive tendencies identified by
og
these process dissociation procedures and negligibly with the controlled ten-
ol
dencies that override these automatic tendencies. In contrast, we predict that
ch
variables such as self-regulatory strength, sobriety, and negative outcome
y
cies identified by these process dissociation procedures and positively with the
controlled tendencies that override these automatic tendencies.
an
inhibitory forces may turn out to be more effective on average than interven-
tions designed to weaken impelling forces. Early research inspired by I3 the-
©
can reduce aggressive behavior (Finkel et al., 2009; Slotter et al., 2009; see
Finkel et al., 2002, for evidence that relationship commitment is amenable
C
I3 THEORY 47
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 48
factors such as prevalent social norms also seem to influence aggressive behav-
ior (Guerra et al., 2003; Sampson, Raudenbush, & Earls, 1997; Chapter 10,
this volume), which hints at the possibility that large-scale social interventions
could potentially reduce aggression at the societal level.
A)
In conclusion, I3 theory is an attempt to categorize aggression risk fac-
tors into instigating triggers, impelling factors, and/or inhibiting factors and
P
(A
to identify the interplay among variables across categories (see Figure 2.1).
To the degree that extant theory provides good reason to believe that partic-
n
ular risk factors fit relatively neatly into one of the I3 theory categories, the
io
theory provides an immediately accessible agenda for future research, ori-
at
ented less toward identifying additional risk factors than toward identifying
ci
(a) the processes by which risk factors, considered in isolation, increase
so
aggression and (b) the manner in which they interact to do so. In the long
As
run, I3 theory can inform interventions designed to help individuals manage
their aggressive impulses in a constructive manner. Indeed, scholarship may
al
well progress to the point where interventions can be tailored to the specific
ic
inhibiting risk factors most relevant to a given person, perhaps reducing one
og
individual’s aggression by bolstering self-regulatory strength and reducing
ol
another individual’s aggression by bolstering empathy. Such tailored inter-
ch
ventions, which would require valid assessment instruments, hold particular
y
REFERENCES
ic
er
Anderson, C. A., Anderson, K. B., Dorr, N., DeNeve, K. M., & Flanagan, M. (2000).
Am
Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggres-
th
effects of songs with violent lyrics on aggressive thoughts and feelings. Journal of
ht
Archer, J., & Benson, D. (2008). Physical aggression as a function of perceived fighting
yr
Archer, J., Birring, S. S., & Wu, F. C. W. (1998). The association between testosterone
C
A)
tivity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 775–782. doi:10.1016/
j.jesp.2007.07.004
P
(A
Baron, R. A., & Richardson, D. R. (1994). Human aggression (2nd ed.). New York,
NY: Plenum Press.
n
Baumeister, R. F. (2005). The cultural animal: Human nature, meaning, and social life.
io
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
at
Baumeister, R. F., Gailliot, M., DeWall, C. N., & Oaten, M. (2006). Self-regulation
ci
and personality: How interventions increase regulatory success, and how
so
depletion moderates the effects of trait on behavior. Journal of Personality, 74,
As
1773–1802. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00428.x
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism
al
to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological
ic
Review, 103, 5–33. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.5
og
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York, NY:
McGraw-Hill. ol
ch
Berkowitz, L. (1998). Affective aggression: The role of stress, pain, and negative
y
research, and implications for social policy (pp. 49–72). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
an
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.3.422
Am
0022-3514.75.1.219
by
0033-2909.107.3.341
20
Dabbs, J. M., Jr., Frady, R. L., Carr, T. S., & Besch, N. F. (1987). Saliva testosterone
yr
Dabbs, J. M., Jr., & Hargrove, M. F. (1997). Age, testosterone, and behavior among
C
I3 THEORY 49
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 50
Denson, T. F., Aviles, F. E., Pollock, V. E., Earleywince, M., Vasquez, E. A., &
Miller, N. (2008). The effects of alcohol and the salience of aggressive cues
on triggered displaced aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 25–33. doi:10.1002/
ab.20177
A)
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Stillman, R. G., & Gailliot, M. T. (2007). Vio-
lence restrained: Effects of self-regulation and its depletion on aggression. Jour-
P
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 62–76. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.005
(A
Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustra-
n
tion and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. doi:10.1037/10022-000
io
Dutton, D. G., van Ginkel, C., & Landolt, M. (1996). Jealousy, intrusiveness, and
at
intimate abusiveness. Journal of Family Violence, 11, 411–423. doi:10.1007/
ci
BF02333425
so
Fals-Stewart, W., Leonard, K. E., & Birchler, G. R. (2005). The occurrence of male-
As
to-female intimate partner violence on days of men’s drinking: The moderating
effects of antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
al
chology, 73, 239–248. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.239
ic
Felson, R. B. (1996). Big people hit little people: Sex differences in physical power
og
and interpersonal violence. Criminology, 34, 433–452. doi:10.1111/j.1745-
9125.1996.tb01214.x ol
ch
Finkel, E. J. (2007). Impelling and inhibiting forces in the perpetration of intimate
y
1089-2680.11.2.193
Finkel, E. J. (2008). Intimate partner violence perpetration: Insights from the science
an
tive, affective, and motivational processes (pp. 271–288). New York, NY: Psychol-
er
ogy Press.
Am
Finkel, E. J., DeWall, C. N., Slotter, E., B., Oaten, M., & Foshee, V. A. (2009). Self-
regulatory failure and intimate partner violence perpetration. Journal of Person-
e
Finkel, E. J., & Foshee, V. A. (2009). Impelling and inhibiting forces in the perpetration
by
Finkel, E. J., Rusbult, C. E., Kumashiro, M., & Hannon, P. A. (2002). Dealing
20
3514.82.6.956
ht
Gaertner, L., & Foshee, V. (1999). Commitment and the perpetration of relation-
ig
tb00189.x
op
doi:10.1037/1064-1297.8.4.576
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Guerra, N. G., Huesmann, L. R., & Spindler, A. (2003). Community violence expo-
sure, social cognition, and aggression among urban elementary school children.
A)
Child Development, 74, 1561–1576. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00623
P
Hecaen, H., & Albert, M. L. (1978). Human neuropsychology. New York, NY: Wiley.
(A
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., Stuart, G. L., & Hutchinson, G. (1997). Violent versus non-
violent husbands: Differences in attachment patterns, dependency, and jealousy.
n
Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 314–331. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.11.3.314
io
at
Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from
ci
intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541.
so
doi:10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F
Lau, M. A., Pihl, R. O., & Peterson, J. B. (1995). Provocation, acute alcohol intox-
As
ication, cognitive performance, and aggression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
al
104, 150–155. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.150
ic
Leary, M. R., Kowalski, R. M., Smith, L., & Phillips, S. (2003). Teasing, rejection,
og
and violence: Case studies of the school shootings. Aggressive Behavior, 29,
202–214. doi:10.1002/ab.10061
ol
ch
Leary, M. R., Twenge, J. M., & Quinlivan, E. (2006). Interpersonal rejection as a
determinant of anger and aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
y
Lorenz, K. (1966). On aggression. San Diego, CA: Harcourt, Brace & World.
an
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). Culture of honor: The psychology of violence in the
ic
Norlander, B., & Eckhardt, C. (2005). Anger, hostility, and male perpetrators of inti-
Am
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled
th
Richardson, D. R., Green, L. R., & Lago, T. (1998). The relationship between
11
Ronfeldt, H. M., Kimerling, R., & Arias, I. (1998). Satisfaction with relationship
©
power and the perpetration of dating violence. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
ily, 60, 70–78. doi:10.2307/353442
ht
ig
Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997, August 15). Neighborhoods
yr
Sapolsky, R. M. (1998). The trouble with testosterone and other essays on the biology of
C
I3 THEORY 51
12051-03_CH02-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:29 AM Page 52
Sherman, J. W., Gowronski, B., Gonsalkorale, K., Hugenberg, K., Allen, T. J., &
Groom, C. J. (2008). The self-regulation of automatic associations and behavioral
impulses. Psychological Review, 115, 314–335. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.314
Slaby, R. G., & Guerra, N. G. (1988). Cognitive mediators of aggression in adolescent
A)
offenders: I. Assessment. Developmental Psychology, 24, 580–588. doi:10.1037/
0012-1649.24.4.580
P
(A
Slotter, E. B., Finkel, E. J., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2009). Provocation and commit-
ment in intimate partner violence. Unpublished manuscript, Northwestern Uni-
n
versity, Evanston, IL.
io
Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. J. (2001). If you can’t join
at
them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. Journal of Per-
ci
sonality and Social Psychology, 81, 1058–1069. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1058
so
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2003). Isn’t it fun to get the respect we deserve?
As
Narcissism, social rejection, and aggression. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 29, 261–272. doi:10.1177/0146167202239051
al
Van Baardewijk, Y., Stegge, H., Bushman, B. J., & Vermeiren, R. (in press). “I feel your
ic
pain” (but only when I am forced to focus on it): Psychopathy, distress, and aggres-
og
sive behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines.
ol
Van Goozen, S. H. M., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., Gooren, L. J. G., Frijda, N. H., & Van
ch
de Poll, N. E. (1995). Gender differences in behaviour: Activating effects
y
0306-4530(94)00076-X
Van Goozen, S., Frijda, N., & Van de Poll, N. (1994). Anger and aggression in women:
an
3.0.CO;2-4
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
PA)
APPLYING ADAPTATIONISM
(A
TO HUMAN ANGER:
n
io
THE RECALIBRATIONAL THEORY
at
ci
so
AARON SELL
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
In the late 1960s and early 1970s, evolutionary biology underwent a sci-
ic
fying and describing design in organisms, a method that matches the engineer-
th
human anger. The resultant theory, called the recalibrational theory, states that
20
the factors that mitigate it, and its effects on physiology, perception, and cog-
ht
angry person’s interests so that they become less likely to impose costs or deny
benefits to the angry individual. Anger deploys two main strategies to con-
C
vince the target to treat the angry individual better: (a) toward those with
53
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 54
A)
in design and function to aggression against conspecifics in other animals.
P
(A
EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY OF CONFLICT
n
io
Natural selection is the only process shown capable of creating complex
at
functional design in organisms (Williams, 1966). Evolutionary psychology is
ci
an approach to psychology that uses analyses of selection pressures to discover
so
and understand the cognitive machinery designed by those selection pres-
As
sures. The closer the fit between the logic of the selection pressures and the
known features of the mechanism under study, the more confident one can
al
be about the proposed function of the mechanism, the accuracy of the posited
ic
selection pressures, and any future predictions derived from the model. There-
og
fore, a thorough examination of both the selection pressures and the proposed
ol
cognitive mechanism are necessary for a theoretically sound computational
ch
model of any organic mechanism designed by natural selection. An analysis
y
of the selection pressures inherent in conflicts of interest and the major fea-
Ps
tures of human anger fit together well, making a strong case that human anger
was designed by natural selection to regulate conflicts of interest.
an
ic
to mate with members of the same species, eat the same foods, and seek to
th
occupy the same territories. Selection pressures to gain increased access to finite
by
resources will naturally produce organisms that are designed to compete for
those resources. Excluding kin, and assuming there are no reciprocal conse-
11
quences, natural selection will design one organism to attempt to gain resources
20
flicts of interest led evolutionary biologists to two key variables that animals
ig
The first of these is the relative value of the contested resource to the indi-
op
vidual organisms (i.e., who needs it more). This variable determines an organ-
ism’s fitness budget (i.e., the maximum cost an organism is willing to incur to
C
54 AARON SELL
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 55
A)
isms (Hammerstein & Parker, 1982; Maynard Smith & Parker, 1976). Con-
testants that are better able to impose costs will deplete their competitor’s
P
(A
fitness budgets earlier and win the conflict. If these two selection pressures,
relative formidability and relative valuation, have been active in the design
n
of animal brains, then we would expect to see animals respond in adaptive
io
ways to assessments of these two variables.
at
Five converging lines of evidence demonstrate that animals have been
ci
designed to respond adaptively to the selection pressures inherent in conflicts
so
of interest as modeled by evolutionary biologists.
As
䡲 Animals are designed such that relative formidability partially
al
determines the resolution of resource conflicts. The advantage
ic
of being the more formidable competitor has been noted in
og
species as distantly related as the sea anemone (Brace & Pavey,
ol
1978), African buffalo (Sinclair, 1977), and crayfish (Hazlett,
ch
Rubenstein, & Ritschoff, 1975). A particularly dramatic exam-
ple was found by Petrie (1984), who studied territory size in the
y
Ps
䡲 Animals are designed such that the relative valuation of the con-
er
tial valuation of food; it has also been found for differential value
11
A)
ity. Furthermore, the existence of dominance hierarchies in many
species indicates the ability of animals to measure their relative
P
(A
formidability. (For a review of the nonprimate animal literature,
see Huntingford & Turner, 1987; for primate examples, see Smuts,
n
Cheney, Seyfarth, Wrangham, & Struhsaker, 1987.)
io
䡲 Animals are designed to demonstrate formidability to lower the
at
costs of conflict. Two organisms can minimize the cost of con-
ci
flict if the eventual loser can recognize that it will lose. If a type
so
of animal aggression is designed to demonstrate relative formi-
As
dability, these conflicts should follow a general pattern of esca-
lation in which lower-cost demonstrations (which are probably
al
less accurate) are exchanged before higher-cost demonstrations
ic
or nonritualistic, “no holds barred” combat ensues. Large dis-
og
crepancies in formidability should be evident even in cases
ol
where demonstrations of formidability are somewhat inaccu-
ch
rate. For example, tail beating in male cichlid fish (i.e., shaking
y
red deer (Clutton-Brock & Albon, 1979), and pigs (Jensen &
Yngvesson, 1998).
11
56 AARON SELL
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 57
A)
Constructing a Cognitive Model of Human Anger
P
(A
Human conflicts of interest often look different from nonhuman animal
resource competition largely because of humans’ ability to mentally represent
n
conflicts of interests. A dung fly can fight with a competitor over a piece of
io
food, but a human can fight over whether someone should have gotten a piece
at
of food or even whether someone should have agreed with someone else who
ci
wanted to give a piece of food to a third party, and so on. Most human con-
so
flicts of interest do not involve tangible material resources but instead involve
As
conflicts over courses of action (retrospective and prospective), exchanges of
information, social alliances, and other abstract cost–benefit tradeoffs between
al
individuals. The logic of the selection pressures and the role of relative valua-
ic
og
tion and relative formidability, however, are the same.
An analysis of human conflicts and the selection pressures surrounding
ol
conflicts of interest argues for the existence of an internal variable similar to
ch
that which underlies dominance hierarchies in nonhuman animals, which
y
called a welfare tradeoff ratio (Tooby, Cosmides, Sell, Liberman, & Sznycer,
an
they interact with. For an agent, X, the WTR with respect to agent Y defines
by
the cost–benefit ratio below which X will give Y the resource and above which
X will attempt to take the resource. This is represented mathematically as
11
v ( X ) > v ( Y ) ⴱ WTRxy
20
©
In other words, the WTR X has toward Y (WTRxy) indicates how much weight
ht
X puts on Y’s interests when making decisions that affect them both. For exam-
ig
ple, if X’s WTR toward Y is .5, then X will give up a resource worth 5 to himself
yr
wound but unwilling to ruin a work supervisor’s sweater for that same pur-
pose, because the WTR he or she has with respect to the supervisor is higher
than toward a stranger.
Given mutual human dependence, the costs of contests, and the nature
A)
of kin and friendship, natural selection is predicted to have designed humans
such that WTRs will be set higher based on numerous factors related to
P
(A
another’s ability to enforce his or her own welfare. One set of factors is related
to the ability to enforce WTRs by threatening to inflict harm; these factors
n
include, for example, greater physical strength and more coalitional support.
io
WTRs set primarily by the threat of force will be consulted, presumably, only
at
when there is some possibility that the individual will be present to defend
ci
his or her interests. This is entirely analogous to the relative formidability that
so
is known to affect conflicts of interest in nonhuman animals. Another set of
As
factors that set WTRs is related to the ability to defend one’s welfare by
threatening to withdraw the benefits of cooperation. These include, for exam-
al
ple, the person’s status as a frequent and dependable reciprocation partner,
ic
his or her status as a friend who has a stake in one’s welfare (Tooby & Cosmides,
og
1996), or his or her possession of special abilities that can be deployed to
benefit others. ol
ch
There are at least two kinds of WTRs that govern cost–benefit transac-
y
tions in different contexts: (a) monitored WTRs, which define the threshold of
Ps
unable to defend his or her interests. Presumably, intrinsic WTRs allowed indi-
er
the scope of this chapter, a quick starting point would be as follows: A subset of
th
individuals in one’s social world can improve one’s welfare as a result of their
by
58 AARON SELL
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 59
A)
to the target and to the angered individual and (b) raise the target’s WTR
toward the angered individual, so the target takes that individual’s welfare
P
(A
more into account in the present, the future, or both. The two main negotiation
strategies deployed to recalibrate the target’s WTR are (a) threatening to
n
inflict costs (or actually doing so) and (b) threatening to withdraw cooperation
io
(or actually doing so).
at
ci
Causes of Human Anger
so
As
Anger should be triggered when one individual interprets the actions of
the target as indicating that the target’s WTR toward the individual is lower
al
than the accepted level. WTRs are theorized to be used when making cost–
ic
benefit decisions—indeed, that is their hypothesized function—but they may
og
also be used by other cognitive programs, such as memory storage related to
ol
particular individuals (e.g., we may remember our niece’s birthday but not our
ch
mailman’s birthday), frequency of consideration (e.g., one thinks daily about
y
a spouse but may forget about old friends for weeks at a time), inherent pleasure
Ps
toward friends of friends. Because WTRs are used by so many cognitive systems,
ic
ing anger, each with its own blend of behavioral responses dependent on how
Am
ingness to take actions that impose a large cost on the angry individual in
th
order for the target to receive a small benefit. Holding other variables con-
by
result of that cost decreases, and (c) characteristics of the instigator and the
20
angry individual indicate that it is possible for the angry individual to force
the other to use a higher WTR than was indicated by the cost–benefit trans-
©
action imposed.
ht
ig
The primary functions of anger are to raise the magnitude of the WTR
of an individual who has demonstrated a lower WTR than is acceptable to
C
magnitude of the costs imposed and benefits received. WTRs in the target
should be open to modification when this will allow the target to avoid
being harmed or having cooperation withdrawn—more precisely, in cir-
cumstances that predicted ancestrally that these two negative outcomes
A)
were likely.
Thus, when the anger system is triggered by evidence that the target’s
P
(A
WTR toward the angry individual is too low, it should motivate him or her
to make credible threats or demonstrate qualities that would make such
n
threats credible, if issued. WTRs are hypothesized to be set partly by relative
io
formidability, as is the case with many other animals. Thus if an individual is
at
showing evidence of a low WTR, it could be the result of an underestimation
ci
of one’s willingness or ability to use force and could be recalibrated by a
so
demonstration of said force. As with nonhuman animals, formidability should
As
be demonstrated starting with low-cost, presumably less accurate, demonstra-
tions of physical strength and escalated as needed to more accurate and
al
dangerous demonstrations of strength.
ic
The theory also predicts that anger should be designed to manipulate
og
the target’s estimates of the magnitude of costs and benefits inherent in the
ol
transaction. To the extent that you can increase another’s perception of a cost
ch
he or she imposed on you, you can decrease the probability that the individual
y
will impose such a cost on you again. The same is true of reducing another’s
Ps
Given the breadth of data collected on human anger, the first step when
proposing a new theory must be to determine its consistency with empirical
e
findings that have been shown to be both large in effect and robust across
th
studies. It should be noted that although I did my best to choose the following
by
data sets on the basis of their effect sizes, reliability, and cross-cultural docu-
mentation, I am not providing a complete review of the anger literature.
11
20
Individuals tend to get angry when costs are imposed on them. Most
ht
the magnitude of the cost. Empirical studies that varied the magnitude of the
yr
cost have confirmed this relationship across a host of different cost types, such
op
60 AARON SELL
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 61
A)
(see Epstein & Taylor, 1967). Theories that posit intentionality have used
different definitions. For example, Heider (1958) defined intention as a plan
P
(A
that guides action, Kaufmann (1970) defined an aggressive action as one that is
known by the actor to have a nonzero chance of inflicting harm on the target,
n
and Tedeschi and Felson (1994) defined an intentional action as one “performed
io
with the expectation that it will produce a proximate outcome of value to the
at
actor” (p. 164).
ci
Intentionality, not being directly observable, is a category the human
so
mind uses to classify types of actions and thus must be discovered and explored
As
rather than defined as a given, objective feature of the world. A theory of
intentionality will have to specify, at a minimum, what information must be
al
known for something to qualify as intentional. For example, if someone plans
ic
to hit you with a toy ball and expects that you will enjoy this as part of a
og
game but you become angry when hit because you did not want to play, should
ol
the person’s act be viewed as intentional? Heider’s (1958) definition cannot
ch
answer this question. Kaufmann’s (1970) definition would result in the act
y
being judged as not intentional. Tedeschi and Felson’s (1994) definition would
Ps
The most reliable way to reduce anger, according to the empirical liter-
Am
tinguishing “real” from “false” apologies and discovering why angry individu-
th
Violent and homicidal aggressive acts are most common among young
©
men across cultures and time periods (Daly & Wilson, 1988). These acts are
ht
largely the result of insults and attempts to save face or attain status by fighting.
ig
This account of violent aggression among males has been noted, to some extent
yr
(Berg & Fox, 1947; see also Chapter 10, this volume; Felson, 1982), and evo-
lutionary psychologists (Daly & Wilson, 1988). Most impressively, a host of
C
A)
ican gangs (Toch, 1969). In each of those societies, threats to one’s face or
status are often the trigger for violent episodes between young men.
P
(A
Feature 5: Personal Insults Are One of the Most Reliable Causes of Anger
n
io
Though not usually the object of study, personal insults have been used
at
in aggression research for 40 years and have (in most cases) been shown to be
ci
sufficient causes of anger (Geen, 1968). In nonlaboratory cases of aggression, it
so
has been found that personal insults almost always precede homicides (Berg &
As
Fox, 1947; Luckenbill, 1977; Toch, 1969) and assaults (Felson, 1982).
al
Feature 6: Anger Results in an Exchange of Argument
ic
og
The most common response to an anger-inducing event in naturally
ol
occurring situations is to engage in an argument. Averill (1982) established
ch
this fact with an influential study of a large sample of adults.
y
Ps
ing cultures with no historical contact with the West (Ekman, 1973). It has
Am
logical changes accompanying anger have been found to be similar across cul-
th
tures (Rime & Giovannini, 1986). Subjects from a broad European sample
by
reported that anger felt unpleasant and warm and was frequently associated
with muscular tension. These response patterns were different from those of
11
other emotions.
20
Behavioral responses that result from anger are also similar across European
countries. Specifically, anger often leads to vocal changes involving increased
©
ity, clenched fists, and increased hand movement (Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson,
ig
62 AARON SELL
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 63
A)
Finally, there have been numerous studies of parts of the brain that are
differentially activated by anger, showing that its neural underpinnings are
P
(A
similar across individuals and species and that they are distinct from those of
other emotions (see also Chapter 6). Panksepp (2000) theorized, based on
n
brain imaging and lesion studies, that anger relies mostly on the medial amyg-
io
dala, bed nucleus of stria terminalis, and the medial and perifornical hypo-
at
thalamus. The anger/rage system is moderated primarily by acetylcholine and
ci
glutamate in ways that connect the amygdala and periaqueductal gray with
so
the hypothalamus (Siegel & Schubert, 1995). Finally, a recent meta-analysis
As
confirmed that testosterone tracks individual differences in tendencies toward
anger and aggression with an effect size of .4 (Cohen’s d; Archer, 2006; see
al
also Chapter 2, this volume).
ic
og
ol
HOW DOES THE RECALIBRATIONAL THEORY ACCOUNT
ch
FOR FEATURES OF ANGER?
y
Ps
The recalibrational theory accounts well for the major features of anger
and provides testable predictions that promise to more fully elaborate the
an
The recalibrational theory predicts that the cause of anger is not nega-
th
tive affect but the indication that another person holds a low WTR with
by
magnitude of the benefit constant, the larger the cost one is willing to impose,
20
the more likely anger is to be triggered. Likewise, holding the cost constant
and the more the other person benefits by imposing that cost, the less angry
©
Feature 2: Intentionality
yr
op
A)
Feature 3: Apologies
P
(A
The recalibrational theory suggests that apologies are explicit acknowl-
n
edgements of either (a) a past discrepant WTR that has been recalibrated
io
upward or (b) a misperception or accident on behalf of the target of anger that
at
led them to commit an act that does not reflect their true WTR toward the
ci
angry individual. The contents of WTR-recalibrated apologies (Type 1) are
so
predicted to contain statements that translate into the following cognitive
As
grammar: “I will demonstrate a more favorable welfare tradeoff ratio with
respect to you, such that I will no longer impose costs of that magnitude on
al
you for benefits of that magnitude.” This kind of claim may be best validated
ic
by restitution or by indicating a willingness to incur a cost to repay the angry
og
individual. The content of Type 2 apologies should contain statements about
ol
the magnitude of perceived costs and benefits; for example, “I didn’t realize
ch
that would hurt you so badly; I thought I had a good reason for doing that, but
y
I was wrong.”
Ps
ularly for males (Sell et al., 2009; Sell, Tooby, & Cosmides, 2009). As such,
Am
with low levels of violence (e.g., pushing contests, staring contests) and move
by
Feature 5: Insults
©
ht
64 AARON SELL
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 65
determine one’s WTR toward that individual. One such variable, for males
at least, is physical strength (Sell, Tooby, & Cosmides, 2009). Fitting the
theory, many insults applied disproportionately to men target insufficient
masculinity or strength (e.g., wimp, wuss, nerd, girly-man, pussy, weakling;
A)
Harris, 1993; Preston & Stanley, 1987). Although beyond the scope of this
analysis, it seems likely that other insults fit into categories that make a man
P
(A
socially powerful or not, such as his intent to cooperate (e.g., asshole, prick,
bastard [colloquial meaning]), his being unable or unwilling to function as a
n
reliable cooperator or being otherwise unworthy of having others take his
io
interests into account (e.g., punk, white trash, bum), or his competence (e.g.,
at
idiot, fool, loser).
ci
Insulting beliefs are a kind of implied insult that is often mistakenly
so
revealed. For example, imagine a professor who offers to help her student with
As
a simple math problem. The student may be angry and insulted because he
believes the professor thinks he is stupid. This is a different kind of insult, but
al
it causes anger because it reveals that a trait used to set WTRs (in this case
ic
intelligence) is being underestimated and presumably results in the professor
og
holding a lower WTR toward the student.
ol
ch
Feature 6: Arguments
y
Ps
the angered person’s welfare more fully into account; and (b) to recalibrate
ic
When WTRs are largely based on relative fighting ability, one would predict
Am
arguments about aggressive potential, for example, “I could kick your ass” or
“My dad could beat up your dad!” When one has an expectation of inherent
e
value (e.g., with friends, kin, and others with whom long-term cooperation
th
mother was sick and I took notes for you in all your classes.”
20
tudes of the values involved in the cost–benefit exchange (e.g., “Why did you
ht
do that?”); the offender’s knowledge of the magnitude of the cost, benefit, and
ig
victim identity (e.g., “Do you know how much that hurt?” “Do you know who
yr
you’re messing with?”); the offender’s perception of his or her WTR with
op
respect to you (e.g., “I thought we were friends”); and other variables that are
used to set WTRs; and (b) testing the boundaries of one’s WTR with respect
C
Feature 7: Universality
A)
basic computational structure should be universal across cultures and should
share a phylogenetic relationship with structures in closely related nonhuman
P
(A
animals, residing in similarly localized brain areas. Furthermore, like nonhuman
animals, humans should use signals of aggressive intent based on enhanced
n
features of formidability (e.g., facial and vocal expressions), physiological pre-
io
paredness for aggression if necessary, and a structured functional set of causes
at
and behavioral responses that are similar across cultures.
ci
so
As
CONCLUSION: HOW TO ACCOUNT FOR COMPLEX DESIGN
al
Social science has had difficulty accounting for complex features of
ic
human behavior largely because it has ignored the one known cause of com-
og
plex functional design in organisms: natural selection (Tooby & Cosmides,
ol
1992). Without functional theories capable of generating testable predictions
ch
about many different aspects of the domain of study, researchers have had two
y
choices: (a) retreat into smaller data sets that can be predicted and cogently
Ps
specific but restricted to smaller data sets, are computationally specific, func-
Am
tional theories that posit many testable hypotheses based on a simple model of
selection pressures and the logical extensions of them given what is known
e
about human evolutionary history and the design of other animals. Natural
th
organisms. It is the only process that could have designed anger, and thus any
functional design in the anger system (including facial expressions, vocaliza-
11
anger is the result of learning, natural selection is the only process that could
ht
The selection pressures described in this chapter may or may not be the
yr
ones most responsible for the functional structure of human anger, although
op
they have had great early success in making predictions and explaining many
of the most reliable and significant features of anger. Ultimately, the recali-
C
66 AARON SELL
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 67
A)
evolutionary biology. The data set on anger is now too large to be described
by simple learning mechanisms and too detailed to be accounted for by theories
P
(A
that do not computationally specify their primary components.
n
io
REFERENCES
at
ci
Archer, J. (1988). The behavioral biology of aggression. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
so
University Press.
As
Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: An evaluation of the challenge
hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 319–345. doi:10.1016/
al
j.neubiorev.2004.12.007
ic
Austad, S. (1983). A game theoretical interpretation of male combat in the bowl and
og
doily spider. Animal Behaviour, 31, 59–73. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80173-0
ol
Averill, J. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York, NY: Springer-
ch
Verlag.
y
Balikci, A. (1970). The Netsilik Eskimo. Garden City, NY: The Natural History Press.
Ps
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 614–636. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614
an
Berg, I., & Fox, V. (1947). Factors in homicides committed by 200 males. Journal of
ic
of the public’s view. Law & Society Review, 14, 223–261. doi:10.2307/3053313
Boehm, C. (1984). Blood revenge: The anthropology of feuding in Montenegro and other
e
Brace, R., & Pavey, J. (1978). Size-dependent dominance hierarchy in the anemone
by
Clutton-Brock, T., & Albon, S. (1979). The roaring of red deer and the evolution of
honest advertisement. Behaviour, 69, 145–170. doi:10.1163/156853979X00449
©
80077-6
yr
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
op
Enquist, M., & Leimar, O. (1983). Evolution of fighting behaviour: Decision rules
and assessment of relative strength. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 102, 387–410.
doi:10.1016/0022-5193(83)90376-4
Enquist, M., Leimar, O., Ljungberg, T., Mallner, Y., & Segerdahl, N. (1990). A test
A)
of the sequential assessment game: Fighting in the cichlid fish Nannacara anomala.
Animal Behaviour, 40, 1–14. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80660-8
P
(A
Epstein, S., & Taylor, S. (1967). Instigation to aggression as a function of degree of
defeat and perceived aggressive intent of the opponent. Journal of Personality,
n
35, 265–-289.
io
Fehr, E., & Gaechter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experi-
at
ments. The American Economic Review, 90, 980–994.
ci
so
Felson, R. B. (1982). Impression management and the escalation of aggression and
violence. Social Psychology Quarterly, 45, 245–254. doi:10.2307/3033920
As
Galati, D., Sini, B., Schmidt, S., & Tinti, C. (2003). Spontaneous facial expressions
al
in congenitally blind and sighted children aged 8–11. Journal of Visual Impairment
ic
& Blindness, 97, 418–428.
og
Geen, R. (1968). Effects of frustration, attack and prior training in aggressiveness
upon aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 316–321.
doi:10.1037/h0026054
ol
ch
Guhl, A. M. (1956, February). The social order of chickens. Scientific American,
y
Ps
194(2), 42–46.
Hammerstein, P., & Parker, G. A. (1982). The asymmetric war of attrition. Journal
an
Hansen, A. J. (1986). Fighting behavior in bald eagles: A test of game theory. Ecology,
er
Harris, M. B. (1993). How provoking: What makes men and women angry? Aggres-
sive Behavior, 19, 199–211. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1993)19:3<199::AID-
AB2480190305>3.0.CO;2-D
e
th
doi:10.1163/156854075X00801
11
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, NY: Wiley.
doi:10.1037/10628-000
20
Holtgraves, T. (1989). The form and function of remedial moves: Reported use,
©
Huntingford, F. A., & Turner, A. K. (1987). Animal conflict. New York, NY: Chapman
yr
& Hall.
op
Jensen, P., & Yngvesson, J. (1998). Aggression between unacquainted pigs: Sequen-
tial assessment and effects of familiarity and weight. Applied Animal Behaviour
C
68 AARON SELL
12051-04_CH03-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 69
A)
Luckenbill, D. (1977). Criminal homicide as a situated transaction. Social Problems,
P
25, 176–186. doi:10.1525/sp.1977.25.2.03a00050
(A
Maynard Smith, J., & Parker, G. A. (1976). The logic of asymmetric contests. Animal
Behaviour, 24, 159–175. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(76)80110-8
n
io
Nowlis, V. (1941). The relation of degree of hunger to competitive interaction in chim-
at
panzee. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 32, 91–115. doi:10.1037/h0063065
ci
O’Leary, M., & Dengerink, H. (1973). Aggression as a function of the intensity and
so
pattern of attack. Journal of Research in Personality, 7, 61–70. doi:10.1016/0092-
6566(73)90032-9
As
Owens, D., & Owens, M. (1996). Social dominance and reproductive patterns in
al
brown hyaenas, Hyaena brunnea, of the central Kalahari Desert. Animal Behaviour,
ic
51, 535–551. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0058
og
Panksepp, J. (2000). Emotions as natural kinds within the mammalian brain. In
ol
M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 137–156).
ch
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Petrie, M. (1984). Territory size in the moorhen (Gallinula chloropus): An outcome of
y
Ps
Preston, K., & Stanley, K. (1987). “What’s the worst thing . . . ?”: Gender-directed
ic
Rime, B., & Giovannini, D. (1986). The physiological patterns of reported emotional
Am
Riordan, C., Marlin, N., & Kellogg, R. (1983). The effectiveness of accounts following
transgression. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46, 213–219. doi:10.2307/3033792
by
Rohwer, S., & Rohwer, F. (1978). Status signaling in Harris sparrows: Experimental
11
Sargent, W. (1974). People of the valley. New York, NY: Random House.
©
Sell, A., Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., Sznycer, D., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2009).
op
Human adaptations for the visual assessment of strength and fighting ability
from the body and face. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. Biological Sciences, 276,
C
575–584. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1177
Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the logic of human
anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106(35), 15073–15078.
Shaver, P. R., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge:
Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social
A)
Psychology, 52, 1061–1086. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1061
Siegel, A., & Schubert, K. (1995). Neurotransmitters regulating feline aggressive
P
(A
behavior. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 6, 47–61.
Sinclair, A. (1977). The African buffalo. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
n
io
Smuts, B., Cheney, D., Seyfarth, R., Wrangham, R., & Struhsaker, T. (1987). Primate
at
societies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
ci
Stenberg, C. R., & Campos, J. J. (1990). The development of anger expression in
so
infancy. In N. Stein, B. Leventhal, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Psychological and bio-
logical approaches to emotion (pp. 247–282). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
As
Taylor, S. P. (1967). Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of
al
provocation and tendency to inhibit aggression. Journal of Personality, 35, 297–310.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1967.tb01430.x
ic
og
Tedeschi, J., & Felson, R. (1994). Violence, aggression, and coercive actions. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10160-000
Toch, H. (1969). Violent men. Chicago, IL: Aldine.
ol
ch
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In
y
Ps
University Press.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1996). Friendship and the banker’s paradox: Other pathways
ic
to the evolution of adaptations for altruism. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88,
er
119–143.
Am
Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., Sell, A., Liberman, D., & Sznycer, D. (2008). Internal reg-
ulatory variables and the design of human motivation: A computational and
e
University Press.
op
C
70 AARON SELL
12051-05_CH04-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 71
PA)
A BEHAVIORAL SYSTEMS
(A
PERSPECTIVE ON POWER
n
io
AND AGGRESSION
at
ci
so
PHILLIP R. SHAVER, MICHAL SEGEV, AND MARIO MIKULINCER
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
research (Cassidy & Shaver, 2008). Beginning with a focus on the importance
Am
ing developmental, social, and clinical psychology. The theory has been suc-
by
cessful partly because Bowlby retained some of the most valuable contributions
of prior psychoanalytic theorists (e.g., unconscious mental processes, self-
11
For various reasons, Bowlby (1982) wanted to move away from Freud’s
op
71
12051-05_CH04-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 72
was struck early in his career by the number of his patients who had been sex-
ually abused—or, as he called it, “seduced”—by adults). It also seemed that
infant anger was a reaction to unreliable or frightening parental behavior, not a
primary motivational force in its own right (see Chapters 13 and 14, this volume).
A)
In other respects, Bowlby’s emphasis on what he called behavioral systems—that
is, functional patterns of motivated behavior that evolved over evolutionary
P
time because they contributed to human survival and successful reproduction—
(A
was laudable. It has been one of his ideas that has encouraged behavioral mea-
n
surement of attachment-related phenomena (e.g., Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters,
io
& Wall, 1978), something that earlier psychoanalytic theories did not do. The
at
purpose of the present chapter is to begin exploring the possibility that it would
ci
be worthwhile to supplement attachment theory with a hypothesized behavioral
so
system concerned with power or assertive influence. When this system goes
As
awry, one result might be dysfunctional anger and aggression.
In the following section of the chapter, we briefly explain the behavioral
al
system construct and describe some of the behavioral systems that Bowlby
ic
(1982) proposed (e.g., attachment, exploration, caregiving). We then propose
og
a power or assertion behavioral system and show that individual differences in
ol
the functioning of this system can be measured with self-report scales such as
ch
the ones we have used in the past to measure attachment and caregiving orien-
y
present preliminary evidence concerning the extent to which the new power
Am
universal neural program that governs the choice, activation, and termination
ig
Each system was viewed as having a major aim or goal, for example,
attaining a sense of safety and security, curiously exploring and learning about
one’s environment, and promoting others’ (especially loved ones’) safety and
welfare. Each system was thought to include a repertoire of interchangeable,
A)
functionally equivalent behaviors that constitute the primary strategy used by
the system to attain its goal (e.g., maintaining proximity to a protective attach-
P
(A
ment figure in times of need). These sets of behaviors or behavioral tendencies
were thought to be “activated” automatically by stimuli or situations that made
n
a particular goal salient (e.g., loud noises that signaled danger and aroused a
io
need for protection) and were “deactivated” or “terminated” by other stimuli
at
that signaled goal attainment.
ci
Inasmuch as each behavioral system presumably evolved because it
so
increased the likelihood of coping successfully with environmental demands,
As
it is easy to understand why its optimal functioning in today’s human beings
is important for mental health, social adjustment, and a satisfying life.
al
Consider, for example, the attachment behavioral system. It is activated by per-
ic
ceived threats and dangers, which cause a person to seek proximity to another
og
person who is viewed as a “safe haven” and “secure base” (Bowlby, 1982).
ol
Successfully attaining proximity, protection, and emotional comfort from
ch
such an “attachment figure” results in what Sroufe and Waters (1977) called
y
Bowlby (1982) believed that the strategies associated with each behav-
ioral system undergo experience-based development. People learn to alter the
e
demands, and in the process they form reliable expectations about available
by
access routes and likely barriers to goal attainment. These expectations, which
Bowlby (1973) called internal working models, become part of a behavioral sys-
11
tem’s neural wiring; their systematic and prolonged effects are observable as
20
Cassidy & Kobak, 1988; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Hyperactivating strate-
yr
gies intensify the primary strategy of the system to influence other people
op
and intensely activated until its goal is achieved. They are learned in social
A)
accompanied by heightened agitation and distress, which often upsets inter-
action partners by seeming overly intrusive, demanding, and controlling.
P
(A
In contrast, deactivating strategies involve down-regulation of a behavioral
system to reduce the frustration and anguish of repeatedly unsuccessful efforts
n
to attain the system’s goal. These strategies develop in the presence of people
io
who disapprove of or punish the system’s primary strategy (e.g., crying, reach-
at
ing, clinging). This disapproval or punishment suggests that one can expect bet-
ci
ter outcomes if the primary strategy of a particular behavioral system is blocked
so
or suppressed, which unfortunately means that the system’s goal is not often
As
fully attained. The problem with deactivating strategies is that they require a
narrowing of experience and the relative absence of many of life’s rewards (e.g.,
al
shared intimacy). By suppressing what would, in other circumstances, be nor-
ic
mal behavior, deactivating strategies prevent a person from realizing that there
og
are other social relationships or social environments in which the system’s pri-
mary strategy would be effective. ol
y ch
Ps
ing system (both of which were proposed by Bowlby, 1982) can be measured
and that they are associated in theoretically predicted ways with other psy-
e
speculatively, that human beings are born with the rudiments of a behavioral
by
system the aim of which is to acquire and control material and social resources
(e.g., food, shelter, social status, sexual mates) that contribute to survival and
11
we will call the power system. This system presumably evolved because it con-
ht
depends on his or her ability to acquire and control precious resources and to
op
cope effectively with people and events that threaten resource control (see
Chapter 3, this volume).
C
A)
finite and people compete to acquire and control them, this definition implies
that people who have a sense of power also have control over others’ access to
P
(A
resources and can influence their behavior (see Chapter 3, this volume). This
reasoning led Keltner et al. (2003) to define power as “an individual’s relative
n
capacity to modify others’ states by providing or withholding resources or
io
administering punishments” (p. 265). Another implication of this definition
at
is that power often involves freedom and independence from others’ influence
ci
when seeking desired resources. Galinsky, Magee, Gruenfeld, Whitson, and
so
Liljenquist (2008) stated, “Power, it could also be said, is the capacity to be
As
uninfluenced by others. Without power, one’s outcomes are constrained by
others” (p. 1451).
al
Before conceptualizing the normative components and operations of the
ic
power behavioral system, we should distinguish power from aggression. From
og
an evolutionary standpoint, human aggression is a “fight” mechanism (e.g., Buss
ol
& Shackelford, 1997). It presumably evolved in many animal species because
ch
it facilitates control over precious resources, which makes it natural to equate
y
aggression with power. We emphasize, however, that there definitely are aggres-
Ps
sive acts whose sole purpose is to damage or destroy someone or something else,
and there are many cases in which one’s sense of power can be restored simply
an
would like to consider the possibility that the power system is not “designed”
er
primarily to attack and destroy, but to gain, maintain, or restore one’s sense of
Am
We propose that the main goal of the power system is to remove threats
and obstacles that interfere with a person’s sense of power. In other words, the
11
power system seeks to maintain a stable inner sense of power and to restore
20
this sense when one perceives that others are attempting to constrain one’s
access to valuable resources or influence one’s behavior in a particular situa-
©
tion. This does not imply that people seek power simply for power’s sake.
ht
exploration, affiliation, caregiving, and sex. With a sense of power, people can
more easily explore and master their environment; help and get along with
C
A)
of two kinds of situations: (a) when a person competes for access to valu-
able resources and (b) when other people constrain one’s access to resources
P
(A
or attempt to influence one’s attitudes and actions. In either case, people
are motivated to protect or restore their sense of power when they appraise
n
an event or social interaction as a threat to their power, not when they sim-
io
ply encounter someone who has a certain objective status or acts in a par-
at
ticular way. That is, the power system is not typically activated if a person
ci
detects no threat to his or her sense of power. By the same token, a person
so
can inappropriately appraise something as a threat even in the absence of
As
another person’s explicit signaling of competition, provocation, or superi-
ority (see Chapter 9).
al
Once a person’s power system is activated (appropriately or not), he or
ic
she calls on a repertoire of behaviors aimed at protecting or restoring a sense
og
of power. This repertoire, which reflects the system’s primary strategy, includes
ol
behaviors meant to maintain what Parker (1974) called resource-holding power,
ch
behaviors such as asserting one’s dominance, authority, and competence to
y
deal with the situation; expressing confidence in one’s strengths, attitudes, and
Ps
opinions; deterring others from competing for or exerting control over one’s
resources; and verbally or physically attacking (or threatening to attack)
an
others until power is restored (e.g., Gilbert, 1989; see also Chapter 3, this vol-
ic
ume). Beyond these basic strategies that can be observed in most animal
er
species, Gilbert (1989) also proposed that humans can protect or restore their
Am
taken to reduce or eliminate the threat, repair the damage, or prevent fur-
yr
ther assaults. In the second volume of his Attachment and Loss trilogy,
op
tion of power, these signs indicate that one lacks the power to obtain
needed resources, such as affection and support, and that one is dependent
on others’ unreliable responsiveness.
When the power system, like other behavioral systems, works properly,
it contributes greatly to one’s subjective well-being and social adaptation
A)
(Keltner et al., 2003). Moreover, it encourages what Higgins (1998) called a
promotion focus—a motivational orientation that facilitates goal pursuit and
P
(A
realization of aspirations—because powerful people expect positive outcomes
from their efforts and relatively little interference from others. Research
n
shows that people with a sense of power devote attention to rewards and goal
io
pursuit, have more frequent positive emotions, and experience fewer threat-
at
related thoughts and emotions (for reviews, see Keltner et al., 2003; Chapter
ci
13, this volume).
so
As
Individual Differences in the Activation and Functioning
of the Power System
al
ic
Although we assume that everyone is born with the potential to develop
og
a stable sense of power, the functioning of the power system can be impaired by
ol
experiencing repeated failures to obtain desired outcomes, remove threats, and
ch
overcome obstacles. Such failures may result from physical illnesses that pre-
y
vent the effective use of the power system’s primary strategies. They may also
Ps
serious doubts about one’s power and influence, and a loss of confidence in one’s
Am
is fueled by two motives: an excessive urge to gain power and an extreme fear
of failure in the use of resource-holding power strategies. This combination
©
assert power over others; frequent anger and hostility toward others (who are
yr
A)
mary strategies to defend against threats and damages to one’s sense of power.
Deactivation is evident in submissiveness, self-abasement, and the absence of
P
(A
resource-holding power strategies, even in the presence of clear-cut, explicit
assault or provocation, to the point of experiencing substantial physical or
n
psychological harm as a result. Deactivation also involves a tendency to avoid
io
situations that call for activation of the power system and assertion of one’s
at
rights and opinions: competitions, arguments, disputes, and interpersonal
ci
conflicts. It is important to note, however, that such deactivation does not
so
necessarily involve reduced sensitivity to threats. In fact, powerless people are
As
often highly sensitive to threat-related cues, prone to ruminate about threats,
and experience negative emotions because of perceived threats and injustices
al
(for a review, see Keltner et al., 2003). In other words, deactivation is not a
ic
peaceful or calm state; it is characterized by a blend of worries, doubts, and
og
defenses against the pain and frustration of “losing” or dodging a “fight.”
ol
A review of self-report measures of beliefs, attitudes, and feelings related
ch
to power and aggression led us to conclude that no instrument had been
y
and deactivation. We did gain insights from examining existing scales, how-
ever. We concluded, for example, that Buss and Perry’s (1992) Aggression
an
not distinguish people with an optimally functioning power system from those
who deactivate their power system, because neither kind of person typically
e
who hyperactivate the system, because both groups are capable of asserting
20
Gough, 1964), but they fail to distinguish between people who hyperactivate
ht
or deactivate their power systems because both groups experience doubts and
ig
worries about the extent to which they have power over resources. One measure
yr
that taps constructs similar to the ones we are proposing here is the Inventory of
op
autocratic (e.g., “I try to control other people too much”) and overly competitive
(e.g., “I fight with other people too much”)—and two subscales that describe
forms of deactivation—overly subassertive (e.g., “It is hard for me to be assertive
with another person”) and overly exploitable (e.g., “I let other people take advan-
tage of me too much”). Despite these useful near-approximations to the mea-
A)
sures we were seeking, we decided to create a new measure that, like the
Caregiving System Scale (Shaver et al., 2010) and the Experiences in Close
P
(A
Relationships Inventory (ECR; Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998) that is used
to measure insecure attachment, would be specifically designed to assess hyper-
n
activating and deactivating power strategies. (These scales are all part of an
io
overall effort to create a theory and set of measures to cover all major human
at
behavioral systems related to social behavior.) Here, we present data from the
ci
first stage of this research program: the construction of a self-report Power
so
Behavioral System Scale (PBSS) to assess individual differences in hyper-
As
activation and deactivation of the power system.
al
ic
ASSESSING HYPERACTIVATION AND DEACTIVATION
og
OF THE POWER SYSTEM
ol
ch
In the first stage of scale development, we constructed a pool of 50 items
y
that might index the two secondary power-system strategies. In writing the
Ps
described in the present chapter. For example, the 25 items designed to assess
ic
hyperactivation focused on the urgent and exaggerated need for power and
er
control over resources and other people, frequent bouts of anger and aggres-
Am
sion, and anxieties and worries about being defeated in competitions and dis-
putes. The 25 items designed to assess deactivation of the system focused on
e
the extent to which each item was or was not self-descriptive. Hence, the PBSS
20
measures a person’s general, overall orientation to power rather than the exer-
tion of power in a particular situation or relationship, although the items could
©
Factor Structure
op
undergraduates (185 women and 88 men). Item and factor analyses indicated
that the items did assess the two secondary power-system strategies: hyper-
activating and deactivating. Based on these analyses, we chose the 14 most
representative items from each factor (i.e., the ones that loaded highest
on the intended factor and lowest on the other factor), keeping in mind
A)
our goal of representing various aspects of hyperactivation and deactiva-
tion. We then administered the new 28-item scale to 362 Israeli under-
P
(A
graduates (211 women and 151 men) and conducted a new factor analysis.
As expected, the analysis yielded the intended two factors, which accounted
n
for 52% of the variance. The 14 deactivation items loaded higher than .40
io
on the first factor (28% of explained variance). Here are examples of these
at
items: “I tend to relinquish important goals if their attainment requires
ci
confronting other people”; “I tend to avoid attacking, even if it’s a matter
so
of self-defense”; “I’d rather let others win an argument, even when I know
As
I’m right”; and “I’d rather not show people I’m angry, even when my anger
is justified.” The 14 hyperactivation items loaded higher than .40 on the
al
second factor (24%). The following are examples: “I feel anxious in situa-
ic
tions where I have little control over other people and their actions”;
og
“In an argument or disagreement, my strong desire to fight back makes it
ol
difficult for me to consider other possible responses”; “It’s hard for me to
ch
stop arguing, even when the other person has conceded”; and “When
y
alphas were .85 for the hyperactivation items and .90 for deactivation items.
(Similar results were obtained in a replication study conducted in English
an
vation scores was not statistically significant, r(360) = .07. That is, hyper-
Am
activation and deactivation are orthogonal strategies, and the two scales
form a two-dimensional space in which different power orientations can be
e
represented.
th
by
tives, friends, or romantic partners of each participant to use the PBSS items
ig
tion dimensions, with rs ranging from .46 to .54. These findings imply that
the PBSS measures, in part, behavioral tendencies that can be observed by
C
relationship partners.
Convergent Validity
A)
First, we examined associations between the PBSS and preexisting self-
report measures tapping various aspects of aggression: the Aggression Ques-
P
(A
tionnaire (Buss & Perry, 1992), a measure of violence risk (Plutchik & Van
Praag, 1990), and the Abuse within Intimate Relationships Scale (Borjesson,
n
Aarons, & Dunn, 2003). The hyperactivation score was associated with
io
reports of physical aggression, verbal aggression, anger, and hostility; risk
at
of violent behavior; and abusive behavior in intimate relationship, with rs
ci
ranging from .27 to .46, all ps < .01. The deactivation score was not signif-
so
icantly associated with these measures. As expected, aggressive, violent,
As
and abusive behaviors can be viewed as manifestations of hyperactivated
power, but they do not differentiate between people scoring low or high on
al
the deactivation dimension. Stated differently, aggression and violence are
ic
not default strategies for gaining power but seem to develop from repeated
og
failure to control resources that eventually results in hyperactivation of the
power system. ol
ch
Second, we examined associations between the PBSS and preexisting
y
Anger Expression Scale (Spielberger, Jacobs, Russell, & Crane, 1983), the
Trait subscale of the State–Trait Anger Scale (Spielberger, 1983), the Multi-
an
Scale (Sukhodolsky, Golub, & Cronwell, 2001). In line with our theoretical
er
ps < .01. The deactivation score was not significantly associated with most of
th
these signs of anger, with the exception of a positive association with anger
internalization, r(176) = .37, p < .05. This finding suggests that an angry state
by
of mind is still active despite deactivation of the power system and that anger-
11
related feelings are directed toward the self rather than other people.
20
Abasement scales of the Personality Research Form (Jackson, 1984), and the
ht
tion and deactivation were associated with lower scores on scales measuring
feelings of dominance and power (rs ranging from −.33 to −.42, all ps < .01),
op
implying that these orientations may be alternative ways of coping with lack of
C
A)
Fourth, we examined associations between the PBSS and preexisting
self-report measures of interpersonal conflicts and reactions to others’
P
(A
transgressions: the Styles of Handling Interpersonal Conflict Scale (Rahim,
1983) and the Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motivations Inventory
n
(McCullough & Hoyt, 2002). As expected, hyperactivation was associated
io
with more aggressive and conflict-escalating behavior during conflicts and
at
greater vengeance and less forgiveness following others’ transgressions, with
ci
rs ranging from .29 to .51, all ps < .01. Correlations for the deactivation scale
so
were also compatible with our theoretical analysis: Deactivation was associ-
As
ated with avoidance and giving up during interpersonal conflicts and a ten-
dency to withdraw in response to interpersonal transgressions, with rs ranging
al
from .22 to .56, all ps < .01.
ic
og
Discriminant and Construct Validity
ol
ch
In the eight participant samples we also considered the discriminant
y
and construct validity of the PBSS scores. Participants completed the Crowne-
Ps
Marlowe Social Desirability Scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964), the Big
Five Inventory (John & Srivastava, 1999), and the ECR (Brennan et al.,
an
1998). The two PBSS scores were not associated with social desirability, rs
ic
< .05, but they were correlated in theoretically expected ways with other
er
ing from .26 to .54, p < .01. Deactivation was significantly associated with
th
only moderate in size, suggesting that the PBSS scores are not simply redun-
11
dant with attachment insecurities or with broad personality traits. That is,
20
we believe that the PBSS measures something unique to the power system
that is not measured precisely by the other scales.
©
but agreeable, those who hyperactivate the system tend to be quarrelsome and
ig
tional instability (i.e., neuroticism) and worries about being loved, accepted,
and esteemed by others (i.e., anxious attachment). That is, even the external
C
A)
In the eight samples, we also examined the extent to which hyper- and
deactivated forms of power are associated with regulatory deficits and social
P
(A
skill deficits. Power hyperactivation was associated with deficits in emotion-
regulation, self-regulation, and social skills. The deficits were reflected in lower
n
scores on scales measuring self-control (Tangney, Baumeister, & Boone, 2004),
io
negative mood regulation (Catanzaro & Mearns, 1990), and social skills
at
(Buhrmester, Furman, Wittenberg, & Reis, 1988), with rs ranging from −.38
ci
to −.55, all ps < .01. As expected, hyperactivation was also correlated with two
so
of the interpersonal problems measured by Horowitz et al.’s (1988) Inventory
As
of Interpersonal Problems (IIP): being overly autocratic and overly competi-
tive, with rs of .44 and .47, ps < .01. Although deactivation was not associated
al
with problems in self-control, it was associated with lower scores on negative
ic
mood regulation and social skills, with rs ranging from −.42 to −.50, all ps <
og
.01. As expected, deactivation was also associated with two kinds of IIP inter-
ol
personal problems: being overly subassertive and overly exploitable, with rs of
ch
.58 and .52, ps < .01.
y
Taken together, these correlations imply that people who either hyper-
Ps
activate or deactivate their power system have a difficult time regulating their
negative emotions and lack the social skills that promote effective social
an
have problems with self-control and have difficulties in being overly aggres-
er
sive and competitive (see also Chapters 2 and 6), those who deactivate the
Am
Lieberman, & Mullan, 1981), optimism (Scheier & Carver, 1985), and psycho-
ig
logical well-being (Veit & Ware, 1983). As expected, both hyperactivation and
yr
A)
conflict in the laboratory. Both members of 100 young Israeli heterosexual
couples who had been dating for less than 5 months completed the PBSS, the
P
(A
ECR, and the Big Five Inventory, and then were invited to a laboratory ses-
sion in which they were asked to discuss a major unresolved problem in their
n
relationship. Each couple was videotaped while discussing this problem. Two
io
independent judges who were unaware of participants’ scores on the other
at
measures rated the extent to which each member of the couple displayed signs
ci
of anger, hostility, and distress, and the extent to which they attacked their
so
partner, deferred submissively to their partner, and reached a joint solution
As
to the problem.
As expected, participants who scored higher on power hyperactivation
al
were rated by judges as displaying more anger, hostility, and distress, and as exe-
ic
cuting more attacks on their partner, with rs ranging from .33 to .46, ps < .01. In
og
contrast, participants who scored higher on power deactivation were rated by
ol
judges as displaying more distress, but not anger or hostility, and more submis-
ch
sive behavior, with rs of .49 and .42, ps < .01. In addition, both hyperactivation
y
the relationship problem, with rs of −.32 and −.27, ps < .01. It is important that
these associations were unique to the PBSS and were not explained by attach-
an
ment insecurities or the Big Five personality trait scores. These findings indicate
ic
CONCLUSIONS
th
by
the power, aggression, and anger measures that were already available in the
20
literature. Our new scales, although still preliminary, seem promising in effi-
ciently assessing power system hyperactivation and deactivation as orthog-
©
are viewed as secondary strategies that come into play when a system’s pri-
yr
mary strategy fails to work, are associated with nonoptimal outcomes in adult
op
relationships. The new concepts and scales fit well in our overall conception
of social motivation based on the behavioral system construct. Their addition
C
A)
REFERENCES
P
(A
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attach-
ment: Assessed in the Strange Situation and at home. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
n
Borjesson, W. I., Aarons, G. A., & Dunn, M. E. (2003). Development and confirma-
io
tory factor analysis of the Abuse within Intimate Relationship Scale. Journal of
at
Interpersonal Violence, 18, 295–309. doi:10.1177/0886260502250089
ci
so
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New
York, NY: Basic Books.
As
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depression. New York, NY:
Basic Books.
al
ic
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
og
Basic Books.
ol
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of
adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes
ch
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York, NY:
y
Guilford Press.
Ps
Buhrmester, D., Furman, W., Wittenberg, M. T., & Reis, H. T. (1988). Five domains
an
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 63, 452–459. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452
Am
S0272-7358(97)00037-8
Cassidy, J., & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relationship with other defen-
by
and clinical applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
©
Catanzaro, S. J., & Mearns, J. (1990). Measuring generalized expectancies for nega-
tive mood regulation: Initial scale development and implications. Journal of Per-
ht
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive: Studies in evaluative
dependence. New York, NY: Wiley.
op
Freud, S. (1961). Beyond the pleasure principle (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York, NY:
C
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Gruenfeld, D. H., Whitson, J. A., & Liljenquist, K. A.
(2008). Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, con-
formity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1450–1466.
doi:10.1037/a0012633
A)
Gilbert, P. (1989). Human nature and suffering. Hove, England: Erlbaum.
P
Gough, H. G. (1964). California Psychological Inventory manual. Palo Alto, CA: Con-
(A
sulting Psychologists Press.
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motiva-
n
tional principle. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol-
io
ogy (Vol. 30, pp. 1–46). New York, NY: Academic Press.
at
ci
Horowitz, L. M., Rosenberg, S. E., Baer, B. A., Ureno, G., & Villasenor, L. (1988).
so
Inventory of Interpersonal Problems: Psychometric properties and clinical appli-
cations. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 885–892. doi:10.1037/
As
0022-006X.56.6.885
Jackson, D. N. (1984). Personality Research Form manual. Port Huron, MI: Research
al
Psychologists Press.
ic
og
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measure-
ment, and theoretical perspectives. In O. P. John & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Hand-
ol
book of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). New York, NY:
ch
Guilford Press.
y
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhi-
Ps
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University
Press.
ic
Parker, G. A. (1974). Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behavior. Jour-
nal of Theoretical Biology, 47, 223–243. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(74)90111-8
11
Pearlin, L. I., Menaghan, E. G., Lieberman, M. E., & Mullan, J. T. (1981). The stress
20
Plutchik, R., & Van Praag, H. M. (1990). A self-report measure of violence risk.
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31, 450–456. doi:10.1016/0010-440X(90)90031-M
ht
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment
and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4,
219–247. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.4.3.219
Shaver, P. R., Mikulincer, M., & Shemesh-Iron, M. (2010). A behavioral systems per-
A)
spective on prosocial behavior. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Prosocial
motives, emotions, and behavior: The better angels of our nature (pp. 73–91). Wash-
P
ington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/12061-004
(A
Shaver, P. R., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge:
n
Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
io
chology, 52, 1061–1086. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1061
at
Siegel, J. M. (1986). The Multidimensional Anger Inventory. Journal of Personality
ci
and Social Psychology, 51, 191–200. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.1.191
so
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child
As
Development, 48, 1184–1199. doi:10.2307/1128475
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Golub, A., & Cronwell, E. N. (2001). Development and vali-
al
dation of the anger rumination scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 31,
ic
689–700. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00171-9
og
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts
ol
good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Jour-
ch
nal of Personality, 72, 271–324. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
y
Veit, C. T., & Ware, J. E. (1983). The structure of psychological stress and well being
Ps
Wiggins, J. S., Trapnell, P., & Phillips, N. (1988). Psychometric and geometric char-
ic
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:30 AM
Am
er
ic
Page 88
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-06_CH05-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 89
PA)
DISPOSITIONAL INFLUENCES
(A
ON HUMAN AGGRESSION
n
io
at
ci
JENNIFER L. TACKETT AND ROBERT F. KRUEGER
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
tendencies arise, that is, how genetic and environmental factors combine
to create individual differences in aggression. We begin by considering how
e
aggressive tendencies fit with other personality constructs and how aggression
th
enduring characteristics that can be used to predict future behavior. The most
89
12051-06_CH05-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:30 AM Page 90
widely accepted trait model is the five-factor model (FFM; e.g., Goldberg, 1993).
The FFM is a taxonomy of higher order, broadly defined personality traits that
include Neuroticism, Extraversion, Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, and
Openness to Experience. Neuroticism is defined by the experience of negative
A)
moods and emotions, such as anxiety, dysphoria, and low self-esteem. Extra-
version encompasses positive emotions as well as behaviors such as sociability
P
(A
and assertiveness. Conscientiousness includes abilities to organize and inhibit
behavioral tendencies. Agreeableness includes affiliation and altruism concep-
n
tualized as the opposites of interpersonal irritability and antagonism. Openness
io
to Experience includes intellectual curiosity as well as imagination and fantasy.
at
The FFM is replicable across languages and cultures, making it a useful
ci
general framework for characterizing major individual differences. In addition,
so
recent research has demonstrated empirical connections between various
As
higher order personality trait models, which provides a better understanding
of interrelationships among them (Markon, Krueger, & Watson, 2005). There
al
is much less agreement about the structure of lower order personality traits
ic
(e.g., the facets of the five major traits assessed with the commonly used per-
og
sonality instrument, the NEO–PI–R measure of the traits anxiety, excitement-
ol
seeking, trust, openness to feelings, and self-discipline; for details, see Costa &
ch
McCrae, 1992), even though lower order traits are useful for distinguishing
y
Halverson et al., 2003). Structural analyses of trait measures suitable for chil-
ic
dren have found empirical relations among higher order trait models that par-
er
allel those found in adults (Tackett, Krueger, Iacono, & McGue, 2008),
Am
allowing comparison across studies using different trait models. Scores on the
five trait factors change systematically across the adult years (Srivastava, John,
e
Gosling, & Potter, 2003), but individual differences on the factors generally
th
the FFM, investigators have attempted to find the empirical structure of the
ig
and categorical, extensive evidence suggests that many disorders are better
conceived of in terms of continuous dimensions (Helzer, Kraemer, & Krueger,
C
A)
Psychopathology spectrums bear conceptual similarities to higher order
personality traits, both of which can be organized in a multidimensional space.
P
(A
This resemblance is empirical as well as conceptual. Especially relevant to this
chapter, forms of psychopathology that involve aggression can be viewed in
n
terms of a broad spectrum that has generally been labeled externalizing disorders.
io
Aggressive personality dispositions also fall within this spectrum (Krueger,
at
Markon, Patrick, Benning, & Kramer, 2007). The externalizing spectrum
ci
encompasses disinhibitory personality traits such as impulsivity and aggression,
so
as well as clinical disorders such as antisocial personality disorder and illicit sub-
As
stance dependence. In FFM terms, the externalizing spectrum is closely aligned
with a combination of disagreeableness and low conscientiousness, domains
al
that are associated with a higher order domain of disinhibition (Markon et al.,
ic
2005). That is, FFM disagreeableness and low conscientiousness are correlated
og
with each other, giving rise to a broader personality trait domain of disinhibi-
ol
tion. Stated somewhat differently, disinhibition (a combination of disagree-
ch
ableness and low conscientiousness) appears to be the personality-trait core of
y
6, this volume).
an
completed self-report questionnaires containing the items, and then the items
20
were pruned and refined based on psychometric analyses of the results. Both
factor and cluster analyses of the item-level data were used to isolate specific,
©
With specific facets in hand, we then used item response theory (IRT)
ig
analyses to ensure that the items indexing a specific facet also covered a range
yr
of individual differences within that facet. IRT models are suited to this task
op
because they parameterize the location of items along a specific dimension. The
goal was to ensure that the items were appropriately arrayed to cover a wide
C
range of the dimension. Items were then deleted or revised, and additional
A)
aggression. In addition to these three facets of aggression, 20 additional facets
of externalizing were identified (for a complete list, see Krueger et al., 2007,
P
(A
Table 2). These additional facets cover a range of content, including deficient
empathy; a tendency to externalize blame (i.e., to blame others for one’s own
n
problems); alienation from others in the interpersonal environment; problems
io
with alcohol, marijuana, and “harder” substances; difficulties controlling
at
impulses; behavior that would be grounds for arrest (e.g., theft and fraud); dis-
ci
honesty; irresponsibility; rebelliousness; a tendency to prefer exciting but
so
potentially unsafe activities to safer but more boring activities; and proneness
As
to boredom.
al
Structural Analysis of the 23 Facets of the Externalizing
ic
Spectrum in Adults
og
ol
Exploratory analyses of the 23 facets of the externalizing spectrum sug-
ch
gested that three factors could account for the relations among the 23 facets:
y
all of the scales to a nontrivial extent but most strongly indicated by irrespon-
sible and impulsive tendencies); (b) a narrower factor encompassing callous
an
indexing lack of empathy); and (c) a narrower factor encompassing use of and
er
matory models: (a) a one-factor model, where all 23 scales were indicators of
th
one and only one general externalizing factor; (b) a higher order model, where
by
the two narrower factors (callous aggression and substance use/problems) load
directly on the 23 facet scales, and the general externalizing factor, in turn,
11
loads on callous aggression and substance use/problems; and (c) a bifactor struc-
20
tural model (also known as a hierarchical model; Yung, Thissen, & McLeod,
1999) of the three factors (i.e., general externalizing, callous aggression, sub-
©
stance use/problems), where all indicators load on the general factor, and indi-
ht
The contrast between the higher order and bifactor models pertains to the
op
ways in which relations among the general, broad externalizing factor and the
narrower callous aggression and substance use/problems factors are modeled.
C
In the higher order model, the general, broad externalizing factor is indicated
indirectly via loadings of the narrower callous aggression and substance use/
problems factors, which are at an intermediate level between the 23 primary
facets and the general, broad externalizing factor. By contrast, in the bifactor
model, all 23 primary facets load directly on the general, broad externalizing
A)
factor, and specific subsets of scales also load directly on the narrower factors.
Multiple fit indices converged to indicate a superior fit for the bifactor
P
(A
model over the higher order model (Krueger et al., 2007). This finding had
intriguing implications for the meaning of individual differences in aggressive
n
tendencies, as indexed by the three aggression facet scales (i.e., relational, phys-
io
ical, destructive). These implications were revealed by considering the struc-
at
ture of the bifactor model (i.e., three mutually uncorrelated factors) along with
ci
the relative magnitudes of the loadings of the aggression scales on those factors.
so
Relational aggression was a stronger indicator of the callous–aggressive factor
As
than of the general externalizing factor, physical aggression was more closely
linked to overall externalizing than to callous aggression, and destructive
al
aggression loaded similarly on overall externalizing and callous aggression
ic
(albeit higher on overall externalizing).
og
The general conclusion is that there are multiple pathways to aggressive
ol
outcomes. Specific aggressive outcomes can emerge because a person’s impulses
ch
are unconstrained in general (i.e., via the general externalizing factor) or
y
because a person is unusually callous and aggressive (i.e., via the callous–
Ps
superior fit of the bifactor model, when compared with the two other models
er
(i.e., the one factor model and higher order model). In the bifactor model,
Am
the factors are mutually uncorrelated. Hence, the factors can be interpreted
as independent, multiple pathways to a specific outcome such as relational
e
are largely consistent with findings for adults, although these younger age
ig
groups are less often studied in the personality literature. Aggressive and
yr
A)
ducted within a vulnerability, or risk, framework, which postulates that per-
sonality or temperament serves as a risk factor for the development of later
P
(A
disorders. An alternative framework is the spectrum or common cause model,
which conceptualizes personality and externalizing psychopathology as
n
dimensionally related and as sharing core etiologic factors. One issue in this
io
literature is that longitudinal studies that fail to measure potentially shared
at
causal factors cannot disentangle evidence for a vulnerability model from evi-
ci
dence for a spectrum model (Tackett, 2006).
so
Externalizing disorders in younger age groups have their own disorder cat-
As
egories in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text
rev., American Psychiatric Association, 2000). They include oppositional defi-
al
ant disorder, conduct disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
ic
(ADHD). Other potential externalizing behaviors in early adolescence include
og
precocious sexual behavior and early substance use. Some behaviors, such as
ol
relational aggression (i.e., using social power and social exclusion to aggress
ch
against one’s victims), are still relatively new as forms of pathological aggres-
y
sion and are not yet incorporated into the childhood externalizing disorders
Ps
One important task for future research is to identify both common and
er
agreeableness (Schell & Tackett, 2010). This finding suggests that we should
by
currently limited by the lack of strong empirical studies of the structure of the
20
2001; Reynolds & Clark, 2001), but the structure of these traits in childhood
yr
and adolescence is also not well studied (Shiner, 1998; Tackett et al., 2008).
op
mentioned previously, lower order traits reflect more narrowly defined charac-
teristics (e.g., warmth) than do more broadly defined higher order traits (e.g.,
extraversion). A few theoretical reviews have begun to compare and integrate
proposed lower order trait structures in different models (e.g., Halverson et al.,
A)
2003; Shiner & Caspi, 2003), which provides an important starting point for
future empirical work.
P
(A
An additional place to look for relevant lower order traits is the research
literature on such narrowly defined personality characteristics as callous–
n
unemotional traits, which are related to conduct problems (e.g., Frick,
io
Cornell, Barry, Bodin, & Dane, 2003). Callous–unemotional characteristics
at
reflect remorselessness and a lack of empathy. Frick and colleagues have begun
ci
to integrate callous–unemotional traits into broader personality and tempera-
so
ment models, which have identified consistent connections with the broader
As
externalizing work by highlighting the importance of negative emotionality
and effortful control (Frick & Sheffield-Morris, 2004) and low levels of agree-
al
ableness and conscientiousness (Essau, Sasagawa, & Frick, 2006). One useful
ic
approach might be to use the 23-facet model developed by Krueger et al.
og
(2007) for adults and extend it downward to see whether it applies to children
ol
and adolescents. Some parallels already seem clear. For example, callousness
ch
figures prominently as a narrow-band factor in the Krueger et al. (2007) model,
y
INTEGRATING ENVIRONMENTAL
ic
to personality research (for a recent review, see Funder, 2008) and is useful
for understanding aggressive and externalizing tendencies (see Chapters 1, 7,
11
genetic factors and this general finding appears to be largely consistent across
the life span.
©
on behavioral outcomes that includes both stability and change and considers
yr
iors (Caspi, Moffitt, Newman, & Silva, 1996), recent research suggests that this
A)
Moffitt, 2003).
Personality can be an important moderator of individual responses to a
P
(A
particular kind of situation, including aggressive responses. For example, indi-
viduals low in Conscientiousness are more likely to engage in aggressive behav-
n
ior in the face of anger-provoking stimuli than individuals who are high in
io
Conscientiousness (Jensen-Campbell, Knack, Waldrip, & Campbell, 2007).
at
That is, the experience of angry affect alone does not determine a person’s
ci
responses, because self-regulatory capacities act on the affective experience to
so
differentiate individual responses. This is consistent with temperament models
As
that differentiate approach/positive emotionality and avoidance/negative emo-
tionality from self-regulatory traits that are considered superordinate in these
al
models (e.g., Ahadi & Rothbart, 1994; Carver, Johnson, & Joormann, 2008;
ic
Clark, 2005).
og
Psychobiological factors such as hormones, genes, and cortical activity
ol
also play an important role in explaining connections between personality traits
ch
and externalizing behaviors (see Chapter 6; see also Chapter 8 and 9). For
y
studies of brain processes have also shed light on associations between person-
Am
has been linked to both externalizing behaviors and trait impulsivity (Iacono,
th
variables (for a taxonomy of such interactions, see Chapter 2, this volume). For
ht
trols and adolescents with ADHD) show increased left-sided amygdala activa-
yr
tion when presented with negative pictures (Herpertz et al., 2008). In related
op
work, individuals with high levels of trait anger (compared with individuals
lower in trait anger) show increased left frontal activation in response to anger-
C
A)
GENETIC RESEARCH ON EXTERNALIZING PHENOMENA
P
(A
In recent years, genetic research on externalizing syndromes and behav-
iors has focused on exploring the coherence of these syndromes as elements
n
within a broader spectrum, following from the phenotypic work described
io
earlier in this chapter. Genetic effects on different externalizing syndromes
at
are mostly common across these syndromes, but there are also specific genetic
ci
effects on substance-dependence syndromes that are not shared with other
so
externalizing syndromes (Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003; Krueger
As
et al., 2002; Young, Stallings, Corley, Krauter, & Hewitt, 2000). This makes
a great deal of physiological sense: It is reasonable for dependence on sub-
al
stances to be traceable to both genetic effects unique to substances (presum-
ic
ably reflecting substance metabolism) and more general effects, presumably
og
reflecting disinhibited personality traits such as disagreeableness and lack of
ol
conscientiousness. Note the similarity of these findings to the model proposed
ch
by Krueger et al. (2007) and described earlier, where drug problems were
y
affected by a specific factor, beyond the effect of the general externalizing fac-
Ps
gene-hunting studies.
ic
genetic linkage was stronger for the externalizing component than for the
th
Going beyond linkage, Dick and her colleagues also studied single
20
ated with risk of alcohol dependence combined with drug dependence, mak-
ht
as compared with the other syndromes, was most strongly associated with with
op
SNPs in CHRM2.
With regard to Gene × Environment interactions, both twin studies and
C
A)
mate that applies to an entire population, without regard to various subgroups
within that population. Refining these general estimates with subgroup infor-
P
mation is one way to pursue Gene × Environment interaction effects, because
(A
genetic effects may be moderated by measurable environmental variables
n
associated with population subgroups.
io
An example is a recent study by Legrand, Keyes, McGue, Iacono, and
at
Krueger (2008). A sample of 17-year-old twins assessed on diverse externaliz-
ci
ing syndromes was divided into those living in rural areas and those living in
so
urban areas. In urban areas, genetic influences predominated, but in rural areas,
As
shared environmental effects (environmental effects making people similar
within families) predominated. This suggests that the previously described 80%
al
heritability masks interesting and potentially important subgroup differences.
ic
In the domain of molecular genetic studies, Caspi et al. (2002) examined
og
how childhood maltreatment interacted with a gene coding for MAO-A
ol
(monoamine oxidase-A, an enzyme that metabolizes major neurotransmitters)
ch
in predicting antisocial behavior in a birth cohort of males. These investiga-
y
hood through adulthood (Blonigen & Krueger, 2007; Tackett, 2006). Lower
yr
order personality traits may offer increased predictive validity over higher
op
A)
sional relations among correlated personality traits and externalizing behaviors.
This approach has been fruitful in identifying factors common to the broader
P
(A
externalizing domain as well as specific factors differentiating types of external-
izing behaviors (Krueger et al., 2002, 2007; Tackett, Krueger, Sawyer, & Graetz,
n
2003; Tackett, Krueger, Iacono, & McGue, 2005; Tackett et al., 2009). Indeed,
io
as work on molecular genetic linkage and Gene × Environment interactions
at
progresses, it is possible that the spectrum approach will be helpful in the search
ci
for specific genes and relevant environmental stressors (Dick et al., 2008).
so
Nevertheless, limitations to a full understanding of personality connec-
As
tions to externalizing behaviors remain. There is not yet a clear picture of the
life span trajectory of the externalizing spectrum (Tackett, 2010). For example,
al
some disorders typically conceptualized as externalizing syndromes in child-
ic
og
hood do not have officially recognized analogs in adult disorder typologies (e.g.,
ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder). Other behaviors, such as relational
ol
aggression, which are often viewed as externalizing (e.g., Baker, Jacobson,
ch
Raine, Lozano, & Bezdjian, 2007; Krueger et al., 2007), are not yet clearly iden-
y
span perspective.
ic
focus on the role of gene expression, are also becoming increasingly salient to
th
REFERENCES
ig
yr
A)
twin study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 219–235. doi:10.1037/0021-
843X.116.2.219
P
(A
Blonigen, D. M., & Krueger, R. F. (2007). Personality and violence: The unifying
role of structural models of personality. In I. Waldman, D. J. Flannery, & A. T.
n
Vazsonyi (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior (pp. 288–305). Cam-
io
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
at
Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., & Joormann, J. (2008). Serotonergic function, two-mode
ci
models of self-regulation, and vulnerability to depression: What depression has
so
in common with impulsive aggression. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 912–943.
As
doi:10.1037/a0013740
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., Taylor, A., &
al
Poulton, R. (2002, August). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in mal-
ic
treated children. Science, 297, 851–854. doi:10.1126/science.1072290
og
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Newman, D. L., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Behavioral obser-
ol
vations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Archives of General
ch
Psychiatry, 53, 1033–1039.
y
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL:
ic
Dick, D. M., Aliev, F., Wang, J. C., Grucza, R. A., Schuckit, M., Kuperman, S.,
Am
310–318.
th
Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Barry, C. T., Bodin, S. D., & Dane, H. E. (2003).
20
ways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33,
yr
54–68. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6
op
and research (3rd ed., pp. 568–580). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
A)
descriptions of children: The Inventory of Child Individual Differences. Journal
of Personality, 71, 995–1026. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.7106005
P
(A
Harmon-Jones, E. (2007). Trait anger predicts relative left frontal cortical activation
to anger-inducing stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66, 154–160.
n
doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.020
io
Hay, D. F. (2007). The gradual emergence of sex differences in aggression:
at
Alternative hypotheses. Psychological Medicine, 37, 1527–1537. doi:10.1017/
ci
S0033291707000165
so
Helzer, J. E., Kraemer, H. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2006). The feasibility and need for
As
dimensional psychiatric diagnoses. Psychological Medicine, 36, 1671–1680.
doi:10.1017/S003329170600821X
al
ic
Herpertz, S. C., Huebner, T., Marx, I., Vloet, T. D., Fink, G. R., Stoecker, T., . . .
og
Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2008). Emotional processing in male adolescents with
childhood-onset conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and
ol
Allied Disciplines, 49, 781–791. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01905.x
ch
Hicks, B. M., Bernat, E., Malone, S. M., Iacono, W. G., Patrick, C. J., Krueger, R. F.,
y
Iacono, W. G., Malone, S. M., & McGue, M. (2003). Substance use disorders, exter-
ic
of child and adolescent psychiatry (5th ed., pp. 377–391). Oxford, England: Black-
th
well. doi:10.1002/9781444300895.ch25
by
doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.05.001
Kaminsky, Z., Petronis, A., Wang, S. C., Levine, B., Ghaffar, O., Floden, D., &
©
identical twins discordant for risk-taking behavior. Twin Research and Human
ig
Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of
op
genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance
use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 929–937.
C
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., Newcombe, R., Craig, I. W., &
Moffitt, T. E. (2006). MAO-A, maltreatment, and gene–environment inter-
action predicting children’s mental health: New evidence and a meta-analysis.
Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 903–913. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001851
A)
Krueger, R. F., Hicks, B. M., Patrick, C. J., Carlson, S. R., Iacono, W. G., & McGue,
M. (2002). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial
P
behavior, and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of
(A
Abnormal Psychology, 111, 411–424. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.3.411
n
Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-based
io
approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annual Review of
at
Clinical Psychology, 2, 111–133. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095213
ci
Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., & Kramer, M. (2007).
so
Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: An integrative
As
quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 116, 645–666. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.645
al
Lahey, B. B., & Waldman, I. D. (2003). A developmental propensity model of the
ic
origins of conduct problems during childhood and adolescence. In B. B. Lahey,
og
T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delin-
ol
quency (pp. 76–117). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
ch
Legrand, L. N., Keyes, M., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., & Krueger, R. F. (2008).
y
Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the structure of nor-
ic
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York, NY: Guil-
ford Press.
e
McGirr, A., Renaud, J., Bureau, A., Seguin, M., Lesage, A., & Turecki, G. (2008).
th
Mill, J., & Petronis, A. (2007). Molecular studies of major depressive disorder:
20
T. E. Moffitt, & A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delin-
yr
j.1469-7610.2006.01612.x
Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big Five factors and facets and the pre-
diction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 524–539.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.3.524
Reynolds, S. K., & Clark, L. A. (2001). Predicting dimensions of personality disorder
A)
from domains and facets of the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 69,
199–222. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00142
P
(A
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of
temperament at 3 to 7 years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child
n
Development, 72, 1394–1408. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00355
io
Saudino, K. J., Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1996). Tester-rated temperament at 14,
at
20, and 24 months: Environmental change and genetic continuity. The British
ci
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 129–144.
so
Schell, G. C., & Tackett, J. L. (2009). Agency and communion as indicators of person-
As
ality in middle childhood. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Shiner, R. L. (1998). How shall we speak of children’s personalities in middle child-
al
hood? A preliminary taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 308–332. doi:10.1037/
ic
0033-2909.124.3.308
og
Shiner, R., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in childhood and adolescence:
ol
Measurement, development, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology and
ch
Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 44, 2–32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101
y
Srivastava, S., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2003). Development of per-
Ps
sonality in early and middle adulthood: Set like plaster or persistent change?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1041–1053. doi:10.1037/0022-
an
3514.84.5.1041
ic
Stallings, M. C., Corley, R. P., Dennehey, B., Hewitt, J. K., Krauter, K. S., Lessem,
er
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.003
Tackett, J. L. (2010). Toward a developmentally integrated externalizing spectrum in
11
Tackett, J. L., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2005). Symptom-based
subfactors of DSM-defined conduct disorder: Evidence for etiologic distinctions.
©
Tackett, J. L., Krueger, R. F., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2008). Personality in
ig
Tackett, J. L., Krueger, R. F., Sawyer, M. G., & Graetz, B. W. (2003). Subfactors of
C
DSM–IV conduct disorder: Evidence and connections with syndromes from the
A)
Abnormal Psychology, 118, 722–733.
Young, S. E., Stallings, M. C., Corley, R. P., Krauter, K. S., & Hewitt, J. K. (2000).
P
(A
Genetic and environmental influences on behavioral disinhibition. American
Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 96, 684–695.
n
doi:10.1002/1096-8628(20001009)96:5<684::AID-AJMG16>3.0.CO;2-G
io
Yung, Y., Thissen, D., & McLeod, L. D. (1999). On the relationship between the
at
higher order factor model and the hierarchical factor model. Psychometrika, 64,
ci
113–128. doi:10.1007/BF02294531
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
A)
P
A SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE
(A
PERSPECTIVE ON THE
n
io
NEUROBIOLOGICAL BASES
at
ci
OF AGGRESSION
so
As
THOMAS F. DENSON
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
chapter reviews research that has increased our understanding of the neural
mechanisms associated with human aggression. This review contains a brief
11
X- and C-systems model, to the study of anger and aggression. This is followed
ht
This chapter was supported by the Australian Research Council’s Discovery Projects funding scheme
(DP0985182). Thank you to Ajay Satpute for comments on an earlier draft of this chapter.
105
12051-07_CH06-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:34 AM Page 106
ANATOMY
A)
2008; Siever, 2008). Indeed, the PFC is broadly involved in the regulation and
control of affect and behavior. Within the PFC, four regions are particularly rel-
P
(A
evant to aggressive behavior. The dorsolateral PFC is involved in planning and
behavioral control. The ventral PFC, which encompasses the ventromedial
n
PFC, ventrolateral PFC, and the orbitofrontal cortex, is involved in emotion reg-
io
ulation. The dorsal region of medial PFC (mPFC) is involved in introspection
at
and the awareness of emotion as well as emotion regulation. Finally, the dorsal
ci
anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) is involved in the detection of conflict and
so
triggering activity in top-down control regions (see Figure 6.1).
As
In terms of neurotransmitter involvement, converging evidence sug-
gests that serotonin has a prominent role in facilitating and inhibiting anger
al
and hostile aggression via 5-HT2 receptors in the PFC (Davidson et al., 2000;
ic
Siever, 2008; see also Chapter 8, this volume). Because a detailed discussion
og
of neurotransmitter modulation is beyond the scope of this chapter, the pur-
ol
pose here is to review the most current research on brain regions implicated
ch
in anger and aggression.
y
Ps
Dorsolateral PFC
an
Dorsal anterior
ic
Ventromedial PFC
yr
op
Orbitofrontal cortex
C
Figure 6.1. Brain regions implicated in anger and aggression. PFC = prefrontal cortex.
A)
classic case of Phineas Gage, who suffered trauma to his orbitofrontal cortex, is
illustrative of the dramatic within-person change from agreeable and conscien-
P
(A
tious to hostile and antagonistic that is associated with trauma to this region. A
fairly large study of 279 Vietnam War veterans with brain lesions revealed that
n
those who suffered injury to the orbitofrontal cortex and mPFC were more irri-
io
table, hostile, and aggressive than control participants (Grafman et al., 1996).
at
More recent neuroimaging studies have examined structural differences
ci
in groups of individuals known to be highly aggressive compared with less
so
aggressive matched controls. Several studies have identified prefrontal struc-
As
tural differences in the brains of aggressive individuals, such as violent offend-
ers and psychopaths, relative to matched controls (for reviews, see Raine, 2008;
al
Yang, Glenn, & Raine, 2008). For instance, individuals with antisocial person-
ic
ality disorder have an 11% to 14% deficit in prefrontal gray matter relative to
og
normal controls, substance-dependent individuals, and individuals with other
ol
psychiatric disorders (Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, LaCasse, & Colletti, 2000).
ch
These differences are not limited to brain structure. Numerous studies
y
ventral PFC, dorsolateral PFC, mPFC, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), pos-
terior cingulate cortex (PCC), hippocampus, and amygdala (Raine, 2008;
an
Raine & Yang, 2008). For instance, a positron emission tomography (PET)
ic
ers compared with a matched control group (Raine, Buchsbaum, & LaCasse,
Am
1997). Another study found that when processing emotional versus neutral
words, criminal psychopaths show less activation in the lateral PFC, ACC,
e
these abnormalities reduce the ability to regulate and control angry feelings and
20
aggressive behavior.
©
ht
A)
Social neuroscience is an emerging field that examines the neural correlates of
social psychological phenomena. Because functional neuroimaging technology,
P
(A
especially functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), is relatively widely
available, we are now able to examine neural processes in normal individuals
n
using experimental methods drawn from social and cognitive psychology. Tools
io
such as fMRI enable us to explore previously unquantifiable aspects of human
at
functioning that are directly relevant to social psychological questions.
ci
In an effort to integrate findings from the rapidly expanding field of
so
social neuroscience, Lieberman and colleagues (Lieberman, 2007; Lieberman,
As
Gaunt, Gilbert, & Trope, 2002; Satpute & Lieberman, 2006) proposed a
dual-process framework known as the X- and C-systems model. The X com-
al
ponent of the model, which we likely share with other animals, represents our
ic
continuous stream of current experience (Lieberman et al., 2002, p. 204). The
og
X stands for the x in reflexive. The X-system is phylogenetically older than
ol
the C-system, operates quickly, supports spontaneous processes, and engages
ch
in parallel processing. Brain regions that make up the X-system are the ventro-
y
medial PFC, dACC, amygdala, basal ganglia, and lateral temporal cortex.
Ps
control processes. The C stands for the c in reflective. The C-system is phylo-
Am
genetically newer than the X-system, operates slowly, is associated with voli-
tional processes, uses serial processing (i.e., not parallel processing), and
e
problem. Brain regions that make up the C-system include the lateral and
20
mPFC, lateral and medial parietal cortex, medial temporal lobe, and ventro-
lateral PFC. Social psychological phenomena associated with the C-system
©
reappraising emotional events, and moral reasoning (see Lane, Fink, Chau,
ig
anger and aggression was not considered in Lieberman’s (2007) review, the
op
X- and C-systems provide a valuable framework for discussing the neural bases
of anger and aggression. Specifically, many of the processes associated with
C
A)
viduals. Specifically, I review social neuroscience research relevant to anger,
angry rumination, cognitive neoassociation theory, social learning, media
P
influences, and aggressive behavior.
(A
n
Anger
io
at
Most functional imaging studies of anger in nonclinical samples have
ci
exposed participants to angry faces or asked them to recall and reexperience
so
anger-inducing events. Two recent meta-analyses of nine PET and f MRI
As
studies revealed that some of the most prominent areas of frontal and limbic
brain reactivity were the mPFC, ventromedial PFC, ACC, PCC, lateral PFC,
al
and thalamus (Murphy, Nimmo-Smith, & Lawrence, 2003; Phan, Wager,
ic
Taylor, & Liberzon, 2002). It is interesting that whereas the amygdala has a
og
prominent role in fear, it was not implicated in these studies of angry faces
and memories (Phan et al., 2002). ol
ch
My colleagues and I recently examined the neural correlates of anger more
directly by exposing participants to an interpersonal insult (Denson, Pedersen,
y
Ps
Ronquillo, & Nandy, 2009). Anderson and Bushman (2002) described such a
provocation as “perhaps the most important single cause of human aggression”
an
(p. 37). In this fMRI study, participants were asked to complete difficult ana-
grams and state the answer aloud or say “no answer” if they did not know the
ic
answer. Following two polite prompts to speak louder, the experimenter then
er
this is the third time I’ve had to say this! Can’t you follow directions?” We found
that, relative to baseline, participants showed increased activation in many
e
of the same regions active during exposure to angry faces and autobiographical
th
(r = .56). Activity in the dACC was also correlated with scores on the Buss and
20
involved in the subjective experience of anger, individuals who have had por-
ht
tions of the ACC removed demonstrate decreased anger (Cohen et al., 2000).
ig
yr
Angry Rumination
op
of the C-system that have been implicated in anger and aggression include
the lateral PFC, mPFC, and medial parietal cortex. As noted earlier,
following provocation, participants demonstrated increased activity
in regions of the C-system as well as the X-system. One important reflec-
tive process that can influence aggressive behavior is the way one
A)
regulates emotions. One particularly pernicious form of emotion regula-
tion is angry rumination. Immersive rumination on anger-inducing expe-
P
(A
riences increases anger, aggression, cardiovascular arousal, and cortisol
levels (Bushman, 2002; Bushman, Bonacci, Pedersen, Vasquez, & Miller,
n
2005; Denson, Fabiansson, Creswell, & Pedersen, 2009; Denson, Pedersen,
io
& Miller, 2006; Ray, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2008; Rusting & Nolen-Hoeksema,
at
1998).
ci
In addition to these negative consequences of angry rumination, our
so
recent research demonstrates that angry rumination also increases activity
As
in parts of the C-system (Denson, Pedersen, et al., 2009). In the second part
of the experiment described earlier, following the provocation, participants
al
were asked to engage in a “memory task,” which served as a rumination
ic
manipulation. Using a modified within-participants rumination task from
og
prior research (Bushman et al., 2005; Denson et al., 2006; Rusting &
ol
Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998), during the rumination task participants were
ch
asked to think about what had occurred in the experiment so far, who they
y
interacted with, and their current mood. During the distraction period, par-
Ps
nation was correlated with activity in the mPFC (r = .42), as were scores
er
the fact that when provoked, individuals high in trait displaced aggression
th
tend to ruminate about the event rather than immediately retaliate against
by
the provocateur.
We expected that the mPFC would be especially relevant to angry rumi-
11
rumination. For example, the mPFC is activated during tasks that require the
self-awareness of emotions and self-relevant cognition (Lane et al., 1997;
©
Lieberman, 2007; Ochsner et al., 2004). This region is also active when mon-
ht
tional responses to distressing stimuli (Amodio & Frith, 2006; Ochsner et al.,
yr
2004; Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002). Furthermore, Ray et al.
op
(2005) reported that when participants were asked to decrease their negative
affective responses to aversive photographs, a composite measure of trait
C
A)
(Denson, Pedersen, et al., 2009). The dACC is of social psychological inter-
est because it is involved in at least two additional negative emotional states
P
(A
that have been shown to increase aggression. This is relevant because a core
tenet of Berkowitz’s (1993) cognitive neoassociationistic model of aggression
n
states that any form of negative affect can increase aggression. A large body
io
of evidence supports Berkowitz’s supposition. For example, physical pain and
at
social rejection both increase aggression and activation in the dACC
ci
(Berkowitz, Cochran, & Embree, 1981; Eisenberger, Liberman, & Williams,
so
2003; Rainville, Duncan, Price, Carrier, & Bushnell, 1997; Twenge, Baumeis-
As
ter, Tice, & Stucke, 2001). These and the anger findings converge to suggest
the existence of a common neural mechanism underlying the process Berkowitz
al
proposed, although the mediating role of dACC activation between anger,
ic
pain, social rejection, and actual aggressive behavior remains to be investi-
og
gated. Future neuroimaging research examining additional aversive stimuli
ol
known to increase aggression, such as noxious odors and heat, might demon-
ch
strate increased activity in the dACC as well.
y
Ps
learn how and when to behave aggressively by observing others either in per-
er
behavioral scripts from aggressive media exposure. Scripts are closely con-
th
chronic exposure to violent media. When activated, such scripts can increase
the likelihood of aggressive behavior (see Chapter 7, this volume).
11
dren most responsive to violent media, children who showed heart rate accel-
ht
and nonviolent (i.e., animal) scenes while functional images were acquired
yr
(Murray et al., 2006). The strongest activity was located in the right PCC and
op
the right precuneus (in the medial parietal cortex). Because of its role in
memory, these authors speculated that activity in the PCC might correspond
C
research showing that the PCC was active during angry rumination, because
the revenge planning that occurs during angry rumination likely involves the
activation of aggressive scripts (Denson, Pedersen, et al., 2009). Indeed, fac-
tor analytic work indicates that angry rumination involves rehearsing acts
A)
of revenge (Caprara, 1986; Denson et al., 2006; Sukhodolsky, Golub, &
Cromwell, 2001).
P
(A
Two additional studies found that violent media influence brain activ-
ity in what is thought to be a maladaptive manner. Specifically, in an inves-
n
tigation of the hypothesis that chronic exposure to violent media desensitizes
io
individuals to aggressive content, one study examined the P300 component
at
of the event-related brain potential in people who played relatively high lev-
ci
els of aggressive video games and those who played aggressive video games less
so
often (Bartholow, Bushman, & Sestir, 2006). When exposed to violent
As
images in the laboratory, chronic exposure to violent video games was asso-
ciated with decreased P300 amplitude to violent images but not to nega-
al
tive or neutral images. These findings support the notion that violent video
ic
game play can desensitize basic neural responses to violent stimuli. Further-
og
more, the P300 deficit predicted increased aggression as assessed by choosing
ol
to deliver loud noise blasts to a fictitious participant. Although electro-
ch
encephalogram methods do not allow for precise localization of brain processes,
y
presumably the automatic response of the P300 reflects activity in the X-system,
Ps
suggesting that chronic exposure to media violence can alter even quite rudi-
mentary information processing such that individuals with high levels of
an
exposure.
er
ity in the dACC preceded suppression in the rostral ACC (rACC), which is
th
aggressive “search and destroy” sequences. Recall that the dACC is associ-
ated with the subjective experience of anger (Denson, Pedersen, et al., 2009).
11
This suppression of the rACC by the dACC when committing acts of video
20
game violence suggests that the dACC overrides affective input from the
rACC. Consistent with the suppression function of the dACC, when partic-
©
ipants were in danger, under attack, or using a weapon, the dACC was more
ht
Aggressive Behavior
op
actual acts of aggression. The first such fMRI study examined brain activity in
A)
est, activity in the mPFC was positively correlated with the intensity of pain
participants chose to have administered to the confederate. This latter finding
P
(A
is consistent with the role of the mPFC in angry rumination, emotion regula-
tion, attributions, and theory of mind (e.g., Amodio & Frith, 2006; Denson,
n
Pedersen, et al., 2009; Harris, Todorov, & Fiske, 2005; Ochsner et al., 2002).
io
A second study of normal young adults used a modified white-noise
at
Taylor (1967) paradigm in which the decision phase (i.e., deciding what noise
ci
level to choose) and the outcome phase (i.e., the aggressive act) were analyzed
so
separately (Krämer, Jansma, Tempelmann, & Münte, 2007). Furthermore,
As
participants were given the opportunity to aggress against highly provocative
and less provocative bogus participants as well as against a computer. This
al
allowed the authors to identify the neural mechanisms specifically associated
ic
with aggression rather than social interaction per se. Under high provocation,
og
the dACC and mPFC were active during the decision phase, suggesting the
ol
presence of anger and rumination (e.g., Denson, Pedersen, et al., 2009).
ch
Perhaps the most intriguing result of this study is that a component of
y
phase in which participants chose the level at which to blast the highly
provocative participant with noise. This finding, which the authors called the
an
neural basis that might partially explain why it is difficult to reduce retalia-
Am
ever, the authors concluded that this was most likely due to relief derived
th
ries of aggression and media violence, involve elements of both the X- and
20
C-systems. The concept of a dual system harkens back to the days of Freud,
who posited an innate form of destruction motivation (i.e., Thanatos) ema-
©
nating from the id that resists control by the ego and superego. This notion
ht
remains with us today (see Chapter 2). However, the X- and C-systems frame-
yr
work differs markedly from Freud’s notion of destructive drives. The X- and
op
C- systems are compatible. When the situation calls for it, the C-system inter-
venes. For example, when someone cuts us off on the freeway while making
C
an obscene gesture, the dACC sounds the neural alarm and snaps us out of
A)
ROLE OF TOP-DOWN CONTROL AND EMOTION
P
(A
REGULATION MECHANISMS
n
For the aggressor, aggressive behavior can have positive consequences
io
(e.g., self-defense, achievement of dominance) and negative consequences
at
(e.g., developing a bad reputation, instigating retaliation, and even being
ci
killed). Evolutionary theorists argue that in our ancestral past, aggression was
so
a risky strategy, but when successfully executed, aggression likely increased
As
reproductive success (e.g., MacDonald, 2008; see also Chapters 3 and 4, this
volume). Although he did not use the X- and C-system framework, in a review
al
of the literature on effortful control, MacDonald (2008) argued that a con-
ic
scious system located in the PFC (C-system) allows humans to inhibit prepo-
og
tent impulses toward aggression stemming primarily from limbic structures
ol
(X-system). When the X-system cannot solve a problem, it calls on the
ch
C-system. It is the C-system that allows individuals to make “explicit appraisals
y
The general aggression model (GAM; Anderson & Bushman, 2002; see
Am
also Chapter 1, this volume) highlights the importance of the explicit decision-
making process. According to the GAM, appraisals and decision-making
e
Chapter 2, this volume). Acute alcohol intoxication has similar effects, likely
ht
In the modern world, aggression is still risky. One might even argue that
yr
are typically a more likely outcome following aggression than any positive
consequences. Thus, the ability to effectively weigh the costs and benefits of
C
aggression is critical (see Chapter 3). This is not to say that the C-system flaw-
lessly functions in our best interest. Indeed, in modern society we still find
that most aggressive acts are impulsive acts. For example, the majority of
homicides and other aggressive crimes occur when people are provoked and
angry and either explicitly decide to aggress or are simply unable to resist the
A)
motivation to behave aggressively. Thus, the immediate cause of many acts
of aggression is often a loss of self-control (DeWall et al., 2007).
P
(A
Substantial neuropsychological and imaging research indicates that the
PFC is the seat of self-control (Banfield, Wyland, Macrae, Munte, & Heather-
n
ton, 2004), and integral parts of the circuit that underlie self-control are the
io
dACC, dorsolateral PFC, and mPFC (Cohen, Botvinick, & Carter, 2000).
at
The neural substrates of emotion regulation include the medial, ventrolateral
ci
PFC, and ventromedial PFC. The lateral and medial regions of the PFC share
so
rich connectivity with cortical and limbic structures such as the dACC and
As
ventromedial PFC and have been implicated in emotion regulation and
behavioral control (Inzlicht & Gutsell, 2007). Accordingly, Davidson et al.
al
(2000) proposed that impaired functioning of an emotion regulation circuit
ic
involving the dACC, ventromedial PFC, and the dorsolateral PFC predisposes
og
individuals to aggressive behavior. Indeed, engaging in self-control or com-
ol
pleting neuropsychological measures that rely on inhibitory ability recruits the
ch
dACC and dorsolateral PFC (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen,
y
2001; Richeson et al., 2003). As noted previously, both provocation and sub-
Ps
Part of the anger and aggression circuit, the dACC, has been dubbed a
ic
“neural alarm system” because of its role in detecting conflict (Eisenberger &
er
Lieberman, 2004; Kross, Egner, Ochsner, Hirsch, & Downey, 2007). In the
Am
Chapter 3, this volume). This is consistent with the associations of the dACC
th
with self-reported social distress and anger following ostracism and provoca-
by
tion (Denson, Pedersen, et al., 2009; Eisenberger et al., 2003). Because there
are costs and benefits to aggression, there is also likely to be a conflict between
11
suggested, when a person is unjustly wronged, it is likely that the dACC ini-
tiates regulatory behavior via activity in the dorsolateral PFC.
©
lescents who had been diagnosed with a disruptive behavior disorder, includ-
yr
ing aggressive features, and matched controls who had either high or low levels
op
The aggressive group demonstrated decreased activity, and this activation was
not different from that in the normal adolescents who had been exposed to
high levels of media violence. Only the normal adolescents with low levels of
exposure to media violence demonstrated the typical pattern of lateral PFC
and dACC activity during the Stroop task.
A)
P
(A
CONCLUSION AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
n
The evidence reviewed here implicates a network of neural regions that
io
underlie anger, angry rumination, aggression, and media violence. Activity
at
in these regions and the processes that they support are consistent with social
ci
psychological models of aggression, such as cognitive neoassociation theory,
so
script theory, and the GAM. The hostile aggression circuit described here
As
implicates limbic and top-down prefrontal regions, which support both reflec-
tive and reflexive processes. Anger, pain, and social rejection are mediated by
al
activity in the dACC, whereas angry rumination is mediated by the mPFC.
ic
There is also evidence that high levels of exposure to media violence are asso-
og
ciated with abnormal functioning in the aggression circuit. Furthermore, dur-
ol
ing actual acts of aggression, neural regions involved in reward processing are
ch
active. This finding partially explains the difficulty associated with effectively
y
reducing aggression.
Ps
while. Future research could also investigate genetic markers that might influ-
er
Additional work could also investigate the connection between neural activity,
other systems (e.g., cardiovascular, endocrine), and actual aggressive behavior
e
in more detail. Furthermore, one might also examine the effects of inter-
th
REFERENCES
ht
ig
Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and
yr
Anderson, C. A., Berkowitz, L., Donnerstein, E., Huesmann, L. R., Johnson, J. D.,
Linz, D., . . . Wartella, E. (2003). The influence of media violence on youth.
C
A)
The cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs
(Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 62–83). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
P
(A
Bartholow, B. D., Bushman, B. J., & Sestir, M. A. (2006). Chronic violent video
game exposure and desensitization to violence: Behavioral and event-related
n
brain potential data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 532–539.
io
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.08.006
at
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York, NY:
ci
McGraw-Hill.
so
Berkowitz, L., Cochran, S., & Embree, M. C. (1981). Physical pain and the goal of
As
aversively stimulated aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40,
687–700. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.40.4.687
al
ic
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001).
og
Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
ol
Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis,
ch
rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding. Personality and Social
y
Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N.
(2005). Chewing on it can chew you up: Effects of rumination on triggered dis-
an
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.969
er
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality
Am
90074-7
by
Carnagey, N. L., Anderson, C. A., & Bartholow, B. D. (2007). Media violence and
social neuroscience: New questions and new opportunities. Current Directions in
11
Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal
cortex: Who’s in control? Nature Neuroscience, 3, 421–423. doi:10.1038/74783
©
Cohen, R. A., Paul, R., Zawacki, T. M., Moser, D. J., Sweet, L., & Wilkinson, H.
ht
38–50. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.38
yr
Dao-Castellana, M. H., Samson, Y., Legault, F., Martinot, J. L., Aubin, H. J., Crouzel,
op
Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., & Larson, C. L. (2000, July). Dysfunction in the
neural circuitry of emotion regulation: A possible prelude to violence. Science,
289, 591–594. doi:10.1126/science.289.5479.591
Denson, T. F., Fabiansson, E. C., Creswell, J. D., & Pedersen, W. C. (2009). Experimen-
tal effects of rumination styles on salivary cortisol responses. Motivation and
A)
Emotion, 33, 42–48. doi:10.1007/s11031-008-9114-0
P
Denson, T. F., Pedersen, W. C., & Miller, N. (2006). The Displaced Aggression Ques-
(A
tionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 1032–1051. doi:10.1037/
n
0022-3514.90.6.1032
io
Denson, T. F., Pedersen, W. C., Ronquillo, J., & Nandy, A. S. (2009). The angry
at
brain: Neural correlates of anger, angry rumination, and aggressive personality.
ci
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 734–744. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21051
so
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Stillman, T. F., & Gailliot, M. T. (2007). Violence
As
restrained: Effects of self-regulation and its depletion on aggression. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 62–76. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.12.005
al
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common
ic
neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8,
og
294–300. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010
ol
Eisenberger, N. I., Liberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003, October). Does rejec-
ch
tion hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292. doi:10.1126/
science.1089134
y
Ps
Grafman, J., Schwab, K., Warden, D., Pridgen, A., Brown, H. R., & Salazar, A. M.
(1996). Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: A report of the Vietnam
an
Harris, L. T., Todorov, A., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Attributions on the brain: Neuro-
er
implications for social policy (pp. 73–109). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
by
Inzlicht, M., & Gutsell, J. N. (2007). Running on empty: Neural signals for self-control
failure. Psychological Science, 18, 933–937. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02004.x
11
Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Brink, J., &
20
Krämer, U. M., Jansma, H., Tempelmann, C., & Münte, T. F. (2007). Tit-for-tat:
ig
j.neuroimage.2007.07.029
op
Kross, E., Egner, T., Ochsner, K., Hirsch, J., & Downey, G. (2007). Neural dynamics of
rejection sensitivity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 945–956. doi:10.1162/
C
jocn.2007.19.6.945
Lane, R. D., Fink, G. R., Chau, P. M.-L., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Neural activation
during selective attention to subjective emotional responses. Neuroreport, 8,
3969–3972. doi:10.1097/00001756-199712220-00024
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes.
Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.
A)
110405.085654
P
Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflexion and
(A
reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference.
n
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 199–249. doi:10.1016/S0065-
io
2601(02)80006-5
at
Lotze, M., Veit, R., Anders, S., & Birbaumer, N. (2007). Evidence for a different role
ci
of the ventral and dorsal medial PFC for social reactive aggression: An interactive
so
fMRI study. NeuroImage, 34, 470–478. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.028
As
MacDonald, K. B. (2008). Effortful control, explicit processing, and the regulation
of human evolved predispositions. Psychological Review, 115, 1012–1031.
al
doi:10.1037/a0013327
ic
Mathews, V. P., Kronenberger, W. G., Want, Y., Lurito, J. T., Lowe, M. J., & Dunn,
og
D. W. (2005). Media violence exposure and frontal lobe activation measure by
ol
functional magnetic resonance imaging in aggressive and nonaggressive adoles-
ch
cents. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, 29, 287–292. doi:10.1097/
01.rct.0000162822.46958.33
y
Ps
207–233. doi:10.3758/CABN.3.3.207
Murray, J. P., Liotti, M., Ingmundson, P. T., Mayberg, H. S., Pu, Y., Zamarripa,
ic
S1532785XMEP0801_3
Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking
e
Ochsner, K. N., Knierim, K., Ludlow, D. H., Hanelin, J., Ramachandran, T., Glover,
G., & Mackey, S. C. (2004). Reflection upon feelings: An fMRI study of neural
11
Raine, A., Buchsbaum, M., & LaCasse, L. (1997). Brain abnormalities in murderers
indicated by positron emission tomography. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 495–508.
C
doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00362-9
Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., LaCasse, L., & Colletti, P. (2000). Reduced prefrontal
gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality dis-
order. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 119–127. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.2.119
Raine, A., & Yang, Y. (2008). Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial
A)
behaviour. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 203–213.
Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, B., & Bushnell, M. C. (1997,
P
(A
August). Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somato-
sensory cortex. Science, 277, 968–971. doi:10.1126/science.277.5328.968
n
Ray, R. D., Ochsner, K. N., Cooper, J. C., Roberston, E. R., Gabrieli, J. D. E., &
io
Gross, J. J. (2005). Individual differences in trait rumination and the neural sys-
at
tems supporting cognitive reappraisal. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuro-
ci
science, 5, 156–168. doi:10.3758/CABN.5.2.156
so
Ray, R. D., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2008). All in the mind’s eye? Anger rumi-
As
nation and reappraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 133–145.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.133
al
Richeson, J. A., Baird, A. A., Gordon, H. L., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C. L.,
ic
Trawalter, S., & Shelton, J. N. (2003). An fMRI investigation of the impact of
og
interracial contact on executive function. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 1323–1328.
doi:10.1038/nn1156 ol
ch
Rusting, C. L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Regulating responses to anger: Effects
y
86–97. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.005
er
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Golub, A., & Cromwell, E. N. (2001). Development and vali-
dation of the Anger Rumination Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 31,
11
689–700. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00171-9
20
297–310. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1967.tb01430.x
ht
Twenge, J. M., Baumeister, R. F., Tice, D. M., & Stucke, T. S. (2001). If you can’t join
them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior. Journal of Per-
ig
Weber, R., Ritterfeld, U., & Mathiak, K. (2006). Does playing violent video games
induce aggression? Empirical evidence of a functional magnetic resonance imaging
C
II
A)
P
(A
GENETIC AND
n
io
ENVIRONMENTAL
at
ci
DETERMINANTS
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
12051-08_PT2-CH07-rev2.qxd
11
by
5/25/10
th
e
Am
11:38 AM
er
ic
an
Page 122
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-08_PT2-CH07-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:38 AM Page 123
A)
P
THE TRANSMISSION OF
(A
AGGRESSIVENESS ACROSS
n
io
GENERATIONS: BIOLOGICAL,
at
ci
CONTEXTUAL, AND SOCIAL
so
LEARNING PROCESSES
As
al
L. ROWELL HUESMANN, ERIC F. DUBOW, AND PAUL BOXER
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
sion within a generation across the life span. We note that this continuity is
due as much to the continuity of unaggressiveness as to the continuity of aggres-
e
social–cognitive model that has evolved to explain aggressive behavior and its
continuity in the life span, and we discuss how this model can be applied to
©
This research was supported in part by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health and the
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The authors wish to acknowledge the
C
contributions of Leonard Eron, Monroe Lefkowitz, and Leopold Walder to the Columbia County
Longitudinal Study.
123
12051-08_PT2-CH07-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:38 AM Page 124
A)
dinal study that shows that children’s aggressiveness after they grow up is
predicted by their parents’ own childhood aggressiveness and their parents’
P
(A
aggressiveness when the children were young or adults but not by their par-
ents’ adolescent aggressiveness. It is shown that continuity of aggression
n
within the life span plus transmission between generations during the criti-
io
cal childhood period of the second generation can adequately explain the
at
cross-generational relations. These findings are consistent with intergenera-
ci
tional transmission through genetic and biological predispositions and learn-
so
ing processes during the childhood years.
As
al
WITHIN-PERSON CONTINUITY OF AGGRESSIVE
ic
BEHAVIOR ACROSS THE LIFE SPAN
og
ol
One of the most consistently asked questions of prospective, longitudinal
ch
data is whether personality traits, behaviors, or other aspects of psychosocial
y
functioning remain stable over the life course. The continuity of intellec-
Ps
ior are also statistically predictable from childhood aggression and antisocial
er
Dubow, & Boxer, 2009; Huesmann, Eron, Lefkowitz, & Walder, 1984; Kokko,
Pulkkinen, Huesmann, Dubow, & Boxer, 2009; Loeber & Dishion, 1983;
e
Our most recent estimates are based on analyses of data from the Colum-
by
in 1982, and at age 48 in 2000. Their parents were also interviewed in 1960,
20
and 525 of their children were interviewed in 2000, providing data on three
generations. We call the main sample the Generation 2 (G2) sample, and
©
they have provided 40 years of data on continuity of aggression across the life
ht
span. When the continuity coefficients for 40 years are estimated with struc-
ig
tural equation models that control for measurement error and method vari-
yr
ance (see Huesmann et al., 2009), we obtain coefficients of .50 for males and
op
.42 for females. These approach the continuity of intellectual ability. The
consequences of such continuity can be serious. As Kokko et al. (2009)
C
recently showed with data from Finland and the United States, early aggres-
A)
they are to high aggressive children staying high on aggression throughout life
(Huesmann et al., 2009). To demonstrate this fact, we grouped participants
P
(A
based on age-8 peer-nominated aggression into “high” and “low” categories
using both median and one-third splits and examined how many stayed in the
n
same category over 40 years. For the full sample, 37% (52 of 141) of individu-
io
als who were low in third grade stayed low through late adolescence (i.e.,
at
age 19), through young adulthood (i.e., age 30), and into middle adulthood
ci
(i.e., age 48) on the composite measure of aggression. Of individuals who were
so
high in third grade, 35% (i.e., 31 of 89) stayed high through adolescence
As
and into middle adulthood. In other words, continuity was as much due to
low aggressives staying low as to high aggressives staying high. When we
al
inspected these patterns by gender, interesting differences became evident.
ic
For males, 38% of those who were low in childhood stayed low through age
og
48. Similarly, 36% of females who were low in childhood stayed low into
ol
middle adulthood. However, the differences were striking with regard to
ch
those classified as high on aggression. Among males, 47% of those who were
y
high in childhood stayed high into middle adulthood, whereas for females,
Ps
only 18% who were high in childhood continued to be high into middle
adulthood (χ [1] = 14.97, p < .001).
an
males and females places more pressure on females to reduce their aggres-
er
sion than it does on males. Early aggressiveness may also have more last-
Am
ing serious consequences for males. For example, Huesmann, Eron, and
Dubow (2002) found that early aggressiveness was the most important pre-
e
dictor of males being arrested by the time they were 30 years old, in com-
th
ior and is probably related to context (Broidy et al., 2003; Sameroff, Seifer,
ig
antisocial behavior that emerges early in life that has lasting negative con-
op
and is relatively short lived. This kind of aggression seems to have few long-
A)
Similar findings regarding continuity have been reported in studies of
other indicators of adjustment over time. For example, Helson, Jones, and
P
(A
Kwan (2002) demonstrated quadratic changes in various indicators of person-
ality functioning from early to late adulthood: Certain attributes such as
n
dominance and independence peaked in middle adulthood. In her influential
io
island of Kauai prospective study, Werner (2002) summarized long-term out-
at
comes for children and adolescents with behavior disorders: By age 40, only
ci
one third of those males and one fifth of those females exhibited continuing
so
difficulties (e.g., financial, marital, substance use). However, far less is
As
known about the degree of continuity of positive psychosocial adjustment
from childhood to adulthood, although this appears to be an emergent con-
al
cern of longitudinal researchers. As an example, with data from the Jyväsklyä
ic
Longitudinal Study, Pulkkinen and her colleagues demonstrated that pro-
og
social behavior in childhood predicted greater self-esteem and subjective
ol
well-being and shorter lived periods of unemployment in adulthood (Kokko
ch
& Pulkkinen, 2000; Pulkkinen, Nygren, & Kokko, 2002). Flouri and Buchanan
y
CONTINUITY OF AGGRESSION
Am
adjustment are relatively few and are limited primarily to examining contem-
20
published in the child development literature over the past decade (e.g.,
ht
Constantino, 1996; MacEwen, 1994). Those reviews indicated that most rel-
ig
from two generations. However, response bias problems cloud the interpretation
op
sion (Cairns, Cairns, Xie, Leung, & Hearne, 1998; Capaldi & Clark, 1998;
Serbin et al., 1998), and all reported modest to moderate cross-generational con-
tinuity. But those studies included only two generations.
More recently, four studies on cross-generational consistencies in par-
enting and in aggressive and antisocial behavior were published in a special
A)
issue of the Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology (Capaldi, Pears, Patterson,
& Owen, 2003; Conger, Neppl, Kim, & Scaramella, 2003; Hops, Davis,
P
(A
Leve, & Sheever, 2003; Thornberry, Freeman-Gallant, Lizotte, Krohn, &
Smith, 2003; see also Smith & Farrington, 2004). All four studies measured
n
parenting by two generations (G1 and G2) and aggressive behavior in two
io
generations (G2 and G3). Two of the four studies found significant inter-
at
generational continuity of aggression. The two that did not report such
ci
continuity had the smallest sample sizes. All four studies also reported inter-
so
generational continuity for some parenting factors relevant to aggression
As
(e.g., negative affect in parent–child interactions, consistency of discipline).
The results also suggested that child aggression mediates some of the conti-
al
nuity of parenting as well as parenting mediating some of the continuity of
ic
aggression. For example, Thornberry et al. (2003), using self-report question-
og
naire data, found a chain of relations for males from G1 parenting to G2
ol
aggression to G2 parenting to G3 aggression. For females, the pattern was
ch
similar, although the link from G1 parenting to G2 aggression was not sig-
y
nificant. Conger et al. (2003), using observational data, found both that G1
Ps
A plausible conclusion that can be drawn from these similar results in the
four studies is that parenting behavior and aggressive behavior seem to have
11
negative adjustment are transmitted from parents to children are not yet well
op
A)
mission of aggression, we need a model for the psychological processes
through which predisposing personal factors and precipitating situational fac-
P
(A
tors interact to determine whether a person behaves aggressively. The model
needs to include a representation of the enduring psychological structures that
n
control and influence these processes. Such a model has been provided by
io
Huesmann (1998) in a unified information-processing model for social prob-
at
lem solving. According to this model that integrates previous theorizing of
ci
Huesmann (1988), Bandura (1977), Dodge (1982), and Anderson (Anderson
so
& Huesmann, 2003), an individual’s emotional state and encoded schemas
As
about the world interact with situational cues to lead the individual to make
attributions about the situation. These attributions change the individual’s
al
emotional state and prime the activation of scripts for behaving. The scripts
ic
are filtered through a set of normative beliefs about appropriateness until a
og
script is accepted and followed.
ol
Within this model, the long-term determinants of aggressiveness
ch
include (a) encoded cognitions represented by schemas about the world,
y
(b) scripts for behavior, and (c) normative beliefs for filtering scripts, along
Ps
and the type of script an individual will retrieve to deal with a social situ-
Am
used as guides for behavior and social problem solving. Not all scripts that
th
occur to the child will be used. Before acting out the script, the child
by
normative beliefs and expectations about outcome must also have been
acquired over time.
©
the study of behavioral development for some time, particularly with regard
to developmental psychopathology (e.g., Cicchetti, Ackerman, & Izard,
C
A)
person (e.g., a parent) to another (e.g., the parent’s child)? Our position is
that there are four major processes by which this may occur that need to
P
(A
be considered: (a) through the transmission of genes that influence social
behavior, (b) by changes parents make in the child’s environment or through
n
continuity in the parent’s and child’s environment, (c) through children
io
observing parents’ behaviors, and (d) through conditioning of the children’s
at
behavior in which the parent participates.
ci
so
Genetic Influences on Aggression
As
Genetic influences are well established empirically (see Chapter 8).
al
Individual differences in emotional arousal, neurotransmitter levels, percep-
ic
tual biases, and other characteristics relevant to aggression seem to be influ-
og
enced by genes. The evidence for a heritable predisposition to aggression from
twin and adoption studies is impressive (Cloninger & Gottesman, 1987;
ol
Deater-Deckard & Plomin, 1999; Mednick, Gabrielli, & Hutchings, 1984).
ch
Miles and Carey (1997) performed a meta-analysis of 24 genetically infor-
y
mative studies that included twin and adoption designs and found significant
Ps
istic influences. Estimates from behavior genetic analyses of low shared environ-
mental variance depend on unlikely assumptions of genes being uncorrelated,
e
to look for the specific genes responsible (e.g., Chapter 8). Strategies based
on genome scans have proved disappointing in the search for loci that influ-
©
date genes have been surprisingly productive. Although methods are still
ig
and serotonin promoters have been confirmed and others are under study (for
a review, see Munafò et al., 2003). Studies by Caspi and colleagues (2002,
C
A)
of aggressive behavior (Raine, Brennan, & Mednick, 1995). Whatever their
source, a variety of individual differences in neurophysiology, neurotransmit-
P
(A
ters, hormones, and heart rate correlate with individual differences in early
aggressiveness (Knoblich & King, 1992; Olweus, Mattsson, Schaling, &
n
Loew, 1988; Raine & Jones, 1987).
io
at
Intergenerational Environmental Continuity and Change and Their Influences
ci
on Continuity and Change in Aggression
so
Many environmental influences on risk and resilience for aggression and
As
violence have been identified. These include stress, poverty, abuse, parental
al
rejection, peer behaviors, and religion, to name a few. To the extent that such
ic
environments are passed from parent to child, one can expect cross-generational
og
continuity. To the extent that such environments change from parent to child,
one can expect cross-generational discontinuity.
ol
A major question in developmental research concerns how changes
ch
over time in the social contexts people inhabit affect development (Higgins
y
& Parsons, 1983; Huesmann, Dubow, Eron, & Boxer, 2006; Sameroff, 1983).
Ps
from what a current parent experienced in interactions with his or her par-
er
ents to what the parent delivers to his or her child? Does continuity or change
Am
into trouble with the law, or lose their jobs. Evidence suggests that the stress
th
the strongest effects on the number of years of education the child attained
C
by early adulthood. This suggests that the environment in which the parent
lives and the child develops would be more important than the parent’s own
early environment.
A third issue of great relevance is the extent to which the larger histor-
ical context surrounding individual development influences the trajectories
A)
taken by cohorts embedded in those circumstances. As an example, what is
the differential impact of being an adolescent in a society that is at war as
P
compared with the same society experiencing relative peace, and how might
(A
the institution of selective or compulsory service magnify that impact?
n
These issues lead to a fourth broad contextual concern: major life tran-
io
sitions or turning points in individual development (Rönkä, Oravala, &
at
Pulkkinen, 2002; Rutter, 1996). Turning points are positive or negative events,
ci
over which the individual may or may not have control, which significantly
so
alter the life trajectory. According to Rutter (1996), an event can be a turn-
As
ing point only if it leads to an enduring, long-term modification of the trajec-
tory. Thus, turning points cannot be assessed contemporaneously for their
al
impact. Longitudinal data are necessary because often individuals do not rec-
ic
ognize turning points in their lives until some time has passed and the indi-
og
vidual has understood the importance and meaningfulness of the event.
ol
ch
Children Observing Parents’ Behaviors
y
In the short run, when children see their parents behave aggressively,
schemas, scripts, and normative beliefs associated with aggression are primed
e
are aroused in the child (e.g., anger may be stimulated). These processes alone
by
ulating processes make it likely that a child will behave aggressively after
observing his or her parents behaving aggressively.
©
encode into their repertoire of scripts the scripts they see their parents using,
they tend to adopt the world schemas they perceive their parents to be hold-
C
ing, and they tend to accept the normative beliefs of their parents about the
A)
ligence, in particular to language, culture, and the ability to understand other
minds” (Hurley & Chater, 2005, p. 1). Imitation of parents’ behaviors appears
P
(A
to be innate and occurs automatically in young primate infants (Meltzoff &
Moore, 1983). Specific mirror neurons seem to organize imitation in primate
n
brains (Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996). However, recent work
io
suggests that imitation goes far beyond the copying of specific sequences of
at
behaviors to the encoding of social cognitions fundamental to the control of
ci
social behavior (Meltzoff, 2007).
so
These observational learning processes are also involved in the develop-
As
ment of emotion regulation. Eisenberg, Cumberland, and Spinrad (1998)
described three processes by which emotion socialization leads to social com-
al
petence. First, through everyday family interactions, children display a wide
ic
range of both positive and negative emotions, thus providing parents with
og
numerous opportunities to react in both positive (e.g., encouraging) and neg-
ol
ative (e.g., punishing) ways. Second, when parents discuss various aspects of
ch
emotion (e.g., causes and consequences, emotional experience and regulation)
y
with their children, they serve as models for understanding and coping with
Ps
emotion. A child who receives a high degree of emotion knowledge from his
or her parents should be better equipped to contend with emotionally charged
an
tional regulation and, in turn, social behaviors, as well as taking into account
th
may also deliberately and directly reinforce their children for being aggressive
if the parents believe aggression is appropriate.
op
Parents who harshly punish and abuse their children may also classically
C
A)
Analysis of Intergenerational Continuity in
the Columbia County Longitudinal Study
P
(A
The first analyses of the four waves of data spanning three generations
n
from the Columbia County Longitudinal Study suggest that genetic and dis-
io
positional processes, learning processes operating during the child-rearing
at
years, and environmental continuities may all contribute to cross-generational
ci
continuity of aggressiveness. The sample we used in these analyses consists of
so
349 of the 551 G3 offspring who were interviewed when their G3 parent was
As
48 years old. The 349 represent one child of each G2 subject interviewed—
the youngest child for subjects with more than one child (in 202 families two
al
children were interviewed). The ages of the G3 sample at the time of the inter-
ic
view ranged from 6 to 33, with a median age of 18.
og
In these analyses, we assessed aggression in the G2 generation with the
ol
same composite measure we developed to assess continuity of aggression across
ch
the life span in the G2 generation. The indicators we used in that analysis of
y
four waves of G2 data were peer nominations at age 8 and 19; severe physical
Ps
aggression at ages 19, 30, and 48; and aggressive personality at ages 19, 30,
and 48. The “severe physical aggression” measure assessed how often the per-
an
son punched, choked, beat up, or used a knife or a gun against another person
ic
in the preceding year. The “aggressive personality” measure was the sum of the
er
Hathaway & McKinley, 1940). For the G3 data, we used the same two indi-
cators of adult aggression as with G2: aggressive personality and behaving
e
rately because the sample sizes are quite different for the two variables, given
by
that only those G3 offspring who were over 13 received the aggressive person-
ality assessment.
11
spring’s aggressive personality and severe physical aggression at the time the
ht
G2 parent was 48. For a subsample of 125 of the G3 offspring we had data on
ig
to a prediction equation.
TABLE 7.1
Correlations Between Generation G2’s (Parents’) Aggression at Four Ages
and Generation G3’s Aggression When G2 Parent Was Age 48
Child aggression when parent was age 48
A)
G3 child’s G3 child’s severe
P
aggressive physical
(A
G2 parent’s aggression personality aggression
at different ages (N = 212) (N = 348)
n
Age 8
io
G2’s composite aggression 0.24*** 0.09*
at
Age 19
ci
G2’s composite aggression 0.26*** 0.18***
so
Age 30
G2’s composite aggression 0.46*** 0.17***
As
Age 48
G2’s composite aggression 0.19*** 0.26***
al
* p < .10. ** p < .05. *** p < .01.
ic
og
ol
The correlations with G2s’ age-8 aggression are consistent with genetic
ch
and biological predisposition models of intergenerational transfer, whereas
y
the correlations between G2s’ later aggression and the G3 offsprings’ aggres-
Ps
are not large, an inspection of the correlations suggests that the strongest rela-
ic
tions with the offspring’s aggressive personality are from the parent’s aggres-
er
sion years earlier when the parent was a child, adolescent, or young adult.
Am
19, 30, and 48 (i.e., during the early child-rearing years). These results are
th
ment in which the offspring develops. The highest correlations with G3s’
aggressive personality around age 18 is with the parents’ age 30 aggressiveness
©
for both genders 18 years earlier. However, the highest correlations with G3s’
ht
age 48 aggressiveness.
yr
which displays the mean aggression scores for the offspring of G2 parents who
scored low (i.e., < 25th percentile), medium, or high (i.e., > 75th percentile)
C
0.6
G3 offspring's severe physically aggressive behavior
0.5
A)
when G2 parent was age 48
0.4
P
(A
Lo G2 Agg
0.3 Med G2 Agg
n
io
Hi G2 Agg
at
0.2
ci
so
0.1
As
al
0
ic
G2 parent's aggression at age 48
og
210
ol
ch
G3 offspring's aggressive personality when G2
200
Ps
190
an
180
parent was age 48
ic
Lo G2 Agg
er
170
Med G2 Agg
Am
160 Hi G2 Agg
150
e
th
140
by
130
11
120
20
Figure 7.1. In the upper panel, the relation between a parent’s age-48 aggression and
ht
their offspring’s concurrent severe physical aggression when the offspring averaged
18 years of age, F (2, 312) = 11.7, p < .001. In the lower panel, the relation between
ig
later (i.e., when the offspring averaged 18 years of age), F (2, 113) = 6.24, p < .01.
op
are significant and illustrate that it is the offspring of parents who were above
the 75th percentile on aggression at age 30 who are most at risk of develop-
ing an aggressive personality, and it is the offspring of parents who are above
the 75th percentile on aggression at age 48 who are most at risk of being seri-
A)
ously physically aggressive around age 18.
Given these relations, we decided to test a model of cross-generational
P
(A
transmission that combines continuity of aggression within the G2 generation
with transmission across generations only during the age 30 to age 48 period
n
when the G3 child is being reared. The model incorporated both the measure
io
of aggressive personality and the measure of severe physical aggression. This
at
two-generational structural model for continuity of aggression is shown in
ci
the left panel of Figure 7.2. The model assumes that the only path from gen-
so
eration G2 to generation G3 is the path from the aggression of the G2 parent
As
at age 30 to the aggression of the child 18 years later. The model fits the data well
with a nonsignificant chi-square statistic and other reasonable goodness-of-fit
al
statistics. The path from G2 aggression at age 30 to G3 aggression 18 years
ic
later is a highly significant and large effect.
og
We next expanded the model to incorporate all three generations we had
ol
studied. The results are shown in the right panel of Figure 7.2. Unfortunately,
ch
our only good measure of G1’s aggression is the individual’s tendency to hit
y
G2 when G2 was age 8. This measure is related to G2s’ aggression, but it prob-
Ps
ably represents G2s’ tendency to behave badly as well; thus, the path relating
it to G2s’ aggression at the same time is bidirectional. Still, the model fits the
an
data well, and the best estimates of the path coefficients for the model suggest
ic
improve the fit of the model, indicating that the strong relation between G2s’
age 48 aggression and G3s’ concurrent severe physical aggression (see Table 7.1
e
and Figure 7.1) is a consequence of the stability of G2s’ aggression through the
th
child-rearing years. Thus, the estimated path coefficients are consistent with
by
SUMMARY
ht
ig
We elaborated a model that takes into consideration all of these factors, and
Sev Phys Agg MMPI F49 Sev Phys Agg MMPI F49
.63# .51# .63# .51#
A)
G2 SUBJECT
P
G2 Agg 8 G2 Agg 19 G2 Agg 30 G2 Agg 48
(A
.64*** .86*** .80***
n
.67# .67# .63# .51# .65***
io
PrAgg PrAgg Sev Phys Agg MMPI F49
at
G3 Agg
ci
.63# .51#
so
Sev Phys Agg MMPI F49
Chi-Sq(43) = 47.9, p > .28, RMSEA = .018, CFI = .989
As
G3 CHILD
al
ic
Continuity of Aggression Over 40 Years Across 3 Generations
og
ol
ch
G1 Par
Hit G2 G1 PARENT
y
Ps
.80#
Sev Phys Agg MMPI F49 Sev Phys Agg MMPI F49
G1 Agg
.63# .51# .63# .51#
an
.13
.25*
ic
G2 SUBJECT
er
G3 Agg
.63# .51#
by
G3 CHILD
20
Figure 7.2. In the upper panel, the continuity of aggression over 40 years across 2 gen-
©
erations (G2 and G3). X2 (43) = 47.9, p > .28, RMSEA = .018, Cumulative Fit Index =
.99. In the lower panel, the continuity of aggression over 40 years across 3 generations
ht
(G1, G2, and G3). X2 (52) = 56.1, p > .32, RMSEA = .015, Goodness of Fit Index =
ig
.991. # indicates that the measurement parameter was fixed at the value estimated in
the within-generation model. Correlated errors were specified for the same measures
yr
across ages. PrAgg = peer nominated aggression; Sev Phys Agg = severe physical
op
aggression; MMPI F49 = the sum of scales F, 4, and 9 on the Minnesota Multiphasic
Personality Inventory; G1 Par Hit G2 = the amount of hitting of G2 child at age 8 by
C
the G1 parents; G2 Agg ‘n’ = G2’s composite aggression score at age ‘n’.
*p<.05 **p<.01 ***p<.001.
A)
A model fits the data well in which the cross-generational effects across two
and three generations are mostly mediated by the parent’s aggression during
P
(A
the child-rearing period.
n
io
REFERENCES
at
ci
Anderson, C. A., & Huesmann, L. R. (2003). Human aggression: A social–cognitive
so
view. In M. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 296–323).
As
London, England: Sage.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
al
Bergman, L. R., & Andershed, A. (2009). Predictors and outcomes of persistent or age-
ic
limited registered criminal behavior: A 30-year longitudinal study of a Swedish
og
urban population. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 164–178. doi:10.1002/ab.20298
ol
Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K. A.,
ch
. . . Vitaro, F. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive
behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study. Develop-
y
Ps
0012-1649.34.6.1162
Am
Capaldi, D. M., Pears, K. C., Patterson, G. R., & Owen, L. D. (2003). Continuity of
parenting practices across generations in an at-risk sample: A prospective com-
by
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., . . . Poulton, R.
20
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington,
ht
science.1083968
op
Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B. P., & Izard, C. E. (1995). Emotions and emotion regu-
lation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 7,
C
1–10. doi:10.1017/S0954579400006301
A)
Conger, R. D., Neppl, T., Kim, K. J., & Scaramella, L. (2003). Angry and aggressive
behavior across three generations: A prospective, longitudinal study of parents
P
and children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 143–160. doi:10.1023/
(A
A:1022570107457
n
Constantino, J. N. (1996). Intergenerational aspects of the development of aggres-
io
sion: A preliminary report. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
at
17, 176–182. doi:10.1097/00004703-199606000-00006
ci
Deater-Deckard, K., & Plomin, R. (1999). An adoption study of the etiology of
so
teacher and parent reports of externalizing behavior problems in middle child-
As
hood. Child Development, 70, 144–154. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00011
Dodge, K. A. (1982). Social cognition and children’s aggressive behavior. Child
al
Development, 53, 620–635. doi:10.2307/1129373
ic
Dubow, E. F., Huesmann, L. R., & Boxer, P. (2003). Theoretical and methodologi-
og
cal considerations in cross-generational research on parenting and child aggres-
ol
sive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 185–192. doi:10.1023/
ch
A:1022526325204
y
Farrington, D. P. (2003). Key results from the first forty years of the Cambridge Study
Am
limited, late-onset, and persistent offenders from age 8 to age 48. Aggressive
Behavior, 35, 150–163. doi:10.1002/ab.20296
11
Flouri, E., & Buchanan, A. (2002). What predicts good relationships with parents in
adolescence and partners in adult life: Findings from the 1958 British birth cohort.
20
Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to con-
ht
duct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 54–68.
doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6
ig
yr
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the
premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609. doi:10.1093/brain/119.2.593
op
Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1940). The MMPI manual. New York, NY:
C
Psychological Corporation.
Helson, R., Jones, C., & Kwan, V. S. Y. (2002). Personality change over 40 years
of adulthood: Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of two longitudinal sam-
ples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 752–766. doi:10.1037/
0022-3514.83.3.752
Higgins, E. T., & Parsons, J. E. (1983). Social cognition and the social life of the
A)
child: Stages as subcultures. In E. T. Higgins, D. W. Ruble, & W. W. Hartup
P
(Eds.), Social cognition and social behavior: Developmental issues (pp. 15–62). New
(A
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
n
Hops, H., Davis, B., Leve, C., & Sheeber, L. (2003). Cross-generational transmission
io
of aggressive parent behavior: A prospective, mediational examination. Journal
at
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 161–169. doi:10.1023/A:1022522224295
ci
Huesmann, L. R. (1988). An information processing model for the development of
so
aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 14, 13–24.
Huesmann, L. R. (1998). The role of social information processing and cognitive
As
schema in the acquisition and maintenance of habitual aggressive behavior. In
R. G. Geen & E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human aggression: Theories, research, and
al
implications for social policy (pp. 73–109). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
ic
og
Huesmann, L. R., Dubow, E. F., Eron, L. D., & Boxer, P. (2006). Middle childhood
family-contextual and personal factors as predictors of adult outcomes. In A. C.
ol
Huston & M. N. Ripke (Eds.), Developmental contexts in middle childhood: Bridges
ch
to adolescence and adulthood (pp. 62–86). New York, NY: Cambridge University
y
Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511499760.005
Ps
Huesmann, L. R., Dubow, E. F., & Boxer, P. (2009). Continuity of aggression from
childhood to early adulthood as a predictor of life outcomes: Implications for the
an
136–149. doi:10.1002/ab.20300
er
Huesmann, L. R., Eron, L. D., & Dubow, E. F. (2002). Childhood predictors of adult
Am
Huesmann, L. R., & Guerra, N. G. (1997). Children’s normative beliefs about aggres-
sion and aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
11
408–419. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.408
20
Huesmann, L. R., & Kirwil, L. (2007). Why observing violence increases the risk of vio-
lent behavior in the observer. In D. J. Flannery, A. T. Vazsonyi, & I. D. Waldman
©
(Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior and aggression (pp. 545–570).
ht
Hurley, S., & Chater, N. (Eds.). (2005). Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience
yr
A)
sity of aggression in childhood as a predictor of different forms of adult aggres-
sion: A two-country (Finland and United States) analysis. Journal of Research on
P
Adolescence, 19, 9–34. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00579.x
(A
Loeber, R., & Dishion, T. (1983). Early predictors of male delinquency: A review.
n
Psychological Bulletin, 94, 68–99. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.94.1.68
io
MacEwen, K. E. (1994). Refining the intergenerational transmission hypothesis.
at
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 9, 350–365. doi:10.1177/088626094009003005
ci
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Like me: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental
so
Science, 10, 226–134. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.x
As
Mednick, S. A., Gabrielli, W. F., Jr., & Hutchings, B. (1984, May). Genetic influ-
ences in criminal convictions: Evidence from an adoption cohort. Science, 224,
al
891–894. doi:10.1126/science.6719119
ic
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial ges-
og
tures. Child Development, 54, 702–709. doi:10.2307/1130058
ol
Miles, D. R., & Carey, G. (1997). Genetic and environmental architecture of human
ch
aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 207–217. doi:10.1037/
y
0022-3514.72.1.207
Ps
0033-295X.100.4.674
ic
Munafò, M. R., Clark, T. G., Moore, L. R., Payne, E., Walton, R., & Flint, J. (2003).
er
Olweus, D., Mattsson, A., Schaling, D., & Loew, H. (1988). Circulating testosterone
by
Pulkkinen, L., Nygren, H., & Kokko, K. (2002). Successful development: Childhood
yr
Raine, A., Brennan, P., & Mednick, S. A. (1995). Birth complications combined
C
with early maternal rejection at age 1 year predispose to violent crime at age
A)
583–599. doi:10.1007/BF00917243
Rönkä, A., Oravala, S., & Pulkkinen, L. (2002). “I met this wife of mine and things
P
(A
got on a better track”: Turning points in risk development. Journal of Adoles-
cence, 25, 47–63. doi:10.1006/jado.2001.0448
n
Rutter, M. (1996). Transitions and turning points in developmental psychopathology:
io
As applied to the age span between childhood and mid-adulthood. International
at
Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 603–626. doi:10.1080/016502596385712
ci
Rutter, M. (1998). Some research considerations on intergenerational continuities
so
and discontinuities: Comment on the special section. Developmental Psychology,
As
34, 1269–1273. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1269
Sameroff, A. J. (1983). Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution. In
al
W. Kessen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. History, theory, and methods
ic
(pp. 237–294). New York, NY: Wiley.
og
Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., Baldwin, A., & Baldwin, C. (1993). Stability of intelli-
ol
gence from preschool to adolescence: The influence of social and family risk
ch
factors. Child Development, 64, 80–97. doi:10.2307/1131438
y
Serbin, L. A., Cooperman, J. M., Peters, P. L., Lehoux, P. M., Stack, D. M., &
Ps
1649.34.6.1246
er
parenting across three generations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43,
230–247.
by
Thornberry, T. P., Freeman-Gallant, A., Lizotte, A. J., Krohn, M., & Smith, C. A.
11
Werner, E. E. (2002). Looking for trouble in paradise: Some lessons learned from the
©
A)
P
GENETIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL
(A
INFLUENCES ON AGGRESSION
n
io
at
ci
SOO HYUN RHEE AND IRWIN D. WALDMAN
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
and antisocial behavior, and it has relied on many different theoretical and
er
relative magnitudes. More than 100 twin and adoption studies of antisocial
th
behavior have been published, which led us (Rhee & Waldman, 2002) to
by
including aggression.
20
ogy (e.g., Robins, 1966), coercive parenting styles (e.g., Patterson, Reid, &
ht
Dishion, 1992), physical abuse (Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990), and family
ig
yr
op
This work was supported in part by NIDA DA-13956 to Soo Hyun Rhee and NIMH MH-01818 to
Irwin D. Waldman. Earlier versions of this chapter were presented at the meeting of the American
C
Society of Criminology in 1996 and the meeting of the Behavior Genetics Association in 1997, and a
more extensive version has been published in Psychological Bulletin, 128, pp. 490–529.
143
12051-09_CH08-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:38 AM Page 144
conflict (e.g., Norland, Shover, Thornton, & James, 1979), are significantly
associated with antisocial behavior. Often, these variables are considered to
be environmental influences, and the possibility that they may also reflect
genetic influences is not considered. This is unfortunate because disentan-
A)
gling the influences of nature and nurture is the first step toward reaching the
eventual goal of explaining the specific etiology of antisocial behavior (see
P
(A
Chapter 7, this volume). Also, estimating the relative magnitude of genetic
and environmental influences on antisocial behavior is an important step in
n
the search for specific candidate genes and environmental risk factors under-
io
lying antisocial behavior. Although it is not possible to disentangle genetic
at
from environmental influences in family studies because genetic and environ-
ci
mental influences are confounded in nuclear families, twin and adoption
so
studies have the unique ability to disentangle genetic and environmental
As
influences and to estimate the magnitude of both simultaneously.
Twin studies can disentangle genetic and environmental influences on
al
a trait by comparing the similarity between monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs,
ic
who are genetically identical, with the similarity between dizygotic (DZ) twin
og
pairs, who share 50% of their genes, on average. Traits with genetic influences
ol
will show greater similarity between MZ twins than between DZ twins. Adop-
ch
tion studies demonstrate (a) genetic influences on a trait if there is a significant
y
correlation between the trait in adoptees and their biological relatives and
Ps
and 9) and explore how the pattern and magnitude of these influences com-
Am
pare with those on antisocial behavior in general. In the twin and adoption
studies reviewed here, aggression is usually studied as a personality character-
e
istic and assessed with such measures as the Adjective Checklist (Gough &
th
1966) to observations of the number of times a child hits a Bobo doll (Plomin,
20
Foch, & Rowe, 1981). For the present review, the operationalization of aggres-
sion was restricted to behavior that meets the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
©
for conduct disorder (e.g., bullying, initiating physical fights, using a weapon
ig
that can cause serious physical harm). Toward the end of the chapter, we qual-
yr
A)
examining the PsycINFO and Medline databases. The search terms used in this
process were aggressive, aggression, crime, criminality, delinquent, or delinquency
P
(A
in combination with the terms twin(s), adoptee(s), adoptive, genetic, genetics,
genes, environmental, and environment. We examined the references cited in
n
research articles and review articles found in the search process, to locate any
io
additional studies that might have been missed or published before the databases
at
were established.
ci
so
Inclusion Criteria
As
After excluding unsuitable studies according to the criteria described
al
later (i.e., construct validity, inability to calculate tetrachoric or intraclass cor-
ic
relations, and assessment of related disorders) and addressing the problem of
og
nonindependence in these studies, 14 studies examining aggression remained.
ol
Table 8.1 lists the behavior genetic studies examining aggression included in
ch
the meta-analysis. Also listed are the method of assessment and method of zygos-
y
ity determination (in twin studies) used in each study, the mean or median age
Ps
of the sample, the sex of the participants, the number of pairs, the relationship
of the pairs, and the effect sizes.
an
ic
Construct Validity
er
included because it was not clear whether they examined aggression or some
related but distinct trait. An additional study (Partanen et al., 1966) was excluded
by
because the aggression items used by the authors (e.g., “Are you readily insulted?”
and “Do you easily become unhappy about even small things?”) suggested that
11
negative affect or anger, rather than aggression per se, was being assessed.
20
The effect sizes used in this meta-analysis were the Pearson product-
ig
centages reported in the studies. These effect sizes were analyzed using model-
fitting programs that estimate the relative contribution of genetic and
C
146
12051-09_CH08-rev2.qxd
20
11
5/25/10
by
TABLE 8.1
Effect Sizes for Behavior Genetic Studies Examining Aggression
th
Study
e
Assessment Zygosity Age Sex N Relationship Effect size
11:38 AM
Midwest twins (Cates, Houston, Self-report Blood grouping/ 42.50 F–F 77 MZ .07 (assault)
Am
21 DZ .01 (indirect)
an
California twins (Ghodsian-Carpey Parent report Questionnaire 5.20 Both–both 21 MZ .78
& Baker, 1987) 17 DZ .31
Ps
London twins (adults, 1970s; Reaction to stimuli y 30.50 Both–both 49 MZ .59
Wilson, Rust, & Kasriel, 1977) 52 DZ .34
London twins (adults, 1980s; Self-report Blood grouping/ 30.00 M–M 90 MZ .33
ch
Rushton, Fulker, Neale, Nias, questionnaire
ol 46 DZ .16
& Eysenck, 1986) F–F 206 MZ .43
133 DZ .00
og
ic M–F 98 DZ .12
Minnesota twins (reared together, Self-report Blood grouping 21.65 Both–both 217 MZ .43
1970s; Tellegen et al., 1988) 114 DZ .14
al
Minnesota twins (1990s, adults; Self-report Blood grouping/ 37.76 M–M As 220 MZ .37
Finkel & McGue, 1997) questionnaire 165 DZ .12
F–F 406 MZ .39
so
352 DZ .14
M–F 114 DZ .12
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
Boston twins (children; Scarr, 1966) Parent report Blood grouping 8.08 F–F 24 MZ .35
12051-09_CH08-rev2.qxd
20 28 DZ −.08
Philadelphia twins (Meininger, Teacher report Blood grouping 8.50 Both–both 61 MZ .67
Hayman, Coates, & Gallagher, 11 34 DZ .11
1988)
Missouri twins (Owen & Sines, 1970) Reaction to stimuli Blood grouping 10.00 M–M 10 MZ .09
5/25/10
11 DZ
by
−.24
th F–F 11 MZ .58
e 13 DZ .22
Colorado twins (1980s; Plomin, Objective test Questionnaire 7.60 Both–both 53 MZ .42
Foch, & Rowe, 1981) 32 DZ .42
11:38 AM
California twins (Rahe, Hervig, & Self-report Blood grouping 48.00 M–M 82 MZ .31
Am
Rosenman, 1978) 79 DZ .21
British Columbia twins (Blanchard, Self-report 36.18 Both–both 96 MZ .59
er
Vernon, & Harris, 1995)
ic 48 DZ .34
Page 147
Dutch twins (van den Oord, Verhulst, Parent report anBlood grouping 3.00 M–M 210 MZ .81
& Boomsma, 1996) (questionnaire 265 DZ .49
F–F 236 MZ .83
238 DZ .49
Ps
y M–F 409 DZ .45
Swedish Twins (adults; Gustavsson, Self-report ch Both–both 15 MZ ra .22 (indirect)
Pedersen, Åsberg, & Schalling, 26 MZ .41 (indirect)
1996) 29 DZ .27 (indirect)
ol
og 15 MZ ra −.03 (verbal)
26 MZ .22 (verbal)
29 DZ .23 (verbal)
ic
Note. M = male; F = female; Both = both male and female; MZ = monozygotic twin pairs; DZ = dyzygotic twin pairs; MZ ra = monzygotic twin pairs reared apart.
al
147
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-09_CH08-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:38 AM Page 148
Nonindependent Samples
Another justification for exclusion from the meta-analysis was nonin-
dependent sampling. Several effect sizes from studies in the original reference
list were based on nonindependent samples because researchers examined
A)
more than one dependent measure of antisocial behavior in their sample or
P
published follow-up data from the same sample in separate publications. Sev-
(A
eral suggestions for dealing with nonindependent samples have been offered
in the meta-analytic literature (Mullen, 1989; Rosenthal, 1991). For exam-
n
io
ple, Mullen (1989) gave four options for dealing with this problem: choosing
at
the best dependent measure, averaging the effect sizes of the different depen-
ci
dent measures, conducting separate meta-analyses for each of the dependent
so
measures, or using nonindependent samples as if they were independent sam-
ples (the least recommended approach). We did not follow the option of
As
choosing the best dependent measure unless one of the dependent measures
al
did not fulfill the inclusion criteria described previously, making the decision
ic
easy. Taking this option would have required making subjective choices
og
because we were aware of the effect sizes associated with each of the depen-
dent measures. The option of conducting separate meta-analyses for each of
ol
the dependent measures was not chosen simply because it was impractical:
ch
There were many effect sizes from nonindependent samples. The most prac-
y
tical and prudent option was to average the effect sizes from nonindependent
Ps
samples.
an
ple size was identical across the nonindependent samples. If the sample size was
er
not identical across the nonindependent samples, we used the effect size from
Am
follow-up analyses), we chose the effect size estimated from the largest sam-
th
used in the same sample (e.g., when the author of one publication examined
more than one dependent measure or authors of different publications exam-
11
ined different dependent measures in one sample), the effect sizes were aver-
20
aged if the sample size was the same across the nonindependent samples, and
the effect size from the largest sample was used if the sample size differed across
©
correlations, which were the effect sizes used from these studies in the meta-
A)
transformed into a contingency table, which was then used to estimate the
tetrachoric correlation (i.e., the correlation between the latent continuous
P
(A
variables that are assumed to underlie the observed dichotomous variables),
which was the effect size used in the meta-analysis. For some studies, we directly
n
estimated the tetrachoric correlation from the raw data because it had to be esti-
io
mated from contingency tables. For these studies, we were also able to estimate
at
the weight matrix (i.e., the asymptotic covariance matrix of the correlation
ci
matrix). If the weight matrix can be estimated, it is possible to use weighted
so
least squares estimation in the model-fitting analyses, which is more appropri-
As
ate for nonnormally distributed variables (e.g., diagnoses) than maximum
likelihood estimation.
al
ic
Model-Fitting Analyses
og
ol
The magnitude of additive and nonadditive genetic influences (a2 and
ch
2
d ) constitutes the proportion of variance in the liability for aggression that
y
tive, the effects of alleles from different loci are independent and “add up” to
influence the liability underlying a trait. If genetic influences are nonadditive,
an
the alleles interact with each other to influence the liability for a trait, either
ic
Many studies report the magnitude of additive and nonadditive genetic influ-
Am
in common and make family members similar to one another, whereas non-
by
and make family members different from one another. It is important to note
20
alternative models containing different sets of causal influences for their fit to
ig
models posit that aggression is caused by the types of influences described pre-
op
ences (E). In the present meta-analysis, we contrasted the fit of the ACE
model, the AE model, the CE model, and the ADE model. We assessed the
fit of each model, as well as of competing models, using both the chi-square sta-
tistic and the Akaike information criterion (AIC), a fit index that reflects both
the fit of the model and its parsimony (Loehlin, 1992). The AIC has been used
A)
extensively in both the structural equation modeling and behavior genetic lit-
eratures. Among competing models, that with the lowest AIC and the lowest
P
χ2 relative to its degrees of freedom is considered to be the best-fitting model.
(A
Unfortunately, it is not possible to estimate c2 and d2 simultaneously or
n
test an ACDE model with data only from twin pairs reared together because
io
the estimation of c2 and d2 both rely on the same information (i.e., the differ-
at
ence between the MZ and DZ twin correlations). If the DZ correlation is
ci
greater than half the MZ correlation, the ACE model is the correct model,
so
and the estimate of d2 in the ADE model is always zero. If the DZ correlation
As
is less than half the MZ correlation, however, the ADE model is the correct
model, and the estimate of c2 in the ACE model is always zero.
al
ic
Meta-Analytic Findings
og
ol
The ACE model was the best-fitting model for aggression (a2 = .44, c2 =
ch
.06, e = .50), although the fit of this model was close to that of the AE model,
2
y
ies of antisocial behavior there were moderate additive genetic (a2 = .32),
er
nonadditive genetic (d2 = .09), shared environmental (c2 = .16), and non-
Am
shared environmental (e2 = .43) influences. These results suggest that the
magnitude of genetic and nonshared environmental influences is slightly
e
higher for aggression than for antisocial behavior in general and that evidence
th
between overt and relational aggression (Crick, Casa, & Mosher, 1997; Crick
ht
& Grotpeter, 1995) because there are no published twin or adoption studies
ig
age or the threat thereof, whereas relational aggression harms others by dam-
op
harm the victims, it has serious consequences for both the aggressors (e.g.,
A)
ining sex differences in aggression and its causes, given that females are sig-
nificantly more relationally aggressive and less overtly aggressive than males
P
(A
(Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Crick et al., 1997). Given the evidence that overt
and relational aggression are correlated but distinct (Crick et al., 1997),
n
behavior genetic studies are necessary to determine the degree of genetic and
io
environmental influences that are common to both overt and relational
at
aggression rather than being specific to each.
ci
Similarly, few behavior genetic studies have distinguished between reac-
so
tive and proactive aggression (Dodge, Lochman, Harnish, Bates, & Pettit,
As
1997; Vitaro, Brendgen, & Tremblay, 2002; Waschbusch, Willoughby, &
Pelham, 1998). Reactive aggression is characterized by impulsive “hot-blooded”
al
anger, appears to be a response to frustration or perceived threat, and is asso-
ic
ciated with lack of self-control. In contrast, proactive aggression is premedi-
og
tated and “cold-blooded,” less emotional, and more likely to be driven by
ol
expected rewards (Dodge et al., 1997). Evidence suggests that reactively and
ch
proactively aggressive children differ in developmental histories, adjustment,
y
aggression are distinct (Dodge et al., 1997; Vitaro et al., 2002; see also Chap-
ter 9, this volume). Therefore, behavior genetic studies of antisocial behavior
an
variate studies should examine the etiology of the overlap between different
er
have recently made important strides along these lines. In addition, several
behavior genetic studies examining the development of aggression have been
e
conducted.
th
by
old twin pairs from Quebec. The magnitude of genetic influences was similar
for proactive (h2 = .41, e2 = .59) and reactive aggression (h2 = .39, e2 = .61).
ig
yr
The correlation between proactive and reactive aggression ranged from .51
op
to .60 in this sample, with the correlation between genetic influences on the
two types of aggression being .87, and the correlation between nonshared
C
environmental influences being .34. Brendgen et al. (2006) also found that
most of the association between proactive and reactive aggression was due to
common etiological factors influencing physical aggression.
The second study examining proactive and reactive aggression was
Baker, Raine, Liu, and Jacobson’s (2008) study of 1,219 nine- to 10-year-old
A)
twins from southern California. Results were presented separately for child,
mother, and teacher report, because the correlation between these sources
P
(A
was low (.18 to .26). Baker et al. found a significant sex difference in the mag-
nitude of genetic and environmental influences on aggression according to
n
child reports, such that moderate genetic influences were present for boys but
io
not girls, whereas moderate shared environmental influences were present
at
for girls but not boys (boys: h2 = .38 and e2 = .62 for reactive aggression and
ci
h2 = .50 and e2 = .50 for proactive aggression; girls: c2 = .36 and e2 = .64 for
so
reactive aggression and c2 = .14 and e2 = .86 for proactive aggression). In
As
contrast, no differences were found for mother reports (h2 = .26, c2 = .27, and
e2 = .46 for reactive aggression and h2 = .32, c2 = .21, and e2 = .47 for proac-
al
tive aggression) or teacher reports (h2 = .20, c2 = .43, and e2 = .37 for reactive
ic
aggression and h2 = .45, c2 = .14, and e2 = .41 for proactive aggression). The
og
phenotypic correlation between proactive and reactive aggression ranged
ol
from .46 to .80. The authors suggest that both common genetic and environ-
ch
mental influences are responsible for this correlation, but results varied across
y
the three sources (child report: rg = .57 and re = .46 for boys, rc = .53 and re =
Ps
.53 for girls; mother report: rg = .76, rc = .76, re = .43; teacher report: rg = 1.0,
rc = 1.0, re = .53).
an
aggression is that between overt and relational aggression (Crick & Grotpeter,
er
1995; Crick et al., 1997). Two recent twin studies have examined the associ-
Am
ation between relational and overt aggression. Brendgen et al. (2005) exam-
ined the association between physical aggression (i.e., overt aggression) and
e
from Quebec. Teacher and peer reports of physical and social aggression were
by
aggression; h2 = .20, c2 = .20, e2 = .60 for social aggression; peer report: h2 = .54,
e2 = .46 for physical aggression; h2 = .23, c2 = .23, e2 = .54 for social aggres-
20
sion). The phenotypic correlation between physical and social aggression was
©
.43 for teacher ratings and .41 for peer ratings, and there was evidence of com-
ht
Ligthart et al. (2005) examined relational versus direct (i.e., overt) aggres-
op
both relational and direct aggression and found evidence of significant sex dif-
A)
correlation between relational and direct aggression was .58 for boys and .47
P
(A
for girls, and this correlation was due to both common genetic and shared
environmental influences (55% genetic, 33% shared environmental, and 12%
n
nonshared environmental influences in boys; 58% genetic, 30% shared envi-
io
ronmental, and 12% nonshared environmental influences in girls).
at
ci
The Overlap Between Aggression and
so
Other Types of Antisocial Behavior
As
Several studies have examined differences in the etiology of aggressive
al
and nonaggressive antisocial behavior. As shown in Table 8.2, although the
ic
results from these studies are not uniform, several general conclusions can
og
be drawn from them. First, although most studies reported similar, moder-
ol
ate heritabilities for both types of antisocial behavior, some studies (e.g.,
ch
Eley, Lichtenstein, & Stevenson, 1999) suggest that aggressive behavior is
y
c2 = .18, e2 = .34).
th
sive antisocial behavior was moderate in most studies (e.g., .32 in Gelhorn
et al., 2006, and .48 to .76 in Bartels et al., 2003). Third, most studies suggest
11
Lichtenstein, & Moffitt, 2003), which also had genetic influences. However,
ig
hood assessed via parent report and nonaggressive antisocial behavior in ado-
lescent assessed via self-report was lower (r = .07 to .15 in Tuvblad, Eley, &
C
Lichtenstein, 2005).
154
12051-09_CH08-rev2.qxd
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:38 AM
TABLE 8.2
Am
Bartels (2003) Parent report of aggressive/ h2 = .69, c2 = .00, h2 = .79, c2 = .00, h2 = .80, c2 = .02-.06,
rule breaking behavior e2 = .31 (males) e2 = .21 (males) e2 = .12-.14
Ps
h2 = .72, c2 = .00,y h2 = .56, c2 = .23,
e2 = .28 (females) e2 = .21 (females)
Button (2004) Self-report of aggressive h2 = .58-.68
ch h2 = .59-.74 h2 = .87, c2 = .00, e2 = .13
and nonaggressive c2 = .00-.09 ol c2 = .00-.14
antisocial behavior e2 = .32-.33 e2 = .26-.27
Eley (1999) Parent report of aggressive h2 = .55, c2 = .18,
og
h2 = .30, c2 = .44, h2 = .38, c2 = .53,
and nonaggressive e2 = .27 (Swedish males) e2 = .26 (Swedish males)
ic e2 = .09 (Swedish males)
behavior 2
h = .42, c2 = .25, 2 2
h = .13, c2 = .54,
al h = .25, c2 = .56,
e2 = .33 (British males) e2 = .32 (British males) e2 = .19 (British males)
2
h = .76, c2 = .06, 2 2
h = .45, c2 = .34,
As h = .72, c2 = .18,
e2 = .19 (Swedish e2 = .22 (Swedish e2 = .10 (Swedish
females) females) females)
so
h2 = .71, c2 = .04, h2 = .42, c2 = .30, h2 = .88, c2 = .06,
e2 = .25 (British females) e2 = .28 (British females) e2 = .06 (British females)
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
C
op
yr
ig
ht
Eley (2003)
©
Parent report of aggressive h2 = .60, c2 = .15, h2 = .49, c2 = .35, h2 = .70, c2 = .28,
nonaggressive antisocial
12051-09_CH08-rev2.qxd
symptoms
Am
Tuvblad (2005) Parent report of aggressive h2 = .59, c2 = .18, h2 = .37, c2 = .30, h2 = 1.00 (girls)
behavior in childhood e2 = .23 (girls) e2 = .33 (girls)
er
and self-report of non- h2 = .67, c2 = .04,
ic h2 = .27, c2 = .43, h2 = .39, c2 = .47,
Page 155
155
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-09_CH08-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:38 AM Page 156
A)
ior is moderately stable from childhood to adolescence (for a similar conclu-
sion, see Chapter 7) and that genetic influences explain a larger percentage
P
(A
of the stability of aggressive behavior than do shared or nonshared environ-
mental influences. Van der Valk, Verhulst, Neale, and Boomsma (1998) con-
n
ducted a longitudinal study of aggressive behavior in 111 pairs of adopted
io
biological siblings, 221 pairs of adopted nonbiological siblings, and 1,484
at
adopted singletons from the Netherlands. Aggressive behavior was assessed
ci
via parent questionnaires at age 10 to 15 years, then again 3 years later. The
so
correlation between the two time points ranged from .51 to .70, and the
As
covariance between the two time points was due 69% to genetic influences,
14% to shared environmental influences, and 17% to nonshared environ-
al
mental influences. Van Beijsterveldt, Bartels, Hudziak, and Boomsma (2003)
ic
examined the stability of aggression at ages 3, 7, 10, and 12 in a large sample
og
of Dutch twin pairs (ranging from 1,509 pairs at age 12 to 6,488 pairs at age 3).
ol
Aggressive behavior was assessed via parent questionnaires. The correlations
ch
for aggression assessed at different ages ranged from .41 to .77. Genetic influ-
y
new influences, fit best for genetic influences. In contrast, there was a stable
er
environmental influences.
In a longitudinal twin study, Haberstick, Schmitz, Young, and Hewitt
e
early adolescence (through parent report at ages 7, 9, 10, 11, and 12 and
by
teacher report at ages 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, and 12) and reached conclusions slightly
different from those of van Beijsterveldt et al. (2003). The contribution of
11
87% for parent report and 59% to 95% for teacher report, and the rest was
attributable to nonshared environmental influences that were common across
©
that were common across age. The authors noted that there is consistent evi-
ig
despite the fact that different teachers rated the children at each age. Eley,
op
Lichtenstein, and Moffitt (2003) examined over 1,000 twin pairs from the
Swedish Twin Registry assessed at ages 8 to 9 and 13 to 14 years. Aggressive
C
behavior was assessed via parent report at both time points. Continuity in
aggression between childhood and adolescence ranged from .53 to .69 and
was due 84% to genetic influences, 8% to shared environmental influences,
and 8% to nonshared environmental influences.
A)
Candidate Genes for Aggression and Antisocial Behavior
P
(A
Based on the evidence that aggression is heritable, researchers have ini-
tiated attempts to find specific genes that contribute to its etiology using a
n
candidate gene approach. In well-designed candidate gene studies, genes are
io
selected based on the known or hypothesized involvement of their gene prod-
at
uct in the etiology of a trait or disorder (i.e., its pathophysiological function
ci
and etiological relevance). Whereas genome scans may be thought of as
so
exploratory searches for putative genes that underlie a disorder or trait, well-
As
conducted candidate gene studies are targeted tests of the role of specific
genes in the etiology of a disorder or trait, as the location, function, and eti-
al
ological relevance of candidate genes are most often known a priori.
ic
Genes underlying various aspects of the dopaminergic, noradrenergic,
og
and serotonergic neurotransmitter pathways represent viable candidates
ol
given the role of these neurotransmitter systems in the etiology and patho-
ch
physiology of aggression. For example, there is considerable overlap between
y
(ADHD; e.g., Lilienfeld & Waldman, 1990); thus, candidate genes for
ADHD may also be relevant candidates for aggression and antisocial behav-
an
ior. Several genes within the dopamine system appear to be risk factors for
ic
ADHD (Waldman & Gizer, 2006). Dopamine genes are plausible candi-
er
dates for ADHD, given that the stimulant medications that are the most
Am
frequent and effective treatments for ADHD appear to act primarily by reg-
ulating dopamine levels in the brain (Seeman & Madras, 1998; Solanto,
e
ine the behavioral effects of the deactivation of specific genes, have further
demonstrated the potential relevance of genes within these neurotransmit-
11
ter systems. Results of such studies have markedly strengthened the consid-
20
1996) and the dopamine receptor D3 and D4 genes (DRD3 and DRD4;
ht
Accili et al., 1996; Dulawa, Grandy, Low, Paulus, & Geyer, 1999; Rubin-
ig
stein et al., 1997), as well as genes within the serotonergic system, such as
yr
gic genes also are plausible candidates for aggression, given the demon-
strated relations between serotonergic function and aggression (Berman,
C
A)
acid, a serotonin metabolite, in aggressive or violent individuals (e.g., Brown,
Goodwin, Ballenger, Goyer, & Major,1979; Linnoila et al., 1983). Mice lack-
P
(A
ing the HTR1β gene show enhanced aggressive behavior (Saudou et al.,
1994), and a serotonin transporter (5HTT) polymorphism is associated with
n
aggression in nonhuman primates who experienced insecure early attach-
io
ment relationships (Suomi, 2003).
at
Candidate genes for neurotransmitter systems may include (a) precursor
ci
genes that affect the rate at which neurotransmitters are produced from pre-
so
cursor amino acids (e.g., tyrosine hydroxylase for dopamine, tryptophan
As
hydroxylase for serotonin), (b) receptor genes that are involved in receiving
neurotransmitter signals (e.g., genes corresponding to the five dopamine recep-
al
tors, DRD1, D2, D3, D4, and D5, and to the serotonin receptors, such as
ic
HTR1β and HTR2A), (c) transporter genes that are involved in the reuptake
og
of neurotransmitters back into the presynaptic terminal (e.g., the dopamine
ol
and serotonin transporter genes, DAT1 and 5HTT), (d) metabolite genes that
ch
are involved in the metabolism or degradation of these neurotransmitters
y
A and B), and (e) genes that are responsible for the conversion of one neuro-
transmitter into another (e.g., dopamine beta hydroxylase, or DβH, which
an
CONCLUSIONS
th
by
ining relational versus overt aggression and reactive versus proactive aggression
have recently been undertaken. The first set of these studies demonstrates
©
sion. An association between dopamine and serotonin and aggression has been
implicated in some early candidate gene studies, although human studies
C
examining the association between the serotonin transporter gene and vio-
A)
REFERENCES
P
(A
Note: References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in
n
the meta-analysis.
io
at
Accili, D., Fishburn, C. S., Drago, J., Steiner, H., Lachowicz, J. E., Park, B. H.,
ci
. . . Fuchs, S. (1996). A targeted mutation of the D3 dopamine receptor gene is
so
associated with hyperactivity in mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci-
As
ences of the United States of America, 93, 1945–1949. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.5.1945
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
al
disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
ic
Baker, L. A., Raine, A., Liu, J., & Jacobson, K. C. (2008). Differential genetic and envi-
og
ronmental influences on reactive and proactive aggression in children. Journal of
Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1265–1278. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9249-1
ol
ch
Bartels, M., Hudziak, J. J., van den Oord, E. J., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., Rietveld, M. J.,
& Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Co-occurrence of aggressive behavior and rule-
y
breaking behavior at age 12: Multi-rater analyses. Behavior Genetics, 33, 607–621.
Ps
doi:10.1023/A:1025787019702
an
*Blanchard, J. M., Vernon, P. A., & Harris, J. A. (1995). A behavior genetic inves-
Am
2005.00887.x
Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Dionne, G., & Pérusse, D. (2006). Examining
11
Button, T. M., Scourfield, J., Martin, N., & McGuffin, P. (2004). Do aggressive and
C
A)
Behavioral Medicine, 16, 237–256. doi:10.1007/BF00844758
Crick, N. R., Casa, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational and overt aggression in pre-
P
(A
school. Developmental Psychology, 33, 579–588. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.579
Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender, and social–
n
psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 710–722. doi:10.2307/1131945
io
at
Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1996). Children’s treatment by peers: Victims of
relational and overt aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 367–380.
ci
doi:10.1017/S0954579400007148
so
Dodge, K. A., Bates, J., & Pettit, G. S. (1990, December). Mechanisms in the cycle
As
of violence. Science, 250, 1678–1683. doi:10.1126/science.2270481
al
Dodge, K. A., Lochman, J. E., Harnish, J. D., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1997). Reac-
tive and proactive aggression in school children and psychiatrically impaired
ic
og
chronically assaultive youth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 37–51.
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.37
ol
Dulawa, S. C., Grandy, D. K., Low, M. J., Paulus, M. P., & Geyer, M. A. (1999).
ch
Dopamine D4 receptor-knock-out mice exhibit reduced exploration of novel
y
Eley, T. C., Lichtenstein, P., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). A longitudinal behavioral genetic
analysis of the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior. Devel-
an
Eley, T. C., Lichtenstein, P., & Stevenson, J. (1999). Sex differences in the etiology
er
of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior: Results from two twin stud-
Am
Gelhorn, H., Stallings, M., Young, S., Corley, R., Rhee, S. H., Christian, H., & Hewitt,
J. (2006). Common and specific genetic influences on aggressive and nonaggres-
11
sive conduct disorder domains. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Ado-
20
doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1987)13:4<173::AID-AB2480130402>3.0.CO;2-Y
ig
Giros, B., Jaber, M., Jones, S. R., Wightman, R. M., & Caron, M. G. (1996, Feb-
yr
*Gustavsson, J. P., Pedersen, N. L., Åsberg, M., & Schalling, D. (1996). Exploration
into the sources of individual differences in aggression-, hostility-, and anger-
related (AHA) personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 21,
1067–1071. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00146-8
A)
Haberstick, B. C., Schmitz, S., Young, S. E., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Genes and devel-
opmental stability of aggressive behavior problems at home and school in a com-
P
munity sample of twins aged 7–12. Behavior Genetics, 36, 809–819. doi:10.1007/
(A
s10519-006-9092-5
n
Ligthart, L., Bartels, M., Hoekstra, R. A., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2005).
io
Genetic contributions to subtypes of aggression. Twin Research and Human
at
Genetics, 8, 483–491. doi:10.1375/twin.8.5.483
ci
Lilienfeld, S. O., & Waldman, I. D. (1990). The relation between childhood attention-
so
deficit hyperactivity disorder and adult antisocial behavior reexamined: The
As
problem of heterogeneity. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 699–725. doi:10.1016/
0272-7358(90)90076-M
al
Linnoila, M., Virkkunen, M., Scheinin, M., Nuutila, A., Rimon, R., & Goodwin,
ic
F. K. (1983). Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration
og
differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violence behavior. Life Sciences, 33,
2609–2614. doi:10.1016/0024-3205(83)90344-2 ol
ch
Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor, path, and structural
analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
y
Ps
*Meininger, J. C., Hayman, L. L., Coates, P. M., & Gallagher, P. (1988). Genetics
or environment? Type A behavior and cardiovascular risk factors in twin chil-
an
Norland, S., Shover, N., Thornton, W., & James, J. (1979). Intrafamily conflict and
Am
Partanen, J., Bruun, K., & Markkanen, T. (1966). Inheritance of drinking behavior: A
by
study on intelligence, personality, and use of alcohol of adult twins. Helsinki, Finland:
The Finnish Foundation for Alcohol Studies.
11
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Dishion, T. J. (1992). Antisocial boys. Eugene, OR:
20
Castalia Publishing.
*Plomin, R., Foch, T. T., & Rowe, D. C. (1981). Bobo clown aggression in child-
©
doi:10.1016/0092-6566(81)90031-3
ig
*Rahe, R. H., Hervig, L., & Rosenman, R. H. (1978). Heritability of type A behavior.
yr
Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant children grown up. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park, CA:
Sage.
Rubinstein, M., Phillips, T. J., Bunzow, J. R., Falzone, T. L., Dziewczapolski, G.,
A)
Zhang, G., . . . Chester, J. (1997). Mice lacking dopamine D4 receptors are
supersensitive to ethanol, cocaine, and methamphetamine. Cell, 90, 991–1001.
P
(A
doi:10.1016/S0092-8674(00)80365-7
*Rushton, J. P., Fulker, D. W., Neale, M. C., Nias, D. K. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986).
n
Altruism and aggression: The heritability of individual differences. Journal of Per-
io
sonality and Social Psychology, 50, 1192–1198. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.6.1192
at
Saudou, F., Amara, D. A., Dierich, A., LeMeur, M., Ramboz, S., Segu, L., . . . Hen, R.
ci
(1994, September). Enhanced aggressive behavior in mice lacking 5-HT1B recep-
so
tor. Science, 265, 1875–1878. doi:10.1126/science.8091214
As
*Scarr, S. (1966). Genetic factors in activity motivation. Child Development, 37,
663–673. doi:10.2307/1126688
al
Seeman, P., & Madras, B. K. (1998). Anti-hyperactivity medication: Methylphenidate
ic
og
and amphetamine. Molecular Psychiatry, 3, 386–396. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4000421
Solanto, M. V. (1984). Neuropharmacological basis of stimulant drug action in
ol
attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity: A review and synthesis. Psycholog-
ch
ical Bulletin, 95, 387–409. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.387
y
conflict. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1008, 132–139. doi:10.1196/
er
annals.1301.014
Am
*Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K., Segal, N., & Rich, S.
(1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Per-
e
Tuvblad, C., Eley, T. C., & Lichtenstein, P. (2005). The development of antisocial
by
van Beijsterveldt, C. E., Bartels, M., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Causes
20
A:1025735002864
ht
*van den Oord, E. J. C. G., Verhulst, F. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (1996). A genetic study
ig
van der Valk, J. C., Verhulst, F. C., Neale, M. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (1998). Longitudi-
nal genetic analysis of problem behaviors in biologically related and unrelated
C
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2002). Reactively and proactively
aggressive children: Antecedent and subsequent characteristics. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 43, 495–505. doi:10.1111/1469-
7610.00040
A)
Waldman, I. D., & Gizer, I. (2006). The genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 396–432. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.007
P
(A
Waschbusch, D. A., Willoughby, M. T., & Pelham, W. E. (1998). Criterion validity
and utility of reactive and proactive aggression: Comparisons to attention deficit
n
hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, and other
io
measures of functioning. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 396–405.
at
doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_3
ci
*Wilson, G. D., Rust, J., & Kasriel, J. (1977). Genetic and family origins of humor
so
preferences: A twin study. Psychological Reports, 41, 659–660.
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:38 AM
Am
er
ic
Page 164
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-10_CH09-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:53 AM Page 165
PA)
SOCIAL INFORMATION PROCESSING
(A
PATTERNS AS MEDIATORS OF THE
n
io
INTERACTION BETWEEN GENETIC
at
ci
FACTORS AND LIFE EXPERIENCES
so
IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF
As
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR
al
ic
og
KENNETH A. DODGE
ol
y ch
Ps
an
aggressive behavioral acts (see Chapters 1, 2, and 7). For example, in response
er
that the act was committed with hostile intent (a hostile attribution), the prob-
ability that the respondent will react aggressively is high (about .76; Dodge,
e
1980), whereas when that same respondent infers that the act was commit-
th
Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 2008). Although the evidence is less clear that these
social–cognitive processes cause the aggressive behavioral response during the
©
over and over, suggesting that social–cognitive processes, and the develop-
ig
mental processes that support them, are closely intertwined with the matrix
yr
of causes of aggression.
op
165
12051-10_CH09-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:53 AM Page 166
A)
cessing patterns provide the proximal mechanism through which aggressive
behavior occurs; (b) these patterns correlate with neural and psychophysio-
P
(A
logical processes; (c) these patterns are acquired through genetic and envi-
ronmental processes, especially in interaction; and (d) acquired processing
n
patterns account for the effects of genetic and environmental factors in
io
behavioral development and provide the mechanism through which these
at
factors exert their impact. Findings will be presented from the Child Devel-
ci
opment Project (CDP), an ongoing longitudinal study of 585 boys and girls
so
followed from age 4 through young adulthood.
As
al
SOCIAL INFORMATION PROCESSING MECHANISMS
ic
IN AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORS
og
ol
Models of the processing of information in response to social stimuli posit
ch
a sequence of steps that lead to behavioral responding including aggression
y
toward others (Crick & Dodge, 1994; see also Chapter 7, this volume). These
Ps
steps are logically ordered and assumed to flow temporally, although evidence
for the sequential ordering is scant (see Chapter 1, this volume). Methods to
an
occurs and then to answer questions about attributions and possible responses
(Dodge, 1980). The stimuli have been presented orally, in the form of car-
e
toons, or by video.
th
The first several steps of processing describe the sensation and interpreta-
by
tion of cues, and the latter steps describe behavioral decision making. The first
step is attention to and sensation of the stimulus. Because the stimulus array is
11
whereas a second child might attend to the teacher watching in the back-
ht
ground, and a third child might attend to the peers who are laughing nearby.
ig
behavior, for example, when attention to the provocateur’s look of surprise and
op
A)
those cues, often involving an interpretation of the other person’s intention.
As noted earlier, when a hostile intent is inferred, aggressive behavior likely
P
(A
ensues, in contrast with an inference of a benign intention. The process of
mental representation occurs in microseconds and may be updated across
n
time during a social interaction. It is not usually a conscious process, although
io
it can become so if prompted. The process undoubtedly involves neural activ-
at
ity that is conditioned by experience (for evidence regarding neural activity
ci
underlying mental representations of social intent, see Chapter 6). Inferences
so
of hostile intent have also been found to correlate with heightened auto-
As
nomic reactivity (Crozier et al., 2008). Numerous studies have shown a robust
pattern in which hostile attributional biases are associated with aggressive
al
behavior, especially reactive aggression (Dodge, 1980). A review by Orobio
ic
de Castro, Veerman, Koops, Bosch, and Monshouwer (2002) indicates that
og
this pattern holds across ages, demographic and cultural groups, and contexts.
ol
The third step is goal selection, in which the mentally represented stim-
ch
ulus is associated with an emotional reaction and the narrowing of a goal.
y
Again, the respondent is not usually aware of this process but might reflect after-
Ps
ward on the cognitive processes involved. Children who experience anger and
regularly select instrumental and self-defensive goals are likely to behave aggres-
an
sively, whereas children who select social goals are likely to behave nonaggres-
ic
sion, withdrawal, and social deflection. The trigger from mental representa-
th
sive behavior (see Chapters 3 and 6, this volume). Processes of response eval-
yr
uation and decision (called RED by Fontaine & Dodge, 2006) follow. During
op
A)
consequences of a behavior; and (d) outcome valuation, in which the estimated
outcome is given value. Fontaine and Dodge (2006) hypothesized that differ-
P
(A
ent possible responses are compared (response comparison) before the most
appropriate response is selected (response selection). Measurement of response
n
decision during hypothetical social stimuli has yielded robust correlations
io
between all of these subprocesses and chronic aggressive behavior, especially
at
proactive aggression (see the review by Fontaine & Dodge, 2006).
ci
so
As
SOCIAL INFORMATION PROCESSING PATTERNS
AS PREDICTORS OF INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES
al
IN AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR
ic
og
Although these processing patterns are empirically correlated with indi-
ol
vidual differences in aggressive behavior, the correlational findings often suffer
ch
from two problems. First, because the measurement of processing is typically
y
tive processing and not processing itself, which occurs at the neural level and
nonconsciously. Even self-reports that are collected “online” are immediate
an
ing. The second problem is that the empirical correlation between patterns of
Am
variable. Two kinds of evidence have been mounted to test the hypothesis that
th
The first evidence comes from prospective studies in which early levels
of problematic aggressive conduct are controlled statistically. In the CDP of a
©
community sample of 585 boys and girls followed for 20 years, we assessed both
ht
social information that become stable when measured annually for 4 years, as
op
A)
aggression were statistically controlled (Dodge et al., 2003). This pattern sup-
ports the hypothesis that social information processing plays a causal role in
P
(A
generating aggressive behavior.
We also found that we could predict adolescent conduct problems from
n
kindergarten processing patterns. Here, we scored children as displaying prob-
io
lems in social information processing at the early steps (i.e., hypervigilance
at
and hostile attributional biases) or later steps (i.e., response generation or
ci
evaluation) or at both steps or neither step. We found, while controlling for
so
kindergarten externalizing problems as assessed by both mothers and teach-
As
ers, that the four groups differed in mother- and teacher-rated externalizing
behavior problems at the end of Grade 11 (Lansford et al., 2006). Further-
al
more, the effect was cumulative or synergistic, in that the group of children
ic
with kindergarten problems at both stages of processing was especially likely
og
to show conduct problems in high school.
ol
The prediction was even stronger for processing patterns in Grade 8. Con-
ch
trolling for conduct problems before and during Grade 8, the four groups of chil-
y
atic conduct being the one with problems at both early and later stages of
ic
processing.
er
iterative and reciprocal (Fontaine & Dodge, 2006). That is, aggressive behav-
ior at age 14 predicted processing patterns the next year, which in turn pre-
e
dicted growth in aggression the following year, even controlling for prior
th
the following year, which altered processing patterns in the subsequent year.
Across adolescence, this reciprocal effect continues.
11
20
dom assignment and the effect on aggressive behavior is assessed. Graham and
yr
ican boys process information in more benevolent ways (i.e., with a lessened
hostile attribution bias). They found that random assignment to this inter-
C
vention led to lower scores on measures of hostile attribution bias, and this
A)
condition was associated with improvements in processing patterns and
reduced aggressive behavior.
P
(A
Situation and Relationship Specificity
n
io
A pivotal issue in constructing models of social information processing
at
is situational specificity in the link between processing patterns and behavior.
ci
Processing patterns within a certain kind of situation, such as being provoked
so
or attempting to enter a peer group or handling conflict in a romantic relation-
As
ship or with a coworker, predict behavior within that type of situation more
strongly than behavior in other situations. For example, Dodge et al. (1986)
al
assessed processing patterns in provocative peer interactions and peer-group
ic
entry situations. We then placed children in a laboratory setting and exposed
og
them to a provocation by a confederate peer and an entry situation in which
ol
they were asked to initiate entry into a strange peer group. We found that pro-
ch
cessing patterns predicted behavior, and the predictions were stronger within
y
More recently, we found similar evidence for young adults. Two kinds of
situations are important in young adulthood, defined by relationships. The
an
in that age period (Collins & van Dulmen, 2006). Unfortunately, violence is
er
with adult peers are essential to work and community success (Arnett, 2006).
th
We were able to interview 85% of the original CDP sample at age 22 and
follow them through age 24. We assessed processing patterns in situations
©
involving conflict with a romantic partner (e.g., “You are at a gathering with
ht
a group of friends and your girl/boyfriend and learn that your girl/boyfriend and
ig
one of the people at the gathering used to be a couple; they spend most of the
yr
night talking with each other”) and conflict with a coworker or peer (e.g., “You
op
tell a friend something personal and ask your friend not to discuss it with any-
one else. However, a couple of weeks later, you find out that a lot of people
C
We (Pettit, Lansford, Dodge, & Bates, 2009) were able to predict violent
behavior in each of these types of relationships as well as evidence of relation-
ship specificity. Processing patterns in a hypothetical romantic relationship
predicted violent behavior in actual romantic relationships 2 years later, as
A)
reported by both the participant and his or her romantic partner. Also, process-
ing patterns within the peer relationship predicted violent behavior toward
P
(A
peers 2 years later. Furthermore, the predictions were stronger within kinds of
relationships than across kinds of relationships.
n
io
at
NEURAL AND PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL PROCESSES
ci
AND SOCIAL INFORMATION PROCESSING
so
As
A misconception about information processing is that it occurs inde-
pendently of biological processes in real time. Accumulating evidence indi-
al
cates that psychophysiological and neural processes co-occur with information
ic
processing (see Chapter 6, this volume). Most likely, these processes occur in
og
real time outside a person’s awareness, and measures of information processing
ol
represent the individual’s postbehavioral reflection on her or his thoughts and
ch
actions. This suggests that the measures of information processing may be
y
Psychophysiological Processes
an
ic
Ortiz and Raine (2004) reviewed evidence indicating that resting heart
er
low resting heart rate predicts higher aggressive behavior), especially proactive
and life-persistent aggression but not situational or adolescence-limited aggres-
e
sion (Moffit & Caspi, 2001). Raine, Venables, and Mednick (1997) found that
th
low resting heart rate at age 3 predicted aggressive behavior 8 years later at
by
age 11. Raine (2002) hypothesized that deficits in volume and function of
prefrontal cortex may be responsible for low autonomic activity as well as
11
aggressive behavior.
20
tion occurs, heart rate increases and then slowly returns to baseline when
ig
studies showing that aggressive children display higher heart rate reactivity
op
processing responses mediate the link between heart rate changes and aggres-
sive behavior.
Thus, two separate psychophysiological processes may be related to
aggressive behavior. Both low resting heart rate and high heart rate reactiv-
A)
ity in response to threatening cues appear to predict aggressive behavior.
These processes may have their antecedents in both life events and heritable
P
(A
biological processes, and their impact on aggressive behavior may be medi-
ated by the manner in which the individual processes social information in
n
response to threat.
io
at
Neural Processes
ci
so
Neuroimaging studies suggest that regions of the prefrontal cortex (e.g.,
As
the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and ventral prefrontal cortex) and the lim-
bic system (e.g., amygdala) are activated during information processing in
al
response to interpersonal provocations such as unfair allocation of resources
ic
by a peer (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2006; see also Chapter 6, this volume).
og
Meyer-Lindenberg et al. (2006) found significant activation of the amygdala
ol
in response to experimental presentation of stimuli similar to those used to
ch
assess social information processing, such as angry and fearful faces and aver-
y
decision making (Raine, Buchsbaum, & LaCasse, 1997). Raine (2008) stated
Am
to correlate with amygdala volume and probably with its activation (Meyer-
Lindenberg et al., 2006). Release of 5-HT in the prefrontal cortex is thought
11
ceived hostile treatment (Evers et al., 2006), although the evidence was
only correlational until recently. Crockett, Clark, Tabibnia, Lieberman,
©
peer with greater retaliatory behavior (for more evidence concerning the role
of serotonin in aggressive behavior, see Chapters 7 and 8, this volume).
C
Thus, in the same way that social information processing involves multi-
ple, relatively independent steps, functional brain imaging studies have identi-
fied multiple brain regions that are implicated in different aspects of responding
to social stimuli.
PA)
(A
ANTECEDENTS OF SOCIAL INFORMATION
PROCESSING PATTERNS AND NEURAL PROCESSES
n
io
at
I turn now to the antecedents of processing patterns and neural processes
ci
that mediate aggressive behavior. Given the empirical findings of both general
so
prediction and situation-specific prediction, I hypothesize that some antecedent
As
factors apply generally and some apply to specific domains, situations, or
relationships.
al
ic
Child Maltreatment
og
ol
The experience of abuse by one’s parent during the first 5 years of life is
ch
devastating and has been hypothesized to alter one’s central working models of
how human relationships work. Children develop basic trust through inter-
y
Ps
action with caring adults, and violation of that trust through extreme or ongo-
ing maltreatment is hypothesized to lead to schemas, scripts, knowledge
an
structures, and working models that others will act maliciously (see Chapters 4,
ic
13, 14, and 19). Thus, the child develops hypervigilant and selective attentional
er
and Pettit (1992) found that maltreatment in the first 5 years of life predicted
©
found that 30.4% of the maltreated children had an official court record of
arrest by age 24, in contrast with 16.5% of the nonmaltreated children.
Lansford et al. (2007) found that the outcomes that accrue from early-life
maltreatment are broad and include not only aggressive behavior but also early
A)
pregnancy, anxiety, depressive symptoms, school dropout, substance-use prob-
lems, and arrests for a variety of crimes (for a review of these long-term effects
P
(A
of child maltreatment, see Chapter 19, this volume). The processing patterns
that were assessed in early elementary school mediated some but not all of these
n
outcomes, perhaps because the stimuli that were used to assess processing pat-
io
terns were restricted to only selected domains. It is plausible that maltreatment
at
alters processing in multiple domains, which then mediate behavioral out-
ci
comes. More comprehensive assessments of processing patterns are necessary
so
to test this hypothesis.
As
Peer Social Rejection
al
ic
Other life experiences appear to have more circumscribed effects. In
og
American elementary schools, chronic peer social rejection is a fairly com-
ol
mon painful experience. We hypothesized, and found empirically, that this
ch
experience would exacerbate children’s aggressive behavior problems
y
(Dodge et al., 2003). That is, rejection during kindergarten through Grade 2
predicted aggressive behavior in Grade 4, even controlling for early aggres-
an
peer events partially mediated the growth in aggressive behavior during this
er
period.
Am
More recently, Pettit et al. (2009) found that the impact of early peer
rejection lasts through young adulthood. Recall the CDP findings presented
e
earlier about the significant relation between processing patterns in peer rela-
th
tionships at age 22 and aggressive behavior toward peers at age 24. It turns out
by
and those who had not, those who displayed problematic processing about peer
ht
relationships at age 22 and those who did not, and those who displayed vio-
ig
lence toward adult peers versus those who did not. Among those who were not
yr
the probability of becoming violent toward a peer was relatively low (.43),
whereas among rejected children who displayed problematic processing, the
C
probability was high (.70). In between were nonrejected children who dis-
played problematic processing (.47) and rejected children who did not display
problematic processing (.51).
PA)
The antecedents of romantic relationship violence may differ from those
(A
for peer-directed violence. Exposure to parents’ domestic violence in early life
predicts later violence in romantic relationships (Nay, Dodge, Lansford,
n
io
Pettit, & Bates, 2009), but disentangling the effect of this particular exposure
at
from other family violence may prove difficult. We explored the effect of vic-
ci
timization during adolescent romantic relationships on young adult violent
so
behavior toward a romantic partner (for a review of consequences of aggres-
sion within romantic relationships, see Chapter 20, this volume).
As
We found that being victimized violently during an adolescent roman-
al
tic relationship did indeed predict violent behavior toward a romantic partner
ic
in young adulthood. Also, this adolescent experience predicted problematic
og
processing patterns in romantic relationships, which partially mediated the
effect of victimization on later violent behavior toward a romantic partner.
ol
Among non-victims who display non-problematic processing, the probability
ch
of later becoming violent toward a romantic partner was low (.29), whereas
y
Ps
among victims who displayed problematic processing, the probability was high
(.62). In between were nonvictims who displayed problematic processing (.42)
an
Genetic Factors
by
5-HT gene have been associated with increased amygdala activation (Pezawas
et al., 2005) and prefrontal cortex functions (Raine, 2008), as assessed through
20
the X-linked MAO-A gene has been correlated with amygdala volume and
activation in response to threatening stimuli (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2006).
ht
That is, individuals, especially males, with a polymorphism in the gene for
ig
threatening faces and aversive emotional situations. It follows, then, that vari-
op
A)
and 8). The major environmental factors in early life have been reviewed
elsewhere (Dodge et al., 2006; Dodge & Pettit, 2003); they include the kinds
P
(A
of personally threatening experiences addressed earlier, such as early physical
maltreatment, peer social rejection, and victimization, as well as exposure to
n
stressful contexts such as poverty and disadvantage. The latter factors are likely
io
to operate through their effects on life experiences such as parenting quality
at
and success in life tasks such as getting an education (Dodge, Pettit, & Bates,
ci
1994). Major environmental variables in mid-childhood and adolescence
so
involve exposure to deviant peers who influence high-risk youth to engage in
As
aggressive behavior and also involve parental failure to supervise and monitor
youths’ activities, thus increasing their exposure to violence and precipitators
al
of violence (Dodge et al., 2006).
ic
Environmental variables that have an enduring effect are likely to be
og
ones that alter brain processes. Evidence has been growing that life stressors
ol
and early trauma have enduring effects on both prefrontal cortex and amyg-
ch
dala processes measured through fMRI. Liston, McEwan, and Casey (2009)
y
reported that the natural experiment of a potent stressor (an upcoming aca-
Ps
and Temple (2007) found that individuals who had been exposed to major
ic
trauma during the September 11, 2001, attack on the World Trade Center
er
evident for some time (see Chapter 8), although adoption studies have iden-
th
son, Bohman, and von Knorring (1982) found that under conditions of low
heritable risk (i.e., having a biological parent who was not a criminal), the
11
impact of the environment (i.e., having an adoptive parent who was or was
20
not a criminal) on later criminality was rather small (a rate of .03 for non-
criminal adoptive parents vs. .07 for criminal adoptive parents). However,
©
under conditions of high heritable risk, the effect of the environment was
ht
strong (.12 for noncriminal adoptive parents vs. .40 for criminal adoptive par-
ig
ents). Jaffee et al. (2005) studied monozygotic and dizygotic twins from the
yr
British E-Risk study and identified four rank-ordered groups of increasing her-
op
itable risk, with the lowest heritable-risk group being children whose mono-
zygotic twin was not conduct disordered (CD), the next lowest group being
C
children whose dizygotic twin was not CD, the next highest group being chil-
dren whose dizygotic twin was CD, and the highest group being children
whose monozygotic twin was CD. The experience of child physical maltreat-
ment was determined by clinical interview with the mother, following pro-
A)
cedures used by Dodge, Bates, and Pettit (1990). Among the group at lowest
heritable risk, the experience of physical maltreatment had little effect on
P
(A
conduct disorder outcomes (for the nonmaltreated group, the rate was .02; for
maltreated group it was .04). Among those at the next highest level of heri-
n
table risk, the effect of maltreatment was small (.06 vs. .13). Among the group
io
at the next highest level, the effect of maltreatment grew larger (.19 vs. .37).
at
Finally, among those at the highest level, the effect of maltreatment was
ci
largest (.46 vs. .70).
so
The search for specific genetic variables that predict aggressive behav-
As
ior has been plagued by both political pressures and empirical failures to repli-
cate published findings, perhaps because of atheoretical approaches to gene
al
analyses that capitalize on chance (Kim-Cohen et al., 2006). Several theo-
ic
rists argue that genetic factors are likely to operate indirectly through their
og
effects on neurally and biologically mediated dispositions to act impulsively
ol
without planning, without empathy, or without consideration of extenuating
ch
circumstances (Caspi et al., 2002). These dispositions have been called exec-
y
utive functions (Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000), and they likely affect processing
Ps
In line with this hypothesis, the genetic polymorphisms that have been
Am
most frequently correlated with aggressive behavior are ones that relate to neuro-
transmitter functions, especially dopamine (Dick et al., 2006; Moffitt et al.,
e
2008; see also Chapter 8, this volume). Three genes are discussed here. First,
th
and dopamine (Shih, Chen, & Ridd, 1999), which are involved in brain
actions associated with stress regulation (Charney, 2004) and biological sensi-
11
tivity to adverse social contexts (Boyce & Ellis, 2005). A polymorphism in the
20
been correlated with both aggressive behavior and lower brain serotonin and
ig
Nelen, Breakefield, Ropers, & van Oost, 1993), but other studies have found
contradictory patterns (Kim-Cohen et al., 2006).
C
A)
environmental stressor of peer social rejection and not to peer inclusion. They
suggested that MAO-A may dispose individuals to become “hypersensitive” to
P
(A
interpersonal rejection.
If the MAO-A enzyme is involved in regulating stress, especially in
n
reaction to trauma and threat, then it might play a role in moderating the
io
effect of early physical maltreatment on the development of aggressive behav-
at
ior. Caspi et al. (2002) found this interaction effect in their Dunedin longi-
ci
tudinal sample, and Kim-Cohen et al. (2006) replicated the pattern in the
so
British E-Risk study. In the CDP, we recently replicated this interaction effect
As
on externalizing behavior patterns (Edwards et al., 2009). Among children
without the MAO-A polymorphism, those who had been maltreated did not
al
differ from those who had not been maltreated in the proportion who had
ic
been arrested by age 22 (.28 vs. .25), but among children with the polymor-
og
phism, those who had been maltreated had a much higher probability of crim-
ol
inal arrest by age 22 than those who had not been maltreated (.71 vs. .26).
ch
A second gene that has been implicated in a variety of externalizing and
y
alpha 2, also known as GABRA2 (Dick et al., 2006). GABA is the major
inhibitory neurotransmitter in the mammalian brain. A polymorphism in
an
GABRA2 has been associated with conduct disorder, but the more powerful
ic
supervision during early adolescence has been found to buffer children from
Am
the adverse effect of the GABRA2 polymorphism (Dick et al., 2006). Among
children whose parents engaged in low rates of supervision, the likelihood of
e
increases dramatically with the number of copies of the risk allele of GABRA2,
by
from .07 to .19 to .28 for 0, 1, or 2 copies, respectively, whereas among children
whose parents engaged in high rates of supervision, the likelihood of being in
11
(2008; see also Chapter 8, this volume) recently took this work a step fur-
yr
5-HT and hostile attributional biases. Finally, the effect of 5-HT on reactive
aggression was mediated by hostile attributional biases. This work is the first
known attempt to identify molecular genetic bases for social information
processing patterns.
A)
In sum, a comprehensive model of the development of chronic aggres-
sive behavior must take into account environmental main effects, gene main
P
effects, and gene-by-environment interaction effects. Furthermore, the envi-
(A
ronmental variables are likely to include both broad factors that influence
n
aggression across many situations as well as factors that influence aggression
io
within particular kinds of situations.
at
ci
so
A PROPOSED MODEL OF SOCIAL INFORMATION
As
PROCESSING MECHANISMS IN GENE-BY-ENVIRONMENT
INTERACTION EFFECTS
al
ic
Integration of the distal genetic and environmental factors with social
og
information processing proximal mechanisms requires a final leap of theorizing.
ol
The model proposed here builds on models by van Goozen, Fairchild, and
ch
Harold (2008) and Raine (2008) but is unique in positing that within-situation
processing patterns mediate the effects of genes, environments, and their inter-
y
Ps
actions. The overall model is depicted in Figure 9.1, which posits that specific
(albeit as yet unidentified) genes and an early environment characterized by
an
threat, trauma, or adversity pose risks for the long-term development of chron-
ically violent behavior. These distal factors operate as main effects and in inter-
ic
ate aggressive behavior within those situations. When these situations present
themselves, they pose proximal risk of violent behavior.
e
These distal and proximal risk factors are mediated by brain processes
th
A RESEARCH AGENDA
yr
op
Situational
stimulus
A)
P
(A
Genes Neural
activity
n
io
at
ci
Social
so
information Violence
processing
As
al
Psycho-
ic
physiological
og
Early Situational activity
environment environment
ol
ch
Figure 9.1. A proposed model of genetic, environmental, and processing mechanisms
y
processing responses while the individuals are being observed via fMRI and
Am
psychophysiological recording.
Such studies are likely to produce both support and disconfirmation of
e
ment of the model over time. Although empirical studies have already yielded
by
promising results, it is highly unlikely that any single gene or group of genes,
even in interaction with environmental histories, will account for much of
11
The implications of these empirical findings and the model in Figure 9.1
for the design of preventive interventions suggest an additional research
©
tion has the exciting but unrealized potential to reduce or prevent the devel-
ig
that alter social information processing patterns offer the hope of secondary
op
REFERENCES
A)
2909.126.5.651
Arnett, J. J. (2006). The psychology of emerging adulthood: What is known, and
P
(A
what remains to be known? In J. J. Arnett & J. L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging adults
in America: Coming of age in the 21st century (pp. 303–330). Washington, DC:
n
American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/11381-013
io
Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Lavoie, F. (2001). Reactive and pro-
at
active aggression: Predictions to physical violence in different contexts and mod-
ci
erating effects of parental monitoring and caregiving behavior. Journal of
so
Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 293–304. doi:10.1023/A:1010305828208
As
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary–
developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Develop-
al
ment and Psychopathology, 17, 271–301. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050145
ic
Brunner, H. G., Nelen, M., Breakefield, X., Ropers, H., & van Oost, B. (1993, Octo-
og
ber). Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene
ol
for monoamine oxidase A. Science, 262, 578–580. doi:10.1126/science.8211186
ch
Cases, O., Seif, I., Grimsby, J., Gaspar, P., Chen, K., Pournin, S., . . . De Maeyer, E.
y
(1995, June). Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and
Ps
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I.,W., . . . Poulton, R.
ic
Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S., Bohman, M., & von Knorring, A. (1982). Predis-
by
Collins, W. A., & van Dulmen, M. (2006). “The course of true love(s) . . . ”: Origins
20
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-
ig
Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., Tabibnia, G., Lieberman, M. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2008,
June). Serotonin Modulates Behavioral Reactions to Unfairness. Science, 320,
C
1739. doi:10.1126/science.1155577
Crozier, J. C., Dodge, K. A., Fontaine, R. G., Lansford, J. E., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S.,
& Levenson, R. W. (2008). Social information processing and cardiac predic-
tors of adolescent antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117,
253–267. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.253
Dick, D. M., Bierut, L., Hinrichs, A., Fox, L., Bucholz, K. K., Kramer, J., . . . Foroud,
A)
T. (2006). The role of GABRA2 in risk for conduct disorder and alcohol and
P
drug dependence across developmental stages. Behavior Genetics, 36, 577–590.
(A
doi:10.1007/s10519-005-9041-8
n
Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children’s aggressive behavior. Child
io
Development, 51, 162–170. doi:10.2307/1129603
at
Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990, December). Mechanisms in the cycle
ci
of violence. Science, 250, 1678–1683. doi:10.1126/science.2270481
so
Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior
As
in youth. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child
psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed.,
al
pp. 719–788). New York, NY: Wiley.
ic
Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S., Fontaine, R., &
og
Price, J. M. (2003). Peer rejection and social information-processing factors in
ol
the development of aggressive behavior problems in children. Child Develop-
ment, 74, 374–393. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.7402004
ch
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the development
y
Ps
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Socialization mediators of the rela-
tion between socioeconomic status and child conduct problems. Child Develop-
ic
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Valente, E. (1995). Social information-
Am
processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later
conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 632–643. doi:10.1037/
e
0021-843X.104.4.632
th
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., McClaskey, C. L., & Brown, M. (1986). Social compe-
by
Edwards, A. C., Dodge, K. A., Latendresse, S. J., Lansford, J. E., Bates, J. E., Pettit,
20
chiatry. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02196.x
ht
Eisenberger, N. I., Way, B. M., Taylor, S. E., Welch, W. T., & Lieberman, M. D.
ig
(2007). Understanding genetic risk for aggression: Clues from the brain’s
yr
Evers, E. A. T., Van der Veen, F. M., Van Deursen, J. A., Schmitt, J. A. J., Deutz,
C
N. E. P., & Jolles, J. (2006). The effect of acute tryptophan depletion on the BOLD
A)
Fontaine, R. G., Yang, C., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2008). Testing an
P
individual systems model of Response Evaluation and Decision (RED) and anti-
(A
social behavior across adolescence. Child Development, 79, 462–475. doi:10.1111/
n
j.1467-8624.2007.01136.x
io
Ganzel, B., Casey, B. J., Glover, G., Voss, H. U., & Temple, E. (2007). The after-
at
math of 9/11: Effect of intensity and recency of trauma on outcome. Emotion, 7,
ci
227–238. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.227
so
Graham, S., & Hudley, C. (1993). An attributional intervention to reduce peer-
As
directed aggression among African-American boys. Child Development, 64,
124–138.
al
Guerra, N. G., & Slaby, R. G. (1989). Evaluative factors in social problem solving by
ic
aggressive boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 277–289.
og
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Dodge, K. A., Rutter, M., Taylor, A., & Tully,
ol
L. A. (2005). Nature × nurture: Genetic vulnerabilities interact with physical
ch
maltreatment to promote conduct problems. Development and Psychopathology,
17, 67–84. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050042
y
Ps
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., Newcombe, R., Craig, I. W., &
Moffit, T. E. (2006). MAOA, maltreatment, and gene–environment interaction
an
Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Dodge, K. A., Crozier, J. C., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E.
(2006). A 12-year prospective study of patterns of social information processing
Am
Lansford, J. E., Miller-Johnson, S., Berlin, L. J., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., &
th
Liston, C., McEwan, B. S., & Casey, B. J. (2009). Psychosocial stress reversibly dis-
20
pnas.0807041106
ht
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Buckholtz, J. W., Kolachana, B., Hariri, A., Pezawas, L., Blasi,
op
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Jaffee, S. R., Kim-Cohen, J., Koenen, K. C., Odgers,
C. L., . . . Viding, E. (2008). DSM-V conduct disorder: Research needs for an
evidence base. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 3–33.
Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2001). Childhood predictors differentiate life-course per-
A)
sistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females.
Development and Psychopathology, 13, 355–375. doi:10.1017/ S0954579401002097
P
(A
Morgan, A. B., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation
between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive
n
function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 113–136. doi:10.1016/S0272-
io
7358(98)00096-8
at
Murphy, F. C., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Lawrence, A. D. (2003). Functional neuro-
ci
anatomy of emotions: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuro-
so
science, 3, 207–233. doi:10.3758/CABN.3.3.207
As
Nay, S., Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2009). Rejection sen-
sitivity as a mediator of the effect of romantic relationship history on violent behavior
al
in romantic relationships. Unpublished manuscript.
ic
Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monshouwer, H. J.
og
(2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis.
ol
Child Development, 73, 916–934. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00447
ch
Ortiz, J., & Raine, A. (2004). Heart rate level and antisocial behavior in children and
y
social information processing and violent behavior in early adulthood. Manuscript sub-
ic
Pezawas, L., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Drabani, E. M., Verchinski, B. A., Munoz, K. E.,
Am
Raine, A. (2002). Annotation: The role of prefrontal deficits, low autonomic arousal
by
and early health factors in the development of antisocial and aggressive behav-
ior in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,
11
Raine, A., Buchsbaum, M., & LaCasse, L. (1997). Brain abnormalities in murderers
ht
doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00362-9
yr
Raine, A., Venables, P. H., & Mednick, S. A. (1997). Low resting heart rate at age
op
Shih, J. C., Chen, K., & Ridd, M. (1999). Monoamine oxidase: From genes to
behavior. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 197–217. doi:10.1146/annurev.
neuro.22.1.197
Silverman, J. G., Raj, A., Mucci, L. A., & Hathaway, J. E. (2001). Dating violence
A)
against adolescent girls and associated substance use, unhealthy weight con-
trol, sexual risk behavior, pregnancy, and suicidality. JAMA, 286, 572–579.
P
doi:10.1001/jama.286.5.572
(A
van Goozen, S. H. M., Fairchild, G., & Harold, G. T. (2008). The role of neurobiolog-
n
ical deficits in childhood antisocial behavior. Current Directions in Psychological
io
Science, 17, 224–228. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00579.x
at
Waldman, I. D. (2008, June). The etiology of hostile perceptual biases and their relation
ci
with children’s aggression. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Behavior
so
Genetics Association, Louisville, KY.
As
Weiss, B., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1992). Some consequences of
early harsh discipline: Child aggression and a maladaptive social information
al
processing style. Child Development, 63, 1321–1335. doi:10.2307/1131558
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:53 AM
Am
er
ic
Page 186
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-11_CH10-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:54 AM Page 187
10
PA)
VIOLENCE AND CHARACTER: A CUPS
(A
(CULTURE × PERSON × SITUATION)
n
io
PERSPECTIVE
at
ci
so
DOV COHEN AND ANGELA K.-Y. LEUNG
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
have both contributed greatly to our understanding, but both approaches face
er
ences can explain differences between cultures in rates of violence, but it has
difficulty predicting who will be violent and what this means for the violent
e
difference approach that ignores culture are that (a) the approach may fail to
by
only one cultural context but do not hold, or even reverse, in others.
20
logics. Such logics structure behaviors, situations, scripts, and values in ways
yr
that make sense to people within a culture, even if they do not make sense to
op
people outside that culture. We also argue for taking individual differences
seriously by treating people as more than cultural robots who mechanically
C
follow cultural dictates. In the combined approach advocated here, the key
187
12051-11_CH10-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:54 AM Page 188
notion is that individuals are always in a culture, though they are not always
of it. We outline this combined approach, the CuPS approach (culture ×
person × situation) and demonstrate its value by describing experiments
conducted with three different cultural groups.
A)
Before doing so, it is important to say what the CuPS approach is not.
CuPS is not a mediational approach. It does not assume that there is some
P
(A
underlying individual difference that plays itself out similarly across cultures,
and it does not assume that cultural differences would simply derive from such
n
an underlying difference. As such, it does not follow the common procedures
io
for studying cultural and individual differences, namely, (a) showing that
at
there are cultural differences between some dependent variables; (b) measur-
ci
ing an individual-difference variable that also predicts the dependent variable
so
and differs in mean levels across cultures; and (c) showing that the cultural dif-
As
ference is entirely a product of this individual difference, disappearing when
the individual difference is controlled for.
al
We have no doubt that this mediational approach is important and that
ic
it is extremely useful for understanding various phenomena. However, our
og
approach differs in that it treats culture seriously by considering the way cul-
ol
tural logics structure and give psychological meaning to behaviors and situa-
ch
tions, and it allows for different cultures to have different cultural logics.
y
that make perfect sense in one culture may seem puzzling to people outside
that culture: “How come he would risk his life to help me out of a jam, but if
©
I insult his mother, he’d kill me?” “How can this person be so trustworthy
ht
when he has no backbone and won’t stand up for himself?” “How can this per-
ig
son be so arrogant that he ignores other people’s opinions, and yet he is also
yr
Individual differences arise because people are not automata following the
dictates of their culture. People can follow the ideals of their culture, or they
C
can reject them. However, their behavior should not be considered random,
because it is in fact patterned. People choose their behavior, but these choices
are often influenced by ready-made cultural templates that structure certain
sorts of behaviors as belonging together. Thus, within an honor culture, an indi-
vidual who endorses retributive violence may be embracing the honor ideal and
A)
thus may be more honest and trustworthy, these virtues also being elements of
the honor ideal. Conversely, within an honor culture, an individual who rejects
P
(A
retributive violence may be rejecting the honor ideals, including those related
to being an honest and trustworthy person. Outside of an honor culture, the
n
pattern is likely quite different. In a nonhonor culture, a person who endorses
io
retributive violence may be rejecting his culture’s ideals, including ideals of
at
honesty and trustworthiness as well as nonviolence. The particular cultural log-
ci
ics have to be understood in greater detail, but the point is that the person ×
so
situation behavioral signatures in one culture may be very different from the
As
person × situation behavioral signatures in another culture.
al
ic
ORDER AND VALUE IN HONOR, DIGNITY,
og
AND FACE CULTURES
ol
ch
Cultures are defined by how they solve certain problems. Two of the
y
most salient of these are the problem of order and the problem of value. The
Ps
first relates to how cooperation and order emerge in a society, with a central
question being: Who has legitimate authority to use violence and when can
an
they use it? The second relates to how worth is provisioned: Is personal worth
ic
value at least theoretically equal to that of every other person” (Ayers, 1984,
p. 19). Dignity is inalienable in that others cannot take it away. Thus, a per-
11
son with a sense of dignity has an integrity that comes from a sturdy moral
core, centered on a belief in the inherent worth of individual human beings.
©
This sturdy moral core—rather than the threat of being shamed by others—
ht
autonomous agents who work within a system of law that protects property
rights and punishes predation. Violations of the rules are offenses against the
state, and it is the state that punishes, rather than vigilantes.
In a culture of honor, honor is both internal and external; it must be
claimed from and paid by others. Honor is defined by Pitt-Rivers (1966) as
A)
follows:
P
The value of a person in his own eyes, but also in the eyes of his society.
(A
It is his estimation of his own worth, his claim to pride, but it is also the
acknowledgement of that claim . . . his right to pride. (p. 21)
n
io
An individual must assertively strive for honor, but if that honor is not ulti-
at
mately conferred by other people, it is a big problem. Thus, honor requires for-
ci
titude but is itself fragile and precarious. In Ayers’s (1984) metaphor, it is
so
unlike dignity, which resembles
As
an internal skeleton, . . . a hard structure at the center of the self; honor,
al
on the other hand, resembles a . . . suit of armor that, once pierced, leaves
ic
the self . . . no alternative except to strike back in desperation. (p. 20)
og
The fragility of honor derives from the context in which cultures of
ol
honor typically develop. They arise in contexts where the state is weak, where
ch
there is no effective law enforcement, no mechanism to guarantee contracts,
no police to prevent predation, and no justice system to punish the guilty (Pitt-
y
Ps
A reputation for reciprocity is key here because one must be known as some-
ic
one who will pay back, who can be relied on to pay back a good turn, who will
er
deliver on promises (and threats), and who will avenge wrongs done to him
Am
(see Chapter 12, this volume). Insults and trivial conflicts take on great impor-
tance in such cultures because they are probes or tests of who can do what to
e
whom. A man who will not let himself be trifled with on small matters surely
th
will not let himself be trifled with on big matters either. In this context, it pays
by
to have a reputation as a man of honor who will show positive reciprocity and,
if wronged, a thirst for vengeance (see Chapters 3 and 12, this volume).
11
his or her role. A person’s performance in his or her role is not for him or
ig
her to judge but rather must be judged by others; thus, face has primarily an
yr
external quality (Kim & Cohen, 2010). Face can be gained, but the focus is
op
mostly on not losing it—for oneself and for others one interacts with. Indeed,
unlike honor cultures, where one person can often increase his honor by tak-
C
ing someone else’s, in face cultures people often work together to save each
other’s face, avoiding the direct conflicts that become such important con-
tests for status in competitive honor cultures.
Bad behavior in a face culture is not supposed to be punished by the vic-
tim. Doing so would only further upset the harmony that is supposed to pre-
A)
vail in the stable hierarchy of a face culture. Instead, the group or someone
further up the hierarchy will do the punishing. Generally, the three Hs that
P
(A
characterize face cultures are harmony, hierarchy, and humility. The latter,
humility, is quite functional, because in a system where face must be socially
n
conferred, it is not good to claim too much for oneself; otherwise, one may
io
receive a painful and humiliating lesson about where one really falls in the
at
status hierarchy.
ci
These sketches of three kinds of cultures are ideal types (Weber, 1920/
so
1997), and ideal types do not actually exist in the world. However, the sketches
As
are useful because they lead us to predict different behavioral signatures among
people from honor, dignity, and face cultures. It is not the case that individ-
al
uals all follow the ideals of their culture. However, when individuals depart
ic
from the ideals, their departures tend to be systematic. Individuals who depart
og
from the ideals are not simply people who are oblivious to cultural norms, and
ol
they are not random noise. Rather, their behavior—even when they reject
ch
their culture’s dominant ideals—tends to fit preexisting cultural templates,
y
patterned by the culture’s logic even as its ideals are rejected. Again, individ-
Ps
uals are always in a culture, even if they are not always of it.
an
ic
TWO EXPERIMENTS
er
Am
that occur in different cultures. In our first study we examined positive reci-
th
ture group comprised northern Anglo American students, our honor culture
20
group comprised Latino and southern Anglo American students, and our face
culture group comprised Asian Americans whose ancestors were from East
©
Asia (for supporting evidence, see Ayers, 1984; Cohen, Hoshino-Browne, &
ht
Leung, 2007; Hamamura & Heine, 2008; Heine et al., 1999; Ho, 1976; Kim,
ig
Cohen, & Au, in press; Triandis, 1994; Vandello & Cohen, 2003; Vandello,
yr
Cohen, Grandon, & Franiuk, 2009). Obviously, all of the participants were
op
A)
to an insult, affront, or threat, and the participants had to indicate how justified
the violence was, how much the insulter deserved to be beaten, how reasonable
P
(A
the violent person’s actions were, and so on. The measure of (b) was obtained
with simple Likert scale items: “How others treat me is irrelevant to my worth
n
as a person,” “How much I respect myself is far, far more important than how
io
much others respect me,” “No one (except me) can make me feel diminished,”
at
and “No one can take a person’s self-respect away from him or her.” Participants’
ci
endorsements of (a) or (b) were person-level variables in the design.
so
In Experiment 1, participants were given a cover story, according to
As
which we were examining people’s attitudes toward violence in the media.
This was the putative reason for showing film clips and asking about violence.
al
The cover story was followed by a questionnaire that asked about demograph-
ic
ics and also contained the questions described earlier concerning inalienable
og
versus socially conferred worth. As far as the participant knew, that was the
ol
entire experiment. However, there was, of course, more to it than this. The
ch
participant came into the study with two fellow confederates. On learning that
y
the experiment involved watching movies, one of the confederates pulled out
Ps
a small bag of candies and offered some to the participant and the other con-
federate. In half the cases, the candy offerer turned out to be the “disk confed-
an
erate” and in the other half, the offerer turned out to be the “distraction
ic
confederate” (the reason for the names will become obvious in a moment).
er
received a false debriefing and were then dismissed. After they left the lab room,
the distraction confederate buttonholed the participant and started talking to
e
him as the disk confederate rummaged through his own backpack at a table
th
15 feet down the hall. The disk confederate pulled a crumpled piece of paper
by
out of his backpack and asked the participant and confederate, “Do either of
you know where room 25 is? I’ve got to meet a study group there in 5 minutes.”
11
Either the participant or the distraction confederate would reply that room 25
20
was in the basement, and after learning this, the disk confederate packed his
bag up and left. In doing so, however, he “inadvertently” left behind and forgot
©
his bright, neon-colored computer disk that now lay on the desk.
ht
After allowing the disk confederate to make his getaway, the distraction
ig
confederate ended his conversation with the participant. Both of them then
yr
walked down the hall, having to walk past the “lost” disk. If the participant
op
did not notice the disk, the confederate drew attention to it and gave the par-
ticipant a few subtle opportunities to volunteer to find the disk confederate.
C
If the participant did not take up the offer, the study ended. If the participant
did take up the offer, he needed to find the disk confederate, not an easy task
given that (a) the directional signs in the University of Illinois psychology
building are confusing, contradictory, and occasionally completely mislead-
ing (pointing to the wrong floor) and (b) room 25 is actually a locked broom
A)
closet, tucked behind a set of doors at the bottom of a ramp.
Though a few participants could not or did not find room 25, the major-
P
(A
ity of those who attempted to do so succeeded. (Participant progress was mon-
itored by confederates hidden throughout the building and by the exits,
n
communicating via walkie-talkies.) If a participant reached room 25, however,
io
his journey might continue, because room 25 had a sign on the door, “Meet-
at
ing moved to room 841 (8th floor).” The participant then had to decide either
ci
to walk up the nine floors, take the elevator (if he had the time—the elevators
so
were slow), or abandon the search altogether (the building’s exit was located
As
tantalizingly nearby). For those who chose to go on to the eighth floor and
found room 841 (a broom closet again), there was another sign on the door,
al
“Went to get TV and VCR. Will be back soon.” A confederate hidden in the
ic
hallway watched to see whether the participant waited for at least 1 minute
og
for someone to return. The participant’s degree of helping could thus be mea-
ol
sured, with higher scores indicating that the participant had gone further in
ch
trying to find the disk confederate. Because we manipulated whether it was the
y
candy, this situational manipulation dictated whether finding the disk confed-
erate was or was not an act of reciprocity for his offer of the candy.
an
the honor group in the reciprocity condition, there were people who paid
th
back and people who did not. That is, those who paid back insults and affronts
by
(“negative gifts,” as Miller, 1993, called them) were those who also went the
furthest to pay back a favor, traipsing all over the psychology building to help
11
out the person who offered them a piece of candy. Thus, the people who paid
20
back the negative were also the people who paid back the positive. However,
in the nonhonor groups, the pattern was the opposite: Those who rejected
©
retributive violence were most likely to return the favor to the disk confed-
ht
erate who had offered them candy. “Good” people eschewed violence and good
ig
The moral obligation to repay the confederate was binding for both
op
honor culture participants who embraced the ideal of honor and for face and
dignity participants who embraced the ideals of face and dignity, respectively.
C
(With regard to this latter distinction, we further found that the dignity culture
participants who were most likely to pay back a favor were also the ones who
endorsed the notion that personal worth is inalienable; conversely, among
nondignity participants, this relationship between paying back favors and
endorsing the principle of inalienable worth did not hold.) Finding the disk
A)
confederate was a matter of fulfilling a duty to reciprocate rather than a sim-
ple act of altruism. The effects described earlier held for the situation where
P
(A
the disk confederate had offered the participant candy; they did not hold
when the disk confederate had not offered candy.
n
The experiment illustrates in static form the argument that (a) cultural
io
syndromes differ in the way they cluster behaviors together and (b) individ-
at
uals position themselves toward or against the dominant syndromes of their
ci
culture or subculture. The experiment we describe next demonstrates this
so
dynamically, illustrating the way this process works in microcosm. That is,
As
our bicultural honor culture participants (i.e., Latinos and southern Anglos
at the University of Illinois) may live in two worlds and may structure their
al
behavior according to different cultural logics, depending on which one hap-
ic
pens to be salient for them in a given situation.
og
The University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign is an overwhelmingly
ol
White campus in a northern part of the state. Most psychology experiments
ch
there take place in a laboratory room in the Department of Psychology. The
y
honor was “primed” in that all participants watched the film clips before hav-
ing to find the confederate. In the next experiment, half the participants were
an
unprimed, whereas the other half were primed with honor, watching the film
ic
already described).
Am
honor cultures, the mantle of dignity in dignity cultures, and the mantle of
th
For this study, we adapted a procedure from Houston and Ziff (1976),
20
the experimenter “accidentally” left one of these two lists exposed, and the
ht
dependent variable was (roughly) the likelihood that the participant cheated
ig
(as opposed to remembering words from the exposed list due to chance
yr
was measured in the same way as in the previous experiment; however, for
half the participants, the honor–violence film clips were shown before the
C
memory test was given and for the other half, the film clips came after.
A)
from an honor culture who endorses the notion that insults and affronts need
to be paid back with violence if necessary. Imagine person B from an honor
P
(A
culture, a person who lives by the mantra “Sticks and stones may break my
bones but names will never hurt me” and thus rejects honor-related violence.
n
In the unprimed condition, in the context of a psychology laboratory on a
io
northern, predominately White campus, the cultural ideal that is salient and
at
that one reacts toward or against is dignity. In this case, person A, who
ci
embraces the idea of paying back insults, is a rejecter of the ideal of dignity,
so
and as a rejecter of dignity’s ideal, should be less likely to act in an upright
As
and trustworthy fashion. However, person B is an endorser of the dignity
ideal, and as such, should be more likely to act with integrity.
al
Now, change the salient cultural syndrome to which A and B are react-
ic
ing by administering the honor prime. When honor is the salient ideal, per-
og
son A is an endorser of honor and, as such, should follow honor’s strictures
ol
against cheating and be more honest. In contrast, person B goes from being
ch
an endorser of dignity to a rejecter of honor and, as a rejecter of honor, should
y
be more likely to cheat. This is what happened in our experiment. The honor
Ps
prime made endorsers of retributive violence less likely to cheat whereas mak-
ing rejecters of retributive violence more likely to cheat.
an
It interesting that the priming effect was most pronounced among those
ic
most steeped in the culture of honor tradition. In the unprimed condition, for
er
both the honor and the nonhonor groups, more endorsement of violence was
Am
correlated with more cheating. In the primed condition, among those who
grew up entirely in the South, the correlation flipped dramatically: For those
e
honor violence meant embracing all that honor entails, including honesty.
This did not occur for people from nonhonor groups or for people who spent
11
much less of their life in the South. For these groups, one cannot prime—or
20
A few caveats are needed, and they derive from a more general point
ht
about priming. The effectiveness of a prime depends on both (a) the “strength”
ig
of the prime and (b) the participant’s susceptibility to being primed by a given
yr
stimulus. Regarding susceptibility, the strong results with the lifelong South-
op
erners are suggestive but must be regarded with caution due to the small n (we
had only 16 lifelong Southerners in our sample). Although, regarding the
C
strength of the prime, we could not effectively activate honor as an ideal for
our nonhonor groups, this is not to say that the ideal can never be activated.
Our participants watched five film clips. Perhaps stronger primes could induce
honor as a salient ideal, but in the current studies we have no evidence for
this (see also Cohen, 2007).
A)
Cultural Rejectionism
P
(A
As illustrated previously, being an endorser of honor (when honor is the
n
salient ideal) is not the same thing as being a rejecter of dignity (when dig-
io
nity is the salient ideal). And conversely, being a rejecter of honor is not the
at
same thing as being an endorser of dignity. The same inclination toward or
ci
against retributive violence positions one as either a rejecter or endorser of a
so
cultural system (of honor, dignity, or face), and positioning oneself with
As
respect to this cultural system has implications for a wide variety of behav-
iors, including positive reciprocity, honesty, and (as illustrated in our other
al
work not discussed here) financial as well as political actions (see Leung &
ic
Cohen, 2008).
og
A point that these studies reiterate is that cultural rejecters are not sim-
ol
ply oblivious folks who just don’t “get it.” They are not simply undersocial-
ch
ized. Sometimes they understand the cultural ideals perfectly well and just
y
ways that were striking. In the disk study, Southerners and Latinos who
rejected honor-related violence actually helped someone less after that per-
an
son offered them candy than they did when the person did not offer them
ic
candy. The same was true of northern Anglo and Asian Americans who
er
endorsed honor-related violence. And the same was true of northern Anglo
Am
Americans who rejected the ideal of inalienable worth. These participants all
helped the confederate less after the confederate had done something nice for
e
them. They behaved most badly when social obligations pressed most heav-
th
ily on them.
by
motive, or perhaps simply for the sake of spite. There is a Yiddish word—
aftselakhis—translated “very literally, ‘in order to provoke anger,’ the impulse
©
to do things only because someone else doesn’t want you to” (Wex, 2005,
ht
p. 2). In the case of the experiments described here, the word may not apply
ig
because the participants in our studies were not angering the confederate by
yr
not returning his disk. However, the word perhaps captures some of the spirit
op
of how our cultural rejectionists behaved (or misbehaved) when they faced
social obligations. Again, the deeper underlying motives for their behavior,
C
the feelings they experience when they violate social norms (e.g., glee?
A)
results from the second experiment showing that participants from honor
cultures can go from endorsers of dignity to rejecters of honor (or from
P
(A
rejecters of dignity to endorsers of honor) by changing the salience of a cul-
tural syndrome suggests that we should consider not just person-level effects
n
but also the interaction of person-level variables with situational and cul-
io
tural variables as well. (For more on these effects, see Cohen, 2009; Kim &
at
Cohen, 2010.)
ci
so
Rejecting the Logic
As
We have argued that individuals are always in a cultural context,
al
although they are not always of it. That is, people react toward or against the
ic
salient ideals of their culture—embracing them or rejecting them. Individ-
og
uals are “free” to go against the ideals of their culture. Our most important
ol
caveat is that there is another level of freedom beyond this. That is, individ-
ch
uals are free to reject not just the ideals of their culture but also the cultural
y
logic itself that binds together and organizes sets of behaviors, values, and
Ps
weaves together honor by, for example, embracing the notion that one is
er
obligated to pay back good things but not obligated to pay back bad ones or
Am
by embracing the notion that one can show integrity without having to
stand up for him- or herself if challenged. There are obviously people who
e
do this (though in the first experiment there were not many). It seems worth-
th
while to study how, when, and why people challenge not just the ideals but
by
also the underlying logic of their culture. Again, our second study provides
a clue: At least for our bicultural respondents, one might be able to replace
11
one cultural logic (e.g., honor) with that of another (e.g., dignity), or vice
20
versa (see Leung & Cohen, 2009). Particularly in a rather loose social sys-
tem such as that of the United States, people can operate in not just one cul-
©
people pick the niches they occupy in a culture or subculture may be partic-
ig
Our caveat does not modify the claim that individuals are always in a
op
cultural context even if they are not always aware of it. However, it does make
the claim more complex by noting that sometimes people can choose the
C
salient cultural or subcultural system they are responding to. “Choice” does
A)
Benet-Martinez, 2008).
P
(A
CONCLUSION
n
io
In sum, all societies must solve the problem of order. How they solve it
at
and how they conceive of individual worth help to define whether a society
ci
resembles the prototype of an honor, dignity, face, or some other sort of culture.
so
Different cultural logics operate in different cultures, meaning that different
As
sorts of behaviors, scripts, and values get bundled together in different ways.
These bundles are coherent to people in the culture even if they do not always
al
seem coherent to those outside the culture.
ic
Individuals react toward or against the salient ideals of their culture, and
og
even when they choose to reject the culture’s ideals, they are still often guided
ol
by its logic and fit their behavior to preexisting cultural templates. The CuPS
ch
approach helps to explain the distinct patterns of within-culture, as well as
y
who can be counted on to pay back favors. Individuals are always in a cultural
er
context even if they are not always of it. Further research into the way peo-
Am
ple position themselves with respect to cultural ideals, their motives for doing
so, and the forces and choices that influence the cultural syndrome to which
e
REFERENCES
11
20
Ayers, E. (1984). Vengeance and justice. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Cohen, D. (2007). Methods of cultural psychology. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.),
©
Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 196–236). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
ht
A)
face maintenance among Japanese. In E. C. Chang (Ed.), Self-criticism and self-
P
enhancement (pp. 105–122). Washington, DC: American Psychological Associ-
(A
ation. doi:10.1037/11624-007
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D., Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal
n
io
need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, 766–794. doi:10.1037/
at
0033-295X.106.4.766
ci
Ho, D. (1976). On the concept of face. American Journal of Sociology, 81, 867–884.
so
doi:10.1086/226145
As
Houston, J. P., & Ziff, T. (1976). Effects of success and failure on cheating behavior.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 371–376. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.68.3.371
al
Kim, Y. H., & Cohen, D. (2010). Information, perspective, and the self in face
ic
and dignity cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 537–550.
og
doi:10.1177/0146167210362398
ol
Kim, Y. H., Cohen, D., & Au, W. T. (in press). The jury and abjury of my peers. Jour-
ch
nal of Personality and Social Psychology.
y
Leung, A. K.-y., & Cohen, D. (2008). Within- and between-culture variation: Individ-
Ps
ual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. Unpub-
lished manuscript, Singapore Management University.
an
Leung, A. K.-y., & Cohen, D. (2009). The embodiment of moral systems. Unpublished
ic
Pitt-Rivers, J. (1966). Honour and social status. In J. Peristiany (Ed.), Honour and
ht
Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
op
Vandello, J. A., & Cohen, D. (2003). Male honor and female fidelity: Implicit cul-
tural scripts that perpetuate violence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
C
Vandello, J., Cohen, D., Grandon, R., & Franiuk, R. (2009). Stand by your man. Jour-
nal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 81–104. doi:10.1177/0022022108326194
Weber, M. (1997). The theory of social and economic organization. New York, NY:
Free Press. (original work in German published 1920)
A)
Wex, M. (2005). Born to kvetch. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Press.
P
Wrong, D. (1961). The oversocialized conception of man in modern sociology.
(A
American Sociological Review, 26, 183–193. doi:10.2307/2089854
Zou, X., Morris, M., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2008). Identity motives and cultural
n
io
priming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1151–1159. doi:10.1016/
at
j.jesp.2008.02.001
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
III
A)
P
(A
PSYCHOLOGICAL AND
n
io
RELATIONAL PROCESSES
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
12051-12_PT3-CH11-rev2.qxd
11
by
5/25/10
th
e
Am
11:54 AM
er
ic
an
Page 202
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-12_PT3-CH11-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:54 AM Page 203
11
A)
P
MIRROR, MIRROR, ON THE WALL,
(A
WHO’S THE MOST AGGRESSIVE
n
io
OF THEM ALL? NARCISSISM,
at
ci
SELF-ESTEEM, AND AGGRESSION
so
As
SANDER THOMAES AND BRAD J. BUSHMAN
al
ic
What we have to fight for is the necessary security for the existence and
og
increase of our race and people, the subsistence of its children and the
ol
maintenance of our racial stock unmixed, the freedom and independence
ch
of the Fatherland; so that our people may be enabled to fulfill the mission
y
—Adolf Hitler
an
they are better than others? Do they overestimate rather than underestimate
their qualities? Although controversies still exist, research has provided sev-
e
eral important insights into the link between self-views and aggression. This
th
HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVES
ht
ig
causes aggression. Although the origins of this view are difficult to establish
op
explain why this notion sneaked into conventional wisdom. Most important,
203
12051-12_PT3-CH11-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:54 AM Page 204
A)
ing high self-esteem is associated with good things (e.g., behaving prosocially).
Moreover, it is well established that unpleasant events, which make people
P
(A
feel bad, increase aggression (e.g., Berkowitz, 1983). As Nathaniel Branden
(1984), one of the most fervent advocates of the idea that self-esteem is
n
the key determinant of good adjustment, said: “I cannot think of a single
io
psychological problem—from anxiety and depression, to fear of intimacy or
at
of success, to spouse battery or child molestation—that is not traceable to
ci
the problem of low self-esteem” (p.12).
so
A few scholars have gone beyond correlations and tried to explain why
As
low self-esteem should cause aggression. According to one explanation, peo-
ple generally want to experience high self-esteem, so individuals with low
al
self-esteem should suffer from distress and frustration and may behave aggres-
ic
sively in an attempt to feel better about themselves. Aggression is viewed as
og
a strategy to regulate the pain and distress associated with one’s negative self-
ol
feelings (Horney, 1950; Toch, 1969/1993). According to a second explana-
ch
tion, individuals with low self-esteem often reject societal norms, which
y
views and aggression. Is it still valid to believe that low self-esteem is a cause of
er
aggression?
Am
In 1996, Roy Baumeister and his colleagues reviewed the literature and
by
found little support for the view that low self-esteem increases aggression
(Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, 1996). In fact, they concluded that inflated
11
ego. It is possible, however, that more recent findings have yielded a different
picture, especially because the review sparked reevaluations and much new
©
A)
pertinent to the present purposes, weak but significant negative correlations
have also been found between self-esteem and self-reported physical aggres-
P
sion. For example, one study found a correlation of −.11 in a sample of more
(A
than 3,000 undergraduate college students (Donnellan et al., 2005, Study 3).
n
The main exception to this pattern of findings is the absence of a correlation
io
between low self-esteem and self-reported verbal aggression. Thus, although
at
low self-esteem individuals do not claim to be more verbally aggressive, they
ci
do claim to be more prone to anger, more likely to harbor hostile feelings
so
toward others, and more likely to be physically aggressive than their high
As
self-esteem counterparts.
Do these findings justify the claim that low self-esteem is an important
al
cause of aggression? We believe not. Self-report measures are often problem-
ic
atic (e.g., Nisbett & Wilson, 1977), especially when examining aggression.
og
Self-report studies typically find that high self-esteem individuals overstate
ol
their good qualities and understate their negative traits (Baumeister, Camp-
ch
bell, Krueger, & Vohs, 2003). Aggression is generally considered to be socially
y
& Kirkpatrick, 2006). However, this effect became significant only after a
range of closely related self-regard variables (i.e., superiority, mate value,
e
the same lab found no link between self-esteem and the same behavioral
by
opponents with painful noise (e.g., Bushman & Baumeister, 1998; Twenge &
Campbell, 2003), giving a confederate less money (Bushman, Bonacci, van Dijk,
©
violence.
A)
mon in late childhood and adolescence than it is in adulthood, perhaps
increasing the likelihood of establishing a link with low self-esteem. In addi-
P
(A
tion, late childhood and adolescence are developmental periods marked by
profound concern with maintaining desired self-images (Harter, 2006). If it
n
is true that aggression is a regulatory strategy people use to protect their feel-
io
ings of self-worth (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001), then children and adolescents
at
who are low in self-esteem may be more likely than their adult counterparts
ci
to behave aggressively.
so
A pair of studies that used the same longitudinal sample (i.e., a complete
As
birth cohort of over 1,000 participants from Dunedin, New Zealand) provided
evidence that appears to be consistent with this reasoning (Donnellan et al.,
al
2005, Study 2; Trzesniewski et al., 2006). One study found a small but signif-
ic
icant negative relationship between self-esteem at age 11 and teacher- and
og
parent-reported externalizing problems (including some items assessing
ol
aggression) at age 13 (Donnellan et al., 2005). The other study found a nega-
ch
tive relationship between young adolescents’ self-esteem and court convictions
y
respect. They did not rule out the possibility that the link between low self-
er
esteem and aggression was actually due to social or contextual factors related to
Am
both self-esteem and (later) violent and aggressive behavior. This is important
because third variables often inflate the relationship between self-esteem and
e
tried to replicate the findings from these two studies using a similar New Zealand
by
dicted aggressive and violent behavior at ages 18, 21, and 25. However, the rela-
20
esteem and later violent and aggressive behavior, these associations are
yr
in fact very modest in nature, and can be explained by the effects of fam-
op
ily background and contextual factors that are confounded with self-
esteem, rather than the direct effects of self-esteem per se. (Boden et al.,
C
2007, p. 888)
Taken together, many studies have appeared since Baumeister and col-
leagues (1996) challenged the notion that low self-esteem causes aggression. A
few studies have sought to revive the low self-esteem hypothesis. However, these
studies were methodologically flawed in important respects and consistently
A)
failed to replicate when more rigorous research designs were used. Regardless of
whether one studies children, adolescents, or adults, low self-esteem generally
P
(A
fails to predict objective measures of aggressive behavior. Then we might ask
another question: What kind of self-views, if any, predispose people to behave
n
aggressively?
io
at
ci
DOES NARCISSISM INFLUENCE AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR?
so
As
Baumeister and his colleagues (1996) proposed that aggression most
commonly stems from threatened egotism. In other words, people with big
al
egos become aggressive when others threaten their inflated egos. Thus,
ic
“grandiose” and “inflated” self-views, rather than simply “positive” self-
og
views, were predicted to lead to aggressive and violent behavior. Such forms
ol
of exaggerated self-love are characteristic of narcissism. In its extreme form,
ch
narcissism is a personality disorder defined by grandiose self-views and an
y
ality Inventory (NPI). Recent research has confirmed the view that NPI-
Am
results showed that people with low self-esteem were not more aggressive than
20
others. Narcissists who aggressed directly against the person who insulted
them showed the highest aggression levels. The finding that narcissism is pos-
©
et al., 2003; Konrath, Bushman, & Campbell, 2006; Reidy, Zeichner, Foster,
op
& Martinez, 2008; Stucke & Sporer, 2002; Twenge & Campbell, 2003), and
real-world aggression measures (Bushman & Baumeister, 2002; Goldberg
C
et al., 2007).
A)
research have suggested that there are various kinds of narcissists who differ
in their level of self-esteem (Dickinson & Pincus, 2003; Kernberg, 1975;
P
(A
Kohut, 1977). Covert narcissists have relatively low self-esteem and are described
as socially avoidant individuals who are self-absorbed yet shy and introverted.
n
In contrast, overt narcissists have much higher self-esteem and are described
io
as self-assured extraverts who have a dominant interpersonal orientation. We
at
reanalyzed data from a previous experiment (Bushman & Baumeister, 1998)
ci
and conducted a new experiment, finding that aggression was highest in
so
threatened narcissists who also had high levels of self-esteem (i.e., in overt
As
narcissists). To the extent that threatened narcissists harbored somewhat
lower levels of self-esteem (characteristic of covert narcissists), they were
al
much less aggressive.
ic
Some scholars, however, have argued that laboratory findings may not
og
generalize to the real world because the setting and measures are artificial
ol
(e.g., Donnellan et al., 2005). We therefore conducted a follow-up study as a
ch
naturalistic extension of our laboratory work (Bushman et al., 2009, Study 3).
y
from a class were able to evaluate each other’s work and influence each other’s
grade. Ego threat was not manipulated or artificially induced. Instead, it was
an
enced after receiving negative feedback from a fellow student. Similarly, the
er
target of aggression was not an unseen confederate but a fellow student with
Am
actual class work, and they believed they could lower the grades of their eval-
by
experiments. The most aggressive people were those who experienced feel-
20
ings of humiliation and had high levels of both narcissism and self-esteem.
Humiliated narcissists who held lower levels of self-esteem were the least
©
aggressive. Taken together, this set of studies provides new evidence against
ht
the view that low self-esteem causes aggression. There were indirect (rather
ig
than direct) effects of low self-esteem on aggression, but they were in the
yr
A)
are fairly common in late childhood and adolescence, due in part to develop-
mental increases in self-consciousness (Ryan & Kuczkowski, 1994). In our
P
(A
experiment, participants lost to an ostensible opponent in a competitive task.
In the shame condition, they were told their opponent was really bad on the
n
task and that they should easily win. After losing, they saw their own name at
io
the bottom of a ranking list on a bogus web page, below their opponent’s name.
at
In the control condition, they were told nothing about their opponent and did
ci
not see any rankings. Next, participants could blast their opponent with loud
so
noise through headphones (a measure of aggression). Consistent with previ-
As
ous studies of adults, narcissists were more aggressive than others but only
when they were shamed. No support was found for the traditional view that
al
low self-esteem underlies aggression. In fact, that view was contradicted by the
ic
finding that high self-esteem increased narcissistic shame-induced aggression.
og
Once again, low self-esteem eliminated the aggressive behavior characteristic
of shamed narcissists. ol
ch
Taken together, these studies indicate that narcissistic self-views predis-
y
pose people to behave aggressively. The link between narcissism and aggres-
Ps
Narcissists are especially aggressive when their grandiose self-images are chal-
ic
Controversies
th
by
cerns the shape of the relation between self-views and aggression. One might
wonder whether the relationship is U-shaped, such that people holding neg-
©
ative, deflated self-views and people holding positive, inflated self-views are
ht
ries that argue that extremes of self-esteem (both low and high) are maladap-
yr
tive because they typically reflect distorted perceptions (Jahoda, 1958; Kernis,
op
2003). Prior research has mainly tested linear relationships between self-views
and aggression. We know of only two studies that examined the possibility of
C
a curvilinear relationship (Perez, Vohs, & Joiner, 2005; Webster, 2007). The
results from both studies provided little support for a U-shaped relationship, but
both studies used self-report measures of aggression, which as we noted earlier
are problematic. Thus, the issue remains open and awaits further scientific
scrutiny.
A)
Another controversy—one that we already touched on briefly—concerns
the best way to measure aggressive behavior (see Chapters 1, 8, and 9). Some
P
(A
scholars have faulted laboratory aggression studies because they are artificial.
Indeed, participants in laboratory studies are likely aware that researchers will
n
not let them seriously injure or abuse anyone. However, research consistently
io
finds that the same factors that increase aggression in the real world also
at
increase aggression in the laboratory and that the same individual differences
ci
in aggression that exist in the real world also exist in the laboratory (Ander-
so
son & Bushman, 1997; Anderson, Lindsay, & Bushman, 1999). Clearly, one
As
advantage of field studies of aggression is that they do not suffer from lack of
ecological validity. However, field studies typically use informant-based mea-
al
sures of aggression (e.g., teacher, parent, or peer report measures) that also
ic
have limitations (see Chapter 7, this volume). Most important, informants
og
may not validly report on targets’ aggression in response to specific situational
ol
contexts, which makes it difficult to use informant-based measures to test
ch
situation-dependent theories of aggression (e.g., threatened egotism theory).
y
like a doughnut with an empty hole in the middle). Is there any evidence to
er
In fact, the theory is directly contradicted by the finding that low self-esteem
th
esteem does not cause aggression, how could hidden low self-esteem cause
aggression? However, recent developments indicate that the distinction
11
high or low explicit and implicit self-esteem), but other people hold “dis-
ht
crepant” explicit and implicit self-esteem. The doughnut theory argues that
ig
port for this notion. One study involving middle school students found an
A)
ciate positive and negative words with themselves; Sandstrom & Jordan,
2008). We know of no research designed to replicate this finding with adults.
P
(A
However, there is evidence that narcissistic adults can have high explicit and
low implicit self-esteem (Jordan, Spencer, Zanna, Hoshino-Browne, & Correll,
n
2003). Future research is needed to determine whether these kinds of adults
io
are especially prone to aggression.
at
A final controversy concerns how aggression is conceptualized. Aggres-
ci
sion is generally defined as any behavior intended to harm another person who
so
does not want to be harmed (Bushman & Thomaes, 2008). Some researchers
As
have focused on more broadly defined categories of behavior that include not
only aggression but also other antisocial behaviors such as lying, stealing, van-
al
dalism, and being disruptive in class (e.g., Donnellan et al., 2005; Trzesniewski
ic
et al., 2006). It is entirely plausible that the relationship between self-views and
og
aggression is different from the relationship between self-views and other
ol
antisocial behaviors (for a review of different genetic markers for aggression and
ch
other antisocial behaviors, see Chapter 8, this volume). Low self-esteem may
y
foster a tendency to break society’s rules because people with low self-esteem
Ps
lying, cheating, or stealing. Indeed, most research suggests that low self-esteem
ic
2006). There is abundant evidence, however, that low self-esteem does not
make people more likely to behave aggressively. We believe the field will
e
Clinical Implications
©
What do the research findings reviewed in this chapter imply for pre-
ht
sion and violence are complex phenomena that have multiple causes (e.g.,
yr
Chapters 1, 2, and 7). Individual differences in aggression are stable over time
op
and typically emerge by early childhood (see Chapter 7). This means that
effective prevention and intervention strategies should be (a) broadly targeted
C
in children’s development (e.g., Dodge, Coie, & Lynam, 2006; see also
Chapter 9, this volume).
Among the many factors that influence aggression, perpetrators’ self-
regard is important. Until now, many prevention and intervention strate-
A)
gies designed to reduce aggression in children and adolescents have been
aimed at boosting their self-esteem (e.g., Kusché & Greenberg, 1994). For
P
(A
example, boosting self-esteem is a central goal of the Promoting Alterna-
tive Thinking Strategies program, one of the most popular classroom-based
n
aggression prevention programs worldwide. According to the author of the
io
program, “high self-esteem makes it more likely that children will develop
at
internal motivation to eschew antisocial behavior and violence,” whereas
ci
“low self-esteem is emotionally painful and frequently results in . . . angry
so
outbursts including violent and delinquent behavior” (Kusché, 2002,
As
pp. 294–295). The empirical evidence we have reviewed in this chapter
contradicts these claims. In fact, if well-intended efforts to boost self-
al
esteem cultivate the inflated self-views characteristic of narcissism, they
ic
may inadvertently increase (rather than decrease) the aggressive behavior
og
of at-risk youth.
ol
The existing evidence suggests that prevention and intervention strate-
ch
gies should target inflated (rather than deflated) self-views to reduce aggression.
y
in (a) being aware of their strengths and weaknesses, (b) being able to present
ic
themselves in an open and truthful way to others rather than rigidly seeking
er
to promote their self-image, and (c) being able to process self-relevant infor-
Am
hood and the foundation for lifelong aggressive behavior styles is laid in child-
th
in childhood.
One other perhaps complementary way of reducing narcissistic aggres-
11
sion is based on the rationale that narcissists are aggressive only when their
20
egos are threatened and that intervention techniques that reduce the psy-
chological impact of ego-threatening experiences may thus lessen narcissis-
©
sense of self (Sherman & Cohen, 2006; Steele, 1988). Self-affirmation theory
ig
social-evaluative stress (e.g., Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, & Master, 2006; Sherman
& Cohen, 2006).
We recently conducted a field experiment to examine whether self-
affirmation can reduce narcissistic aggression (Thomaes, Bushman, Orobio
A)
de Castro, Cohen, & Denissen, 2009). Participants were 405 middle-school
students who completed either a self-affirmation or a control writing exercise
P
(A
in their classes. In the affirmation condition, participants wrote about their
most important values and why these are important to them. In the control
n
condition, participants wrote about their least important values and why
io
these may be important to others (Cohen et al., 2006). The intervention
at
was administered on a Monday morning so that we could examine its possi-
ci
ble effect on aggressive incidents in class during the following week. These
so
aggressive incidents were measured on Friday afternoon using peer reports.
As
We also obtained a measure of ego threats that had been experienced during
the past week. In the control condition, the now familiar pattern of results
al
emerged, with narcissists being more aggressive than others but only when
ic
they experienced relatively high levels of ego threat. In the intervention con-
og
dition, however, that standard pattern of results was eliminated by the
ol
self-affirmation writing exercise. Thus, as predicted, the self-affirmation inter-
ch
vention reduced narcissistic aggression for a period of 1 week, presumably by
y
attenuating the ego protective motives that normally drive narcissists’ aggres-
Ps
sion. We hope this result will encourage the development of theory and evidence-
based aggression interventions aimed at buttressing people’s self-views against
an
ego threats.
ic
er
Am
FUTURE RESEARCH
e
to test this hypothesis and to help clinicians identify critical age periods in
ig
A)
ing to this perspective, children may learn to seek continuous attention and
admiration to compensate for a lack of parental warmth. The few empirical
P
(A
studies available provide some evidence for both theories (Horton, Bleau, &
Drwecki, 2006; Otway & Vignoles, 2006). These data, however, were based
n
on retrospective adult reports of childhood experiences. Longitudinal research
io
that includes early assessments of parenting behavior and children’s experience
at
with parents is needed. This research should also examine possible interactions
ci
with children’s genetically based temperamental traits.
so
Second, virtually all research on self-views and aggression has been con-
As
ducted in individualistic countries (e.g., United States, Canada, Great Britain,
Germany, the Netherlands, Finland, Australia, New Zealand). Research sug-
al
gests that self-views may play a different motivational role in the lives of peo-
ic
ple from collectivistic cultures. Whereas people from individualistic cultures
og
are typically motivated to enhance their self-views, people from collectivis-
ol
tic cultures tend to be less concerned about sustaining favorable self-views, at
ch
least as a goal in itself (Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama, 1999). People
y
from collectivistic cultures tend to view themselves in terms of the social roles
Ps
they play and the relationships they have with others (see Chapter 10, this
volume). In fact, it has been argued that the concept of self-esteem is itself a
an
result from a sense of satisfaction with the social role one plays rather than
er
esteem (Leary, Tate, Adams, Allen, & Hancock, 2007). Whereas self-esteem
essentially involves a judgment of oneself, self-compassion involves an orien-
C
A)
2007), and so it may also mitigate the aggressive behaviors that often follow
from such events.
P
(A
We recommend that researchers broaden their scope and begin to study
self-compassion or related adaptive self-traits that may reduce aggressive
n
behavior. It would be particularly informative if researchers could show that
io
self-compassion plays a causal role in determining people’s inclinations to
at
aggress. Research has shown that it is possible to induce feelings of self-
ci
compassion, allowing for such a causal test (Leary et al., 2006). This would
so
not only increase our knowledge of how the self is involved in aggressive
As
behavior, but it might also suggest further ways to use self-views as a remedy
for violence and aggression.
al
ic
og
REFERENCES ol
ch
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
y
Ps
doi:10.1037/1089-2680.1.1.19
er
Anderson, C. A., Lindsay, J. J., & Bushman, B. J. (1999). Research in the psycholog-
Am
Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-
th
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism
20
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1135
yr
Boden, J. M., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. (2007). Self-esteem and violence:
op
Testing links between adolescent self-esteem and later hostility and violent
behavior. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 42, 881–891. doi:10.1007/
C
s00127-007-0251-7
Bradshaw, C. P., & Hazan, C. (2006). Examining views of self in relation to views of
others: Implications for research on aggression and self-esteem. Journal of Research
in Personality, 40, 1209–1218. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.004
Branden, N. (1984, August–September). In defense of self. Association for Humanistic
A)
Psychology, pp. 12–13.
Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-
P
(A
esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-hate lead to
violence? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 219–229. doi:10.1037/
n
0022-3514.75.1.219
io
Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Does self-love or self-hate lead to vio-
at
lence. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 543–545. doi:10.1016/S0092-
ci
6566(02)00502-0
so
Bushman, B. J., Baumeister, R. F., Thomaes, S., Ryu, E., Begeer, S., & West, S. G.
As
(2009). Looking again, and harder, for a link between low self-esteem and aggres-
sion. Journal of Personality, 77, 427–446. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00553.x
al
Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., van Dijk, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Narcis-
ic
sism, sexual refusal, and aggression: Testing a narcissistic reactance model of
og
sexual coercion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1027–1040.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1027 ol
ch
Bushman, B. J., & Thomaes, S. (2008). Aggression. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs
y
(Eds.), Encyclopedia of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 20–25). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Ps
Sage.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006, September). Reducing the
an
1307–1310. doi:10.1126/science.1128317
er
pedi.17.3.188.22146
by
Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., & Lynam, D. (2006). Aggression and antisocial behavior
in youth. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psy-
11
chology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 719–788). New
20
D’zurilla, T. J., Chang, E. C., & Sanna, L. J. (2003). Self-esteem and social problem
yr
A)
esteem, narcissism, and theory of mind deficits. Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, 195, 436–442.
P
(A
Harter, S. (2006). The self. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.),
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development
n
(pp. 505–570). New York, NY: Wiley.
io
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a univer-
at
sal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106, 766–794. doi:10.1037/
ci
0033-295X.106.4.766
so
Horney, K. (1950). Neurosis and human growth. New York, NY: Norton.
As
Horton, R. S., Bleau, G., & Drwecki, B. (2006). Parenting narcissus: Does parenting
contribute to the development of narcissism? Journal of Personality, 74, 345–376.
al
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00378.x
ic
Jahoda, M. (1958). Current concepts of positive mental health. New York, NY: Basic
og
Books. doi:10.1037/11258-000
ol
Jordan, C. H., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Hoshino-Browne, E., & Correll, J. (2003).
ch
Secure and defensive high self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
y
Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. New York, NY:
Jason Aronson.
an
Kirkpatrick, L. A., Waugh, C. E., Valencia, A., & Webster, G. D. (2002). The func-
Am
0022-3514.82.5.756
th
Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. Madison, WI: International Universities
by
Press.
Konrath, S., Bushman, B. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2006). Attenuating the link
11
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01818.x
Kusché, C. (2002). Psychoanalysis as prevention. Using PATHS to enhance ego-
©
Kusché, C., & Greenberg, M. (1994). PATHS: Promoting alternative thinking strate-
yr
Leary, M. R., Tate, E. B., Adams, C. E., Allen, A. B., & Hancock, J. (2007). Self-
compassion and reactions to unpleasant self-relevant events: The implications
of treating oneself kindly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,
887–904. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.887
A)
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cogni-
tion, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.
P
(A
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality. DSM-III: Axis II. New York, NY: Wiley.
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A
n
dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 177–196.
io
doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1204_1
at
Neff, K. D. (2003a). The development and validation of a scale to measure self-
ci
compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223–250. doi:10.1080/15298860309027
so
Neff, K. D. (2003b). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy
As
attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85–101. doi:10.1080/15298860309032
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal
al
reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231–259. doi:10.1037/
ic
0033-295X.84.3.231
og
Otway, L. J., & Vignoles, V. L. (2006). Narcissism and childhood recollections: A
ol
quantitative test of psychoanalytic predictions. Personality and Social Psychology
ch
Bulletin, 32, 104–116. doi:10.1177/0146167205279907
y
Perez, M., Vohs, K. D., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2005). Discrepancies between self- and
Ps
Reidy, D. E., Zeichner, A., Foster, J. D., & Martinez, M. A. (2008). Effects of narcis-
ic
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
e
doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00292.x
Sandstrom, M. J., & Jordan, R. (2008). Defensive self-esteem and aggression in child-
11
psychology (Vol. 38, pp. 183–242). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.doi:10.1016/
ht
S0065-2601(06)38004-5
ig
the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21,
op
A)
mental field study. Psychological Science, 20, 1536–1542.
Thomaes, S., Bushman, B. J., Stegge, H., & Olthof, T. (2008). Trumping shame by
P
(A
blasts of noise: Narcissism, self-esteem, shame, and aggression in young adoles-
cents. Child Development, 79, 1792–1801. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01226.x
n
Thomaes, S., Stegge, H., Bushman, B. J., Olthof, T., & Denissen, J. (2008). Develop-
io
ment and validation of the Childhood Narcissism Scale. Journal of Personality
at
Assessment, 90, 382–391. doi:10.1080/00223890802108162
ci
Toch, H. (1993). Violent men: An inquiry into the psychology of violence. Washington,
so
DC: American Psychological Association. (Original work published 1969)
As
Trzesniewski, K. H., Donnellan, M. B., Moffitt, T. E., Robins, R. W., Poulton, R., &
Caspi, A. (2006). Low self-esteem during adolescence predicts poor health,
al
criminal behavior, and limited economic prospects during adulthood. Develop-
ic
mental Psychology, 42, 381–390. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.381
og
Twenge, J. M. (2006). Generation me: Why today’s young Americans are more confident,
ol
assertive, entitled—and more miserable than ever before. New York, NY: Free Press.
ch
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2003). “Isn’t it fun to get the respect that we’re
y
j.jrp.2007.01.001
er
11
by
5/25/10
th
e
Am
11:54 AM
er
ic
an
Page 220
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-13_CH12-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:55 AM Page 221
12
PA)
EVOLVED MECHANISMS FOR
(A
REVENGE AND FORGIVENESS
n
io
at
ci
MICHAEL E. MCCULLOUGH, ROBERT KURZBAN,
so
AND BENJAMIN A. TABAK
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
cognitive systems that underlie humans’ capacities for revenge and forgiveness.
er
but socially important as well. In developed nations, the desire for revenge is cited
as a causal factor in as many as 20% of homicides (Kubrin & Weitzer, 2003).
e
damage in the United Kingdom cite the desire for revenge as a motive
by
(Home Office, 2003), and 61% of U.S. school shootings between 1974 and
June 2000 were vengeance-motivated (Vossekuil, Fein, Reddy, Borum, &
11
Modzeleski, 2002). The desire for revenge also motivates people to enlist in
20
has been fashionable in Western thought since the Stoic (and, later, Christian)
ht
We gratefully acknowledge the support of the National Institute of Mental Health Grant 5R01MH071258,
C
a grant from the Fetzer Institute, and support from the Center for the Study of Law and Religion at
Emory University to the first author.
221
12051-13_CH12-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:55 AM Page 222
social scientists in the past century have also promulgated the idea that the
desire for revenge is indicative of psychological dysfunction (Murphy, 2003).
The “revenge as disease” conceit had a predictable effect on how forgive-
ness came to be studied empirically as well: If the desire for revenge is a disease,
A)
then perhaps forgiveness is the cure. Indeed, many of the earliest empirical
studies on forgiveness were related to the use of interventions for promoting
P
(A
forgiveness in therapeutic settings (DiBlasio & Benda, 1991; Hebl & Enright,
1993). These treatments do promote forgiveness—and reduce psychological
n
symptoms of anxiety and depression and boost self-esteem (Lundahl, Taylor,
io
Stevenson, & Roberts, 2008)—but such facts do not even come close to
at
proving that forgiveness is a “cure” for revenge.
ci
Evolutionary research and scholarship cast considerable doubt on “disease”
so
and “cure” conceits for conceptualizing the human capacities for revenge and
As
forgiveness. In this chapter, we propose that revenge and forgiveness are the
results of distinct psychological adaptations that evolved to solve specific
al
adaptive problems. We posit that one or more revenge mechanisms evolved
ic
because of their efficacy in deterring interpersonal harms and that one or
og
more forgiveness mechanisms evolved because of their efficacy in preserving
ol
valuable relationships despite those harms. Here, we attempt to define revenge
ch
and forgiveness in functional terms that will make them more amenable to
y
that gave rise to them, and to outline what we think are the proximate causes
and the computations involved when people make choices to forgive or to
an
avenge a wrong.
ic
er
Am
EVOLUTION OF REVENGE
e
that might have given rise to psychological mechanisms that produce revenge
20
as a functional output.
©
A great deal of research and writing has been devoted to revenge, and
yr
take a step back and reflect on the evolved function of putative revenge systems.
At its heart, revenge solves a problem that is faced to varying degrees by many
C
benefits and refrain from imposing costs on oneself (see Chapter 3). To the
extent that other organisms can learn that a target organism will retaliate
(or conditionally benefit) as a function of their behavior, it is beneficial for
the target organism to signal that it will do so. One (albeit imperfect) way
A)
to signal that one will retaliate if harmed (or benefit if helped) is to actually
do so. If neural tissue is assembled that reliably motivates these sorts of
P
(A
contingent punishments and contingent rewards, it may boost lifetime repro-
ductive fitness of its bearer and therefore evolve precisely because of these
n
functions. Our analysis begins with this simple, but crucial, idea.
io
This notion contrasts with the way some philosophers have defined
at
revenge, but we think some of the previous definitions create as many problems
ci
as they solve. For example, Govier (2002) wrote, “When we seek revenge, we
so
seek satisfaction by attempting to harm the other (or associated persons)
As
as a retaliatory measure” (p. 2). Elster (1990) likewise defined revenge as “the
attempt, at some cost or risk to oneself, to impose suffering upon those who
al
have made one suffer, because they have made one suffer” (p. 862). Uniacke
ic
(2000) also claimed that “revenge is personal and noninstrumental: With
og
revenge we seek to make people suffer because they have made us suffer, not
ol
because their actions or values require us to bring them down” (p. 62).
ch
These definitions, because they are proximate and do not commit to
y
explanation that must be paid. Why should revenge produce pleasure? For no
ic
costly behavior such as revenge unless the mechanism that creates revenge was
th
structure that does yield fitness payoffs (Andrews, Gangestad, & Matthews,
2002)? What could maintain revenge in humans’ behavioral repertoire?
11
the behaviour of other organisms, which evolved because of that effect, and
which is effective because the receiver’s response has also evolved” (p. 3).
By designating a function, it becomes possible to search for evidence of the
features—behavioral or physiological—that contribute to accomplishing the
putative function.
A)
In similar fashion, we define revenge functionally as behavior resulting from
a mechanism designed to deter the imposition of costs on (or the withholding
P
(A
of benefits from) oneself or one’s allies by the imposition of costs following
a target’s imposition of costs (or withholding of benefits), where costs and
n
benefits are defined in terms of their effects on lifetime reproductive fitness.
io
That is, revenge is a deterrence system designed to change others’ incentives
at
regarding the self and one’s kin or allies (see Chapter 4 for a similar functional
ci
analysis of what the chapter authors call the power behavioral system). By impos-
so
ing costs after harm (or withheld benefits), revenge signals that subsequent
As
acts will be subject to the same contingent response, thereby altering others’
incentives. We hypothesize that humans possess psychological adaptations
al
designed specifically to produce revenge.
ic
This functional definition has several important features. First, it replaces
og
considerations of intentionality (e.g., whether the organism is deliberately
ol
or consciously attempting to do something) with considerations of design
ch
(e.g., what the system that motivates revenge was designed to do). Moreover,
y
strong claim (Williams, 1966), and to the extent that the psychological mech-
anisms do not show features that support a deterrence function, the hypothesis
an
iatory impositions of costs that are caused by a mechanism designed for this
Am
purpose, even acts that are not based on deliberation or awareness and even
those that do not actually manage to deter anything (as when people behave
e
on the road). Such a definition also permits a distinction between costs to the
by
provoker that arise from design for that function versus costs that arise as a
by-product. Harming a provoker is only revenge when the system that motivated
11
the harmful behavior was crafted for that purpose. Avoiding a provoker to
20
avert a second harm is not revenge, but avoiding a provoker to limit his or her
access to benefits might be. Likewise, the phenomenon of displaced aggression,
©
Pedersen, Earleywine, & Pollock, 2003; see also Chapter 6, this volume), may
ig
not be revenge, even if the third party is a genetic relative or ally of the original
yr
A)
displaced aggression as revenge” hypothesis—though it might be wrong—
would likely never have been generated solely by relying on the standard,
P
(A
nonfunctional framework that researchers commonly use to understand dis-
placed aggression.
n
io
at
Selection Pressures That Gave Rise to Mechanisms for Revenge
ci
so
In an influential review article, Clutton-Brock and Parker (1995) noted
that retaliation (which they called punishment) is common among nonhuman
As
animals (for a more recent example, see Jensen, Call, & Tomasello, 2007).
al
They speculated that retaliation yields fitness gains by reducing the probability
ic
that the targets of retaliation will repeat their injurious actions against the
og
retaliator in the future. Consistent with Clutton-Brock and Parker’s analysis,
we hypothesize that natural selection gave rise to one or more comparable
ol
deterrence systems in humans. In this sense, the adaptive consequences of
ch
revenge come not from what revenge causes per se but from what it prevents.
y
Ps
ring the imposition of costs. The third involves deterring the withholding
of benefits.
ic
er
Am
Direct Deterrence
By direct deterrence, we mean that revenge discourages aggressors from
e
harming the avenger a second time. The logic of direct deterrence is straight-
th
take an action that imposes costs on a potential victim to acquire some benefit,
then the potential victim is better off if he or she can change the potential
11
against the potential victim a second time (see Chapter 3). Nevertheless, direct
ig
at Time 1 might predict revenge at Time 2, there is nothing that forces this to
op
be true. An organism could be, for example, intermittently vengeful. This leads
to well-known problems of signaling that one’s vengeful dispositions are stable
C
A)
chooses only after seeing the first player’s choice. In such games, the second
player is much more likely to cooperate after a cooperative move than after
P
(A
a defecting move. More relevant to our present point, defection is almost
always met with retaliatory defection (Clark & Sefton, 2001, Table 6), an
n
observation that holds not only in the United States (Hayashi, Ostrom,
io
Walker, & Yamagishi, 1999). However, because noncooperation and punish-
at
ment are the same in the prisoner’s dilemma, such findings must be interpreted
ci
with care.
so
In the iterated prisoner’s dilemma, subjects play multiple rounds of the
As
game with either the same partner or different ones. For the present pur-
pose, key issues are whether people respond to defection with defection—
al
moves plausibly interpretable as revenge—and whether such moves elicit
ic
subsequent cooperation from one’s partner. Experiments using large num-
og
bers of trials in prisoner’s dilemma games suggest that people do respond to
ol
defection with defection (Bixenstine & Wilson, 1963), though the details
ch
vary across studies (Rapoport & Chammah, 1965). Reciprocal strategies
y
such as “tit for tat” or variants of it tend to elicit cooperation from experimen-
Ps
dyadic negotiation in which partners played 250 consecutive trials during which
er
they could either punish, reward, or withhold reward (and punishment) from
Am
each other, Molm (1997) found that the frequency with which retaliatory
punishment was used (i.e., the infliction of punishment after one’s negotiation
e
partner had previously punished the actor) was positively associated with the
th
frequency with which partners rewarded each other. Likewise, the use of
by
also illustrates this basic point. Consider the difference in play in the dictator
game (DG) as opposed to the ultimatum game (UG). In both games, some
amount of money, say $10, is to be divided between two people. In the DG,
one person unilaterally decides how to split the money. In the UG, one person,
A)
the “proposer,” proposes a split, and the other person, the “responder,” can
either accept that split or reject it, in which case both players receive nothing.
P
(A
Rejection in the UG is revenge; the cost imposed is the amount that the pro-
poser allocated to him- or herself. It is not surprising that typical proposals in
n
the UG (roughly 40% of the stake), in which revenge is possible, are larger
io
than in the DG (roughly 20% of the stake), in which revenge is not possible
at
(Forsythe, Horowitz, Savin, & Sefton, 1994).
ci
Social psychology experiments also show how the prospect of suffering
so
revenge can deter aggressors from harming the prospective avenger. In one study
As
(Diamond, 1977), undergraduate men wrote an essay, which a confederate
then derogated. Participants were then brought back to the laboratory 24 hours
al
later and were given the opportunity to give 10 (bogus) shocks of varying
ic
intensities to the person who wrote the insulting reviews. Half were led to
og
believe that after they administered shocks, they would then switch roles and
ol
receive the shocks themselves. People who believed that they could harm their
ch
insulting evaluators without the threat of retaliation gave stronger shocks to
y
the evaluators.
Ps
used the trust game, a two-step dyadic game in which an “investor” first entrusts
ic
a sum of money to a second person called the “trustee”; the money is then
er
step, the trustee is given the opportunity to return some amount of money to
the investor. Fehr and List permitted investors to indicate a minimum amount
e
of money they required from their trustees in return. If trustees failed to return
th
that minimum amount, that amount was automatically deducted from the
by
trustees’ payoffs. Return transfers were highest when this punishment option
was available but left unused by investors. Nonetheless, the majority of investors
11
used the punishment option. Houser et al. (2008), using a similar design, found
20
that the threat of punishment reduced the fraction of money trustees returned
to investors even if the threat of punishment was applied as a result of a random
©
process rather than as a decision on the part of the investor. Likewise, revenge
ht
Dreber, Rand, Fudenberg, and Nowak (2008) found that using punishment
yr
possibly because punishment in this experiment imposed large costs: The cost
of punishment and the size of the damage it inflicts clearly influence revenge’s
C
deterrent effects.
A)
how third-party deterrence might work. Ancestral humans lived in small, close-
P
knit groups (Boehm, 2008) without the benefit of institutions for protecting
(A
individual rights, so a readiness to retaliate against interpersonal harms might
have been an important component of people’s social reputations. Researchers
n
io
have documented the importance of defense of honor (i.e., more or less, the
at
perceived ability to defend one’s interests with violent force when necessary)
ci
and the revenge that it stimulates as a major cause of violence among indi-
so
viduals from many societies (for a review, see McCullough, 2008; see also
Chapter 10, this volume).
As
Consistent with the idea that revenge is enacted partly out of reputational
al
concerns, laboratory studies show that the psychological mechanisms that cause
ic
revenge are sensitive to the presence of third parties. Victims retaliate more
og
strongly against their provokers when an audience has witnessed the provo-
cation, especially if the audience communicates to the victim that he or she
ol
looks weak because of the harm suffered or if the victim knows that the audience
ch
is aware that he or she has suffered particularly unjust treatment (Brown, 1968;
y
Kim, Smith, & Brigham, 1998). Also, when two men get into an argument,
Ps
the mere presence of a third person doubles the likelihood that the argument
an
delivery of benefits (as opposed to only reducing harm). Public goods games
th
are useful for illustrating how revenge can deter the withholding of benefits.
by
In these games, a few (often four to six) participants receive an initial endow-
ment of money and are instructed to choose how to split that endowment
11
between two different pools. One pool is private, and participants simply keep
20
any money they place in it. The other pool is shared; money placed into this
pool is multiplied by some amount greater than one, and the resultant total
©
is subsequently divided evenly among all the players in the group. Money
ht
are social dilemmas because they create a tension between individual and
op
group outcomes. (The fact that they involve groups rather than dyads is
incidental.)
C
Yamagishi (1986) had subjects play public goods games in groups of four,
repeated over 12 trials. He varied whether participants could punish other
members of the group and varied the price of punishment, that is, the cost one
had to spend to reduce another player’s payoff by one unit. Players used the sanc-
A)
tioning system when it was provided, and, in its presence, players contributed
greater amounts to the public good. However, these results do not distinguish
P
(A
the proximate motive, that is, whether sanctioning is instrumental (i.e., the
result of a motive to increase one’s benefits through the use of incentives) or
n
vengeful (i.e., the result of a motive to impose costs on individuals who had
io
an opportunity to deliver benefits but chose not to do so).
at
Fehr and Gächter’s (2002) results help to clarify the proximate motive
ci
for punishment in this context. Fehr and Gächter ran a similar game with a
so
few modifications, the most important of which was that players changed
As
groups from round to round, so punishment could not be used to induce group
members who were uncooperative in round r to benefit the subject in round
al
r + 1. Nevertheless, their results were similar to Yamagishi’s (1986): Participants
ic
sanctioned uncooperative group members, and group members cooperated more
og
when the punishment option was available (see also Anderson & Putterman,
ol
2006). These results imply the operation of the revenge system, given that
ch
instrumental motives were ruled out. Fehr and Gächter would not agree. They
y
noniterated public goods games and varied whether participants could punish
er
members of their own groups or members of other people’s groups. In the key
Am
themselves did not belong strongly suggests that without the possibility of
by
EVOLUTION OF FORGIVENESS
©
also outline the evolutionary selection pressures that might have given rise to
psychological mechanisms that produce forgiveness as a functional output.
C
A)
direct deterrence, third-party deterrence, and, possibly, deterrence of benefit-
withholding. However, avengers trade off the potential benefits lost by virtue
P
(A
of any damage that revenge does to relations with the harm doer, and they
incur the (probabilistic) costs associated with any counterrevenge that might
n
ensue as the result of their revenge. We therefore presume that the revenge
io
system is designed to adjust its operation in response to the potential costs
at
and benefits associated with revenge in any particular instance. When the
ci
costs of revenge are too high relative to its expected deterrence benefits, an
so
organism might pursue an alternative course of behavior—forgiveness being
As
one of the more likely ones.
Over the past decade, the first author’s research group has defined
al
forgiveness as a set of motivational changes whereby an organism becomes
ic
(a) decreasingly motivated to retaliate against an offending relationship partner;
og
(b) decreasingly motivated to avoid the offender; and (c) increasingly moti-
ol
vated by good will for, and a desire to reconcile with, the offender, despite the
ch
offender’s harmful actions (McCullough, 2008; McCullough, Worthington, &
y
the important conceptual distinctions that other theorists (e.g., Enright &
Am
less avoidant, and more benevolently disposed toward him or her) without
ht
modern humans are capable of forgiving because ancestral humans who deployed
yr
this strategy enjoyed the fitness benefits that came from restoring potentially
op
valuable relationships.
Nevertheless, forgiveness, like revenge, involves costs. Forgiveness
C
the prospect of capturing benefits from that relationship, but forgiveness entails
foregoing revenge and its deterrent effects. Forgiveness, therefore, involves a
loss of gains from changing the harm doer’s incentives, potentially inviting
recidivism (e.g., see Gordon, Burton, & Porter, 2004) and attacks from
A)
those who see the opportunity to exploit the forgiver. In short, forgiveness
undermines the function of the revenge system by undermining deterrence.
P
(A
Thus, a forgiveness system, like a revenge system, should be sensitive to costs
and benefits, and these costs and benefits should have shaped the suite of
n
proximate social–psychological factors that turn the system on and off.
io
at
Forgiveness: Selection Pressures
ci
so
As noted earlier, we hypothesize that putative forgiveness systems evolved
As
in response to selection pressures for restoring relationships that, on average,
would have boosted lifetime reproductive fitness, a quality that researchers
al
have called relationship value (de Waal, 2000). The role of relationship value
ic
in determining animals’ propensity to forgive and/or reconcile after conflict
og
has been demonstrated in many simulations of the evolution of cooperation
ol
among dyads and networks of individuals (e.g., Axelrod, 1984; Hruschka &
ch
Henrich, 2006). Similar findings (Koski, Koops, & Sterck, 2007; Watts, 2006)
y
After all, imposing costs on one’s close genetic relatives directly impairs one’s
own inclusive fitness. Also, kin are most likely, all else being equal, to be the
e
source of direct and reciprocal benefits for reasons associated with kin altruism.
th
kin relationships, with closer relatives being more easily forgiven than more
distant ones.
11
such contexts would acquire two fitness benefits. First, forgiving isolated
ht
transgressions would have inhibited the echo effect (Axelrod, 1984), whereby
ig
noise. Second, individuals who can forgive their reciprocal altruism partners
following defections would have been able to preserve their access to benefits
C
that their partners would have been able to provide them and would have
A)
retaliate, or to withdraw from the relationship, following an isolated defection.
Indeed, in computer simulations of the evolution of reciprocal altruism—
P
(A
especially when the possibility of noise is assumed—evolutionarily stable
strategies tend to be more forgiving than tit for tat, which responds to defection
n
with defection and to cooperation with cooperation (Frean, 1994; Hauert &
io
Schuster, 1998; Wu & Axelrod, 1995). This is especially true when one
at
models reciprocal altruism as occurring largely among small networks of
ci
individuals (e.g., friendship groups, individuals within small living groups)
so
who focus their cooperative efforts on other individuals within the net-
As
work and limit their cooperation with individuals outside of the network
(Levine & Kurzban, 2006). Under such circumstances, agents are expected
al
to forgive up to 80% of other network members’ defections (Hruschka &
ic
Henrich, 2006).
og
Other types of relationships generate still other types of benefits that
ol
redound to lifetime reproductive fitness. The benefits that might accrue from
ch
forgiving a mate are different from the benefits that might accrue by forgiving
y
a friend, which in turn are different from the benefits that a forgiver might
Ps
likely set up to identify the types of benefits that a particular type of relation-
ic
ship is likely to confer (and to weight them appropriately with respect to the
er
probability of capturing those benefits, the time horizon at which they will be
Am
realized, etc.) and then weigh those benefits against the deterrent value of
revenge, which the organism would trade off if it chose to forgive instead of
e
PROXIMATE CAUSATION
20
ciated with sacrificing the deterrence benefits of revenge for the relationship-
ht
sensitive to variables that influence the value of each option. These variables
yr
include, but are not necessarily limited to, characteristics of the offender, the
op
transgression itself, and cues that predict the probabilities of future attacks
and/or the potential future value of the restored relationship. In other words,
C
compare the cost of forgone revenge and the benefits that are expected to
accrue from a restored relationship.
Value of Deterrence
A)
The value of revenge diminishes to the extent that it does not change
P
(A
behavior that would otherwise occur. In the limiting case, suppose that after
an offense, the transgressor could persuasively signal that he or she would
n
never—or could never—again inflict costs. In such a case, revenge would
io
yield no benefit (except through third-party deterrence).
at
Information relevant to inferring intent can come from various sources.
ci
For instance, a transgressor’s apology, expression of sympathy for a victim’s
so
suffering, and declaration of his or her intention to behave better in the future
As
could indicate a low likelihood of trying to harm the victim in the future
(McCullough et al., 1997). Verbal declarations such as these are susceptible
al
to strategic manipulation, of course. Nonverbal displays such as blushing,
ic
which facilitate forgiveness after some transgressions (de Jong, Peters, &
og
de Cremer, 2003), also contain information about changed intent and a
ol
transgressor’s eagerness to distance himself or herself from a transgression, and
ch
their reliability may come from their unfakeability (Frank, 1988).
y
are impossible. When the aggressor’s capacity for violence is removed, for
instance, vengeance yields little additional deterrent value. In some ethno-
e
People more readily forgive people whom they trust (Hewstone, Cairns, Voci,
Hamberger, & Niens, 2006) and people who are reputed to be trustworthy
©
(Vasalou, Hopfensitz, & Pitt, 2008) despite their recent bad behavior.
ht
ig
Against the costs of forgone revenge is the expected value of future ben-
efits in a relationship in which intentions are positive rather than negative.
C
much the same way that it would be in contexts other than the aftermath of
a transgression. Because of the well-known principles of kin selection, close
relatives are likely to be a source of benefits, and thus, we expect that cues of
kinship will facilitate forgiveness, just as they evidently facilitate the restraint
A)
of vengeance (Lieberman & Linke, 2007).
In similar fashion, those with whom one has a close history of association,
P
(A
shared interests, and many opportunities for mutually beneficial transactions
are good candidates for forgiveness because of the possibility of continued gains.
n
Indeed, priming people with the names of close others (e.g., via subliminal
io
presentation) leads to increased judgments of forgiveness, increased accessi-
at
bility of the concept of forgiveness, and reduced deliberation about whether
ci
forgiveness is an appropriate course of action (Karremans & Aarts, 2007).
so
Karremans and Aarts’s (2007) results complement those from several previous
As
studies showing that people are more inclined to forgive individuals with whom
they feel close and committed (Finkel, Rusbult, Kumashiro, & Hannon, 2002;
al
McCullough et al., 1998; see also Chapter 2, this volume). We would argue
ic
that the reason for these associations of closeness and/or commitment with
og
forgiveness is that relationship closeness and commitment act as cues of
ol
relationship value in many types of relationships. We think the importance
ch
of relationship value can also explain why people tend to want some form of
y
the future.
ic
of the forgiveness system or merely an incidental effect that empathy can exert
within the existing forgiveness system, however, is currently difficult to know.
11
20
SUMMARY
©
ht
The desire for revenge and the ability to forgive seem to be universal
ig
have taken an adaptationist stance and posited that revenge and forgiveness
A)
forgiveness are really all about. By outlining the selection pressures that likely
gave rise to humans’ penchants for revenge and forgiveness, we have also tried
P
(A
here to identify the types of information that the structures that produce
revenge and forgiveness should be designed to process. We hope that intro-
n
ducing this sort of thinking can help investigators prioritize their research
io
efforts in the future.
at
ci
so
REFERENCES
As
Anderson, C., & Putterman, L. (2006). Do non-strategic sanctions obey the law of
al
demand? The demand for punishment in the voluntary contribution mechanism.
ic
Games and Economic Behavior, 54, 1–24. doi:10.1016/j.geb.2004.08.007
og
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W., & Matthews, D. (2002). Adaptationism—How to
ol
carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 489–504.
ch
doi:10.1017/S0140525X02000092
y
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Ps
the other player on choices in a prisoner’s dilemma game. Part II. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 139–147. doi:10.1037/h0044242
ic
Boehm, C. (1987). Blood revenge: The enactment and management of conflict in Montenegro
er
and other tribal societies (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Am
Boehm, C. (2008). Purposive social selection and the evolution of human altruism.
Cross-Cultural Research, 42, 319–352. doi:10.1177/1069397108320422
e
th
Bottom, W. P., Gibson, K., Daniels, S. E., & Murnighan, J. K. (2002). When talk is not
cheap: Substantive penance and expressions of intent in rebuilding cooperation.
by
(68)90053-X
©
Carpenter, J. P., & Matthews, P. H. (in press). Norm enforcement: Anger, indignation,
or reciprocity? Journal of the European Economic Association.
ht
Clark, K., & Sefton, M. (2001). The sequential prisoner’s dilemma: Evidence on
ig
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
de Jong, P. J., Peters, M. L., & de Cremer, D. (2003). Blushing may signify guilt:
Revealing effects of blushing in ambiguous social situations. Motivation and
Emotion, 27, 225–249. doi:10.1023/A:1025059631708
de Waal, F. B. M. (2000, July). Primates: A natural heritage of conflict resolution.
Science, 289, 586–590. doi:10.1126/science.289.5479.586
A)
Diamond, S. R. (1977). The effect of fear on the aggressive responses of anger aroused
P
and revenge motivated subjects. Journal of Psychology, 95, 185–188.
(A
DiBlasio, F., & Benda, B. B. (1991). Practitioners, religion and the use of forgiveness
n
in the clinical setting. Journal of Psychology and Christianity, 10, 166–172.
io
Dreber, A., Rand, D. G., Fudenberg, D., & Nowak, M. A. (2008, March 20). Winners
at
don’t punish. Nature, 452, 348–351. doi:10.1038/nature06723
ci
Elster, J. (1990). Norms of revenge. Ethics, 100, 862–885. doi:10.1086/293238
so
Enright, R. D., & Coyle, C. T. (1998). Researching the process model of forgiveness
As
within psychological interventions. In E. L. Worthington (Ed.), Dimensions
of forgiveness: Psychological research and theological perspectives (pp. 139–161).
al
Philadelphia, PA: Templeton Foundation Press.
ic
og
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415,
137–140. doi:10.1038/415137a
ol
Fehr, E., & List, J. A. (2004). The hidden costs and returns of incentives: Trust and
ch
trustworthiness among CEOs. Journal of the European Economic Association, 2,
y
743–771. doi:10.2139/ssrn.364480
Ps
Finkel, E. J., Rusbult, C. E., Kumashiro, M., & Hannon, P. A. (2002). Dealing with a
ic
Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple
bargaining experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6, 347–369. doi:10.1006/
e
game.1994.1021
th
Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York,
by
NY: Norton.
Frean, M. R. (1994). The prisoner’s dilemma without synchrony. Proceedings of the
11
Gordon, K. C., Burton, S., & Porter, L. (2004). Predicting the intentions of women
in domestic violence shelters to return to partners: Does forgiveness play a role?
©
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Parts I & II.
yr
Hauert, C., & Schuster, H. G. (1998). Extending the iterated prisoner’s dilemma
without synchrony. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 192, 155–166. doi:10.1006/
C
jtbi.1997.0590
Hayashi, N., Ostrom, E., Walker, J., & Yamagishi, T. (1999). Reciprocity, trust, and
the sense of control: A cross-societal study. Rationality and Society, 11, 27–46.
doi:10.1177/104346399011001002
Hebl, J., & Enright, R. D. (1993). Forgiveness as a psychotherapeutic goal with elderly
females. Psychotherapy, 30, 658–667. doi:10.1037/0033-3204.30.4.658
A)
Hewstone, M., Cairns, E., Voci, A., Hamberger, J., & Niens, U. (2006). Intergroup
P
contact, forgiveness, and experience of “The Troubles” in Northern Ireland.
(A
Journal of Social Issues, 62, 99–120. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2006.00441.x
n
Home Office. Research, Development and Statistics Directorate. Offending Surveys
io
and Research, National Centre for Social Research and BMRB. (2003). Social
at
Research, Offending, Crime and Justice Survey, 2003 [computer file] (Report
ci
No. SN: 5248, 3rd ed.). Retrieved from http://www.data-archive.ac.uk/doc/5248%
so
5Cmrdoc%5CUKDA%5CUKDA_Study_5248_Information.htm
As
Houser, D., Xiao, E., McCabe, K., & Smith, V. (2008). When punishment fails:
Research on sanctions, intentions and noncooperation. Games and Economic
al
Behavior, 62, 509–532.
ic
Hruschka, D. J., & Henrich, J. (2006). Friendship, cliquishness, and the emergence
og
of cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 239, 1–15. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.
2005.07.006 ol
ch
Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees are vengeful but not spite-
ful. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of Amer-
y
Ps
Kim, S. H., Smith, R. H., & Brigham, N. L. (1998). Effects of power imbalance and
the presence of third parties on reactions to harm: Upward and downward
Am
Koski, S. E., Koops, K., & Sterck, E. H. M. (2007). Reconciliation, relationship quality,
th
50.2.157
Kurzban, R. (2003). Trust, reciprocity, and gains from association: Interdisciplinary
©
Levine, S. S., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Explaining clustering in social networks: Towards
yr
Lundahl, B. W., Taylor, M. J., Stevenson, R., & Roberts, K. D. (2008). Process-based
forgiveness interventions: A meta-analytic review. Research on Social Work
Practice, 18, 465–478. doi:10.1177/1049731507313979
Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. (2003). Animal signals. Oxford, England: Oxford.
A)
McCullough, M. E. (2008). Beyond revenge: The evolution of the forgiveness instinct.
P
San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
(A
McCullough, M. E., Luna, L. R., Berry, J. W., Tabak, B. A., & Bono, G. (in press).
On the form of forgiving: Modeling the time-forgiveness relationship and testing
n
the valuable relationships hypothesis. Emotion.
io
at
McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington, E. L., Brown, S. W., &
ci
Hight, T. L. (1998). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships. II: Theoretical
so
elaboration and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
1586–1603. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1586
As
McCullough, M. E., Worthington, E. L., & Rachal, K. C. (1997). Interpersonal
al
forgiving in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,
ic
321–336. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.321
og
Miller, N., Pedersen, W. C., Earleywine, M., & Pollock, V. E. (2003). A theoretical
model of triggered displaced aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
7, 75–97. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0701_5
ol
ch
Molm, L. D. (1997). Coercive power in social exchange. New York, NY: Cambridge
y
Ps
University Press.
Murphy, J. G. (2003). Getting even: Forgiveness and its limits. New York, NY: Oxford.
an
Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases.
ic
Rapoport, A., & Chammah, A. M. (1965). The prisoner’s dilemma. Ann Arbor:
Am
600698550
by
Uniacke, S. (2000). Why is revenge wrong? The Journal of Value Inquiry, 34, 61–69.
20
doi:10.1023/A:1004778229751
Vasalou, A., Hopfensitz, A., & Pitt, J. V. (2008). In praise of forgiveness: Ways for
©
Vossekuil, B., Fein, R. A., Reddy, M., Borum, R., & Modzeleski, W. (2002). The final
yr
report and findings of the Safe School Initiative: Implications for the prevention of
op
school attacks in the United States. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education,
Office of Elementary and Secondary Education, Safe and Drug-Free Schools
C
A)
evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Wilson, W. (1971). Reciprocation and other techniques for inducing cooperation.
P
(A
The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 15, 167–195. doi:10.1177/002200277101500205
Worthington, E. L. (2005). More questions about forgiveness: Research agenda for
n
2005–2015. In E. L. Worthington (Ed.), Handbook of forgiveness (pp. 557–573).
io
New York, NY: Routledge.
at
Wu, J., & Axelrod, R. (1995). How to cope with noise in the iterated prisoner’s dilemma.
ci
The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 39, 183–189. doi:10.1177/0022002795039001008
so
Yamagishi, T. (1986). The provision of a sanctioning system as a public good. Journal
As
of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 110–116. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.1.110
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:55 AM
Am
er
ic
Page 240
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-14_CH13-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:55 AM Page 241
13
PA)
ATTACHMENT, ANGER,
(A
AND AGGRESSION
n
io
at
ci
MARIO MIKULINCER AND PHILLIP R. SHAVER
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
first to the study of adolescent and adult romantic relationships (e.g., Hazan
Am
& Shaver, 1987) and then more broadly to the study of emotion regulation,
social motives, and diverse forms of social behavior (Mikulincer & Shaver,
e
2003; see also Chapter 4, this volume). In this chapter, we explore the theory’s
th
affect (a) functional and dysfunctional forms of anger, (b) domestic violence,
yr
(c) antisocial criminal behavior, and (d) intergroup hostility and aggression.
op
241
12051-14_CH13-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:55 AM Page 242
attachment orientations and the experience and exercise of power are related.
We also present some new exploratory research concerning the influence of
attachment-related processes on cognition and action when a person’s sense
of power is experimentally enhanced.
A)
P
(A
ATTACHMENT THEORY AND ATTACHMENT STYLE
n
According to Bowlby (1982), human beings are born with an innate
io
psychobiological system (the attachment behavioral system) that motivates
at
them to seek proximity to supportive others (attachment figures) in times of
ci
need. This system accomplishes basic regulatory functions (protection from
so
threats and alleviation of distress) in humans of all ages, but it is most directly
As
observable during infancy and early childhood (Bowlby, 1988). Bowlby (1973)
described important individual differences in the functioning of the attachment
al
system. Interactions with attachment figures who are available and responsive
ic
in times of need facilitate optimal attachment-system functioning and promote
og
a sense of attachment security, a sense that the world is safe, that attachment
ol
figures are helpful when called on, and that it is possible to explore the
ch
environment curiously and engage effectively and enjoyably with other
y
self and others, which Bowlby called internal working models. When attachment
figures are not reliably available and supportive, however, a sense of security
an
is not attained, negative internal working models are formed, and strategies of
ic
have generally focused on a person’s attachment style, that is, the pattern of
th
infancy (i.e., secure, anxious, and avoidant) and on Hazan and Shaver’s (1987)
conceptualization of similar adult styles in the romantic relationship domain.
©
Subsequent studies (e.g., Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998) revealed, however,
ht
independence and emotional distance from partners. People who score low
on both dimensions are said to be secure or to have a secure attachment style.
The two dimensions can be measured with reliable and valid self-report scales
and are associated in theoretically predictable ways with various aspects of
A)
personal adjustment and relationship quality (for a review, see Mikulincer &
Shaver, 2007a).
P
(A
Attachment styles are initially formed in interactions with primary care-
givers during early childhood, as a large body of research has shown (Cassidy
n
& Shaver, 2008), but Bowlby (1988) claimed that memorable interactions
io
with others throughout life can alter a person’s working models and move the
at
person from one region of the two-dimensional space to another. Moreover,
ci
although attachment style is often conceptualized as a single global orienta-
so
tion toward close relationships, it is actually rooted in a complex network of
As
cognitive and affective processes and mental representations, which includes
many episodic, context-related, and relationship-specific as well as general
al
attachment representations (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). In fact, many studies
ic
indicate that a person’s attachment style can change depending on context
og
and recent experiences (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007b). This makes it possible
ol
to study the effects of security and insecurity experimentally.
y ch
Ps
Mikulincer and Shaver (2003) proposed that when the attachment system
er
taining coping flexibility and emotional stability in times of stress, broadens the
person’s perspectives and capacities, and facilitates the incorporation of mental
©
A)
strategies (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988) because they involve up-regulation of the
attachment system, including constant vigilance and intense concern until an
P
(A
attachment figure is perceived to be available and supportive. Hyperactivating
strategies include attempts to elicit a partner’s involvement, care, and support
n
through clinging and controlling responses (Shaver & Mikulincer, 2002);
io
overdependence on relationship partners as a source of protection (Shaver &
at
Hazan, 1993); and perception of oneself as relatively helpless with respect to
ci
emotion regulation (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2003). These strategies help to
so
explain the psychological correlates and consequences of attachment anxiety.
As
If a person has learned instead that relying on attachment figures is not
a safe or effective way to cope with threats, he or she is likely to downplay
al
such threats, inhibit worries and negative emotions, and defensively engage
ic
in what Bowlby (1982) called compulsive self-reliance. These coping strategies
og
are called deactivating (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988) because their goal is to keep
ol
the attachment system down-regulated rather than experience the frustration
ch
and pain of rejection, punishment for expressing feelings, or abandonment.
y
of avoidant attachment.
In short, each attachment strategy has a major regulatory goal (i.e.,
an
along with particular cognitive and affective processes that facilitate goal
er
more attention in the future and thereby provide better, more reliable
care. In general, especially for adults, anger is functional to the extent that
it communicates an intense but justifiable reaction to inconsiderate or
undeserved ill treatment, rather than simply being a way to injure or destroy
A)
a relationship partner through acts of revenge (see Chapters 3 and 12, this
volume). Bowlby (1973) called this constructive form of anger the “anger
P
(A
of hope” because it is intended to bring about a better future state of a
relationship. He also mentioned, however, that anger can become so intense
n
that it alienates or injures a partner, in which case it becomes destructive to
io
a relationship and can even lead to violence or death. He called this the
at
“anger of despair.”
ci
Functional anger is typical of people who feel secure in attachment
so
relationships. Mikulincer (1998) found, for example, that when secure adults
As
were hurt or frustrated by relationship partners’ behavior, they were optimistic
about the partners’ willingness to apologize and “reform.” Moreover, secure
al
people’s memories of their reactions to anger-provoking events were charac-
ic
terized by the constructive goal of repairing the relationship, engaging in
og
adaptive problem-solving, and restoring a positive mood following a conflict.
ol
Another study explored the functional nature of secure adolescents’ anger
ch
(Zimmermann, Maier, Winter, & Grossmann, 2001). Adolescent research
y
help of a friend, and their disappointment and anger during the task and their
negative behavior toward the friend (e.g., rejecting the friend’s suggestions
an
(Cassidy & Kobak, 1988). Avoidant people’s anger tends to be expressed only
th
in indirect ways and to take the form of nonspecific hostility or generally hate-
by
ful attitudes. Mikulincer (1998) found, for example, that avoidant adults did
not report intense anger in response to provocative experiences, but they
11
toward their partner (coded from video recordings) when they worried about
ig
A)
fear, sadness, and depression. Indeed, Mikulincer (1998) found that anxious
people’s memories of prior anger-provoking events included a flood of angry
P
(A
feelings, intense rumination, and a variety of negative emotions.
In a study of couple interactions, Simpson, Rholes, and Phillips (1996)
n
found that attachment anxiety was associated with displaying and reporting
io
more anger, hostility, and distress while discussing with a dating partner
at
an unresolved problem in their relationship. In a study of support seeking,
ci
Rholes et al. (1999) found that, although anxious attachment was unrelated
so
to anger toward a dating partner while waiting to undergo an anxiety-arousing
As
experience, after participants were told they would not have to undergo the
stressful task, attachment anxiety was associated with more intense expressions
al
of anger toward one’s partner. This pattern was particularly strong if participants
ic
had been more worried about the upcoming experience and had sought more
og
support from their partner. Thus, it seems that anxious individuals’ need for
ol
reassurance and support caused them to hold back feelings of frustration and
ch
anger while seeking a partner’s support. But once the support was no longer
y
needed (in that particular laboratory setting), the angry feelings surfaced and
Ps
were expressed.
In a study using psychophysiological measures, Diamond and Hicks
an
(2005) presented men with anger-provoking tasks (e.g., performing serial sub-
ic
and after the tasks, and recorded the men’s vagal tone, a physiological indica-
Am
tone during and after the tasks, suggesting intense anger that was difficult to
th
subdue.
by
11
called the anger of hope. These insecurities make it more likely that people
ig
Domestic Violence
A)
From an attachment perspective, domestic violence is an exaggerated
P
(A
form of protesting a partner’s hurtful behavior (see Chapters 2, 14, and 20).
It is meant to discourage or prevent a partner from violating or breaking off
n
the relationship (e.g., Bartholomew & Allison, 2006). We expect this kind of
io
behavior to be more common among insecurely attached individuals, especially
at
the anxious ones, who are especially vulnerable to hurt feelings and threats
ci
of abandonment (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a). Avoidant individuals, in
so
contrast, might be expected to withdraw from conflict rather than escalate it,
As
because they try to dismiss hurt feelings and avoid expressing vulnerability or
need. Nevertheless, Bartholomew and Allison (2006) found that avoidant
al
people sometimes became violent in the midst of escalating domestic con-
ic
flicts, especially if their partner was anxiously attached and demanded their
og
involvement.
ol
Anxious attachment has been associated with domestic violence in two
ch
kinds of studies (reviewed by Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a). First, anxious
y
Second, abusive men who score relatively high on attachment anxiety report
ic
more severe and more frequent acts of coercion and abuse during couple
er
conflicts.
Am
anxiety and some finding elevations in avoidance as well (Mikulincer & Shaver,
th
findings may indicate either that attachment insecurity puts people at risk of
being abused or that abuse increases attachment insecurity, or both. Also,
11
Antisocial Behavior
yr
op
avoidant individuals are more likely than their secure counterparts to engage
in antisocial behavior, they do so for different reasons. Anxiously attached
people sometimes engage in delinquent or criminal behavior as a way of
crying out for attention and care or of expressing anger and resentment
A)
(Allen, Moore, Kuperminc, & Bell, 1998). Avoidant individuals engage in
antisocial behavior to distance themselves from others (e.g., parents) or to
P
(A
demonstrate, by violating rules and laws, their lack of concern for others
(Allen et al., 1998).
n
Levinson and Fonagy (2004) compared the attachment patterns of
io
22 imprisoned delinquents, 22 patients with personality disorder without a
at
criminal history, and 22 healthy controls. They noted a higher prevalence
ci
of avoidant attachment in the delinquent group than in the other groups.
so
Moreover, delinquents who had committed violent offenses (e.g., murder,
As
malicious wounding) exhibited the same inability or unwillingness to talk
coherently about their emotional experiences that has been noted in other
al
studies of insecure attachment. In a related study, van IJzendoorn et al. (1997)
ic
interviewed 40 male criminals and found that 95% of them had insecure
og
attachment patterns.
ol
If antisocial behavior is associated with attachment insecurity, then
ch
interventions aimed at strengthening a person’s sense of attachment security
y
can reduce antisocial behavior (e.g., Born, Chevalier, & Humblet, 1997). In a
ic
found that those who formed secure attachment bonds with staff members had
Am
lower rates of anger, depression, and behavioral problems and more positive
emotional experiences during the year. Adolescents who formed more secure
e
Intergroup Aggression
20
were associated with more hostile responses to a variety of out-groups (as defined
yr
A)
sense of threat created by encounters with outgroup members and seemed to
eliminate defensive and hostile responses toward outgroup members.
P
(A
Building on these studies, Mikulincer and Shaver (2007b) found that
increasing people’s sense of attachment security reduced actual aggression
n
between contending or warring social groups. Specifically, Israeli Jewish under-
io
graduates participated in a study together with another Israeli Jew or an Israeli
at
Arab (in each case, the same confederate of the experimenter) and were sub-
ci
liminally and repeatedly exposed (for 20 ms on each trial) to the name of their
so
own security-enhancing attachment figure, the name of a familiar person who
As
was not viewed as an attachment figure, or the name of an acquaintance. Fol-
lowing the priming procedure, participants were informed that they would
al
evaluate a food sample and that they had been randomly selected to give the
ic
confederate hot sauce to evaluate. They also learned indirectly that the con-
og
federate strongly disliked spicy foods. (This procedure has been used in other
ol
studies of interpersonal aggression, e.g., McGregor et al., 1998.) The dependent
ch
variable was the amount of hot sauce allocated to the confederate.
y
someone who was not an attachment figure, they delivered a larger amount
of hot sauce to the Arab confederate than to the Jewish confederate, a sign
an
(relatively low) amounts of hot sauce to both the Arab and the Jewish con-
Am
member (Israeli Jew). Thus, it seems that people who are either dispositionally
th
secure or induced to feel more secure in a particular setting are better able than
by
rities and destructive forms of anger and aggression: domestic violence, anti-
yr
dominance behavioral system (for similar analyses, see Chapters 3 and 12).
Following Sroufe and Waters’s (1977) analysis of “felt security” as the proximal
psychological goal of the attachment system, we propose that something we
call “felt power” is the goal of the power/dominance system. This “felt sense”
A)
is associated with feeling that one can control resources without undue
social interference. Only when this goal is blocked by social interference do
P
(A
aggressive behaviors become pervasive, extreme, destructive, or dysfunctional
(see Chapter 4, this volume). In this section, we focus on times when feelings of
n
power and dominance are elevated and aggression serves its adaptive function.
io
In particular, we explore whether and how attachment insecurities affect the
at
experience and exercise of power.
ci
According to the approach/inhibition theory of power (e.g., Galinsky,
so
Gruenfeld, & Magee, 2003; Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003), experi-
As
encing a sense of power facilitates what Carver and White (1994) called an
approach orientation and Higgins (1998) called a promotion orientation, that is,
al
a motivated state involving reward-related thoughts, heightened attention to
ic
rewards, and positive emotions related to rewards. The sense of power also seems
og
to counteract what Carver and White (1994) called an inhibition orientation
ol
and Higgins (1998) called a prevention orientation, a state aimed at avoiding
ch
threats, heightened attention to threats, activation of threat-related thoughts,
y
and negative emotions. Galinsky et al. (2003) reasoned that a sense of power
Ps
because powerless people expect to be subject to more social and material threats
er
and are aware of the constraints that these threats impose on one’s actions.
Am
(e.g., asking people to recall an episode in which they had power over others)
20
2008). At the same time, because powerful people feel they have control over
op
others and do not feel constrained by them, increasing people’s sense of power
may increase their psychological distance from others (Smith & Trope, 2006)
C
and their tendency to objectify others and perceive them as means for attaining
personal goals (Gruenfeld, Inesi, Magee, & Galinsky, 2008). Galinsky, Magee,
Inesi, and Gruenfeld (2006) found that increasing people’s sense of power
reduced their inclination to adopt others’ perspectives and to empathize
with others.
A)
We suspect that these consequences of elevated power are moderated
by people’s attachment orientations because they are likely to depend on how
P
(A
one relates to others and on the goals, wishes, and fears that underlie one’s
social behavior. For example, people who hold negative views of others and
n
dislike closeness and intimacy (i.e., people with an avoidant attachment style)
io
are likely to construe power as a way to gain distance from others and freedom
at
from their influence. In contrast, people who hold positive views of others and
ci
are able to balance dependence and autonomy (i.e., relatively secure people)
so
are probably able to use power and influence to improve their interpersonal
As
relations, respond to others’ needs, and resolve interpersonal conflicts without
deferring too much to a partner’s needs. Moreover, people who fear rejection
al
and abandonment (i.e., anxiously attached people) may feel uneasy when
ic
granted power and be reluctant to act freely, take risks, and step outside the
og
boundaries of conformity. In subsequent sections, we report findings from two
studies that examine these ideas. ol
y ch
optimistic and take risks) as long as the person scores low on attachment anxiety
Am
but not if he or she scores high. People with an anxious attachment style are
likely to be ambivalent about power and dominance. On the one hand, they
e
want to have control over relationship partners (Shaver & Hazan, 1993); on
th
the other hand, they may be reluctant to assert themselves (e.g., Bartholomew
by
& Horowitz, 1991), because this could provoke a partner’s resentment, create
conflict, and threaten relationship stability.
11
sense of power (using Galinsky et al.’s, 2003, technique) on optimism and risk,
taking into account individual differences in attachment anxiety and avoidance.
©
anxiety and avoidance (Brennan et al., 1998), which had alpha reliability
yr
coefficients of .90 and .83 in this sample. Weeks later they came to a labora-
op
to recall a particular incident in which they had power over one or more other
people and to write about what happened, how they felt, and what they did
during and after the episode (for instructions, see Galinsky et al., 2003). In
the control condition (n = 40), participants were asked to recall a particular
TV program they had watched the previous week and to write about it.
A)
All of the participants then completed Weinstein’s (1980) 15-item
optimism scale (with an alpha of .85) and Tversky and Kahneman’s (1981)
P
(A
measure of risk preference. They were told the following:
n
Imagine that you work for a large hi-tech company that is experiencing
io
serious economic troubles and needs to lay off 6000 employees. Plan A
at
will save 2,000 jobs, whereas plan B has a one third probability of saving
ci
all 6,000 jobs but a two thirds probability of saving no jobs.
so
Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they would favor one
As
option over the other using a scale ranging from 1 (very much prefer program A)
to 7 (very much prefer program B). Higher scores reflected greater preference
al
for the riskier program (program B).
ic
Hierarchical regression analyses examining the unique and interactive
og
effects on optimism and risk preference of being primed with memories of power
ol
(a dummy variable contrasting the power condition with the control condition),
ch
avoidant attachment, and attachment anxiety revealed significant main
y
the control condition, β = .29, p < .01. In addition, more anxiously attached par-
ic
ticipants were less optimistic and less likely to prefer the riskier plan, βs of −.33
er
and −.30, ps < .01. These main effects were qualified by significant interactions
Am
between power priming and anxious attachment (β = −.41, p < .01, for optimism;
β = −.29, p < .01, for risk preference). Simple slope tests revealed that power
e
priming led to greater optimism and risk preference than the control condi-
th
and not significant when attachment anxiety was relatively high, βs of −.14
11
distant and detached from them (see the review by Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a).
It seems likely that they will use power as an opportunity to act on their pref-
erences for autonomy and distance, their critical view of others, and their
perception of others as objects to be used instrumentally for personal need
A)
satisfaction. In contrast, more secure people, who hold positive, empathic
views of others and are guided by a desire to form mutually satisfying and har-
P
(A
monious relationships, should be less likely to see others as objects even when
they are granted a degree of power over them.
n
To test these ideas we conducted a second study (Study 2) with a new
io
group of 60 Israeli undergraduate students (41 women and 19 men) who
at
completed the ECR scale during a class period (with resulting alphas of .86
ci
for anxiety and .88 for avoidance). Weeks later they came to a laboratory
so
individually and were randomly assigned to a power priming or a control con-
As
dition as in Study 1. All of them were then asked to think about their relation-
ships with three other students in their classes. For each of these students,
al
participants completed Gruenfeld et al.’s (2008) 10-item objectification scale
ic
(e.g., “I tend to contact this person only when I need something from him/her,”
og
“I try to get him/her to do things that will help me succeed”). Item ratings
ol
were made on a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). For
ch
each participant and each target student, we computed an objectification
y
score by averaging the 10 item scores (with alphas ranging from .88 to .91).
Ps
Because the correlations between the three different student scores were high
(rs > .64), we computed a total objectification score by averaging them.
an
slope tests revealed that power priming led to greater objectification of others
th
mainly when participants scored relatively high on avoidance, β = .43, p < .01.
by
The effect was in the opposite direction but not significant when avoidance
was low, β = −.15. Thus, as hypothesized, when people were primed with a sense
11
CONCLUDING REMARKS
yr
op
A)
have summarized some of the literature on that topic here. But infants are not
prepared to occupy powerful or dominant roles in society, so Bowlby said
P
(A
virtually nothing about power and dominance in his books.
Nevertheless, attachment theory has been fruitfully expanded in several
n
directions, including the conceptualization of leadership and group function-
io
ing in terms of attachment dynamics (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a). And
at
Bowlby’s behavioral-system construct has proven useful in the study of sexu-
ci
ality, empathy, and altruism from an attachment perspective (Mikulincer &
so
Shaver, 2007a), partly because Bowlby explicitly referred to the existence of
As
sexual and caregiving behavioral systems. Because we now want to extend the
theory into the untouched domain of power and dominance, we have begun
al
to conceptualize the existence of a power/dominance system. If such a system
ic
exists, we expect its operations to be colored by attachment security and the
og
major forms of attachment insecurity. In this chapter, we have unveiled our
ol
first efforts to explore links between attachment and power. We think the
ch
results are interesting and well worth pursuing further.
y
Ps
REFERENCES
an
ic
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment:
er
Allen, J. P., Moore, C., Kuperminc, G. P., & Bell, K. (1998). Attachment and ado-
lescent psychosocial functioning. Child Development, 69, 1406–1419. doi:10.2307/
e
1132274
th
Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk-taking. European
by
Dynamics of romantic love (pp. 102–127). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults:
©
226–244. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226
ig
Born, M., Chevalier, V., & Humblet, I. (1997). Resilience, desistance, and delinquent
yr
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New
C
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depression. New York, NY:
Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Basic Books.
A)
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. London,
P
England: Routledge.
(A
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes
n
io
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York, NY:
at
Guilford Press.
ci
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation,
so
and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS
scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. doi:10.1037/
As
0022-3514.67.2.319
al
Cassidy, J., & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relationship with other defensive
ic
processes. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment
og
(pp. 300–323). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ol
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
ch
clinical applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Diamond, L. M., & Hicks, A. M. (2005). Attachment style, current relationship secu-
y
Ps
rity, and negative emotions: The mediating role of physiological regulation. Journal
of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 499–518. doi:10.1177/0265407505054520
an
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical devel-
ic
Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action.
by
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Gruenfeld, D. H., Whitson, J. A., & Liljenquist, K. A.
20
(2008). Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity,
conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
©
1450–1466. doi:10.1037/a0012633
ht
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power and
ig
9280.2006.01824.x
op
Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Power and the
objectification of social targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,
C
111–127. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.111
A)
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment
process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511–524. doi:10.1037/
P
0022-3514.52.3.511
(A
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational
n
principle. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
io
(Vol. 30, pp. 1–46). New York, NY: Academic Press.
at
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhi-
ci
bition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.265
so
Levinson, A., & Fonagy, P. (2004). Offending and attachment: The relationship
As
between interpersonal awareness and offending in a prison population with
psychiatric disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychoanalysis, 12, 225–251.
al
Magee, J. C., Galinsky, A. D., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2007). Power, propensity to
ic
negotiate, and moving first in competitive interactions. Personality and Social
og
Psychology Bulletin, 33, 200–212. doi:10.1177/0146167206294413
ol
McGregor, H., Lieberman, J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J., Simon, L., &
ch
Pyszczynski, T. (1998). Terror management and aggression: Evidence that
y
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2001). Attachment theory and intergroup bias:
Evidence that priming the secure base schema attenuates negative reactions to
e
0022-3514.81.1.97
by
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2003). The attachment behavioral system in adult-
hood: Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. In M. P. Zanna
11
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 53–152). New York,
NY: Academic Press.
20
18, 139–156.
op
Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., & Orina, M. (1999). Attachment and anger in an
anxiety-provoking situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
C
940–957. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.940
Shaver, P. R., & Hazan, C. (1993). Adult romantic attachment: Theory and evidence.
In D. Perlman & W. Jones (Eds.), Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 4,
pp. 29–70). London, England: Jessica Kingsley.
Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2002). Attachment-related psychodynamics. Attach-
A)
ment & Human Development, 4, 133–161. doi:10.1080/14616730210154171
Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships:
P
(A
An attachment perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
899–914. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.5.899
n
Smith, P. K., & Trope, Y. (2006). You focus on the forest when you’re in charge of
io
the trees: Power priming and abstract information processing. Journal of Person-
at
ality and Social Psychology, 90, 578–596. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.578
ci
Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child
so
Development, 48, 1184–1199.
As
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981, January). The framing of decisions and the psy-
chology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458. doi:10.1126/science.7455683
al
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Feldbrugge, J., Derks, F. C. H., de Ruiter, C., Verhagen M. F.,
ic
og
Philipse, M. W., . . . Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (1997). Attachment representations
of personality-disordered criminal offenders. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
67, 449–459. doi:10.1037/h0080246 ol
ch
Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of
y
Zimmermann, P., Maier, M. A., Winter, M., & Grossmann, K. E. (2001). Attachment
and adolescents’ emotion regulation during a joint problem-solving task with a
an
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:55 AM
Am
er
ic
Page 258
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-15_CH14-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:56 AM Page 259
14
A)
P
ATTACHMENT AND VIOLENCE:
(A
AN ANGER BORN OF FEAR
n
io
at
ci
DONALD G. DUTTON
so
As
al
ic
Angry coercive behavior, acting in the service of an affectional bond, is
og
not uncommon. It is seen when a mother, whose child has foolishly run
across the road, berates and punishes him with an anger born of fear.
ol — Bowlby, 1973, p. 287
y ch
Ps
an
arousal reaction that lowers impulse control and increases the likelihood of
by
259
12051-15_CH14-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:56 AM Page 260
protest is often directed at a romantic partner and can take the form of
verbal abuse, coercive control of a partner’s behavior, and even violence
(see Chapters 13 and 20, this volume). Such abusive behavior is most likely
to be precipitated by real or imagined threats of rejection, separation, or
A)
abandonment by the partner. Hence, as Bowlby (1988) stated, “violence . . .
can be understood as the distorted and exaggerated version of behavior that
P
(A
is potentially functional” (p. 12).
This perspective is consistent with a large body of literature indicat-
n
ing that abusive men tend to be insecure and overly dependent on their
io
partners and that jealousy and fears of separation are common triggers of
at
abusive episodes (Dutton, 2006). In adults, tests of the association between
ci
attachment insecurities and violence have indicated that attachment pat-
so
terns that include considerable anxiety (often labeled preoccupied and
As
fearful; e.g., Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991) incline a person to become
highly emotionally aroused in response to possible separation or rejection
al
(Bartholomew & Allison, 2006; Bartholomew, Henderson, & Dutton, 2001).
ic
og
It follows that individuals with these kinds of insecure attachment may
exhibit higher levels of violence toward their close relationship partners,
ol
either as a way of controlling the threat of separation from a partner or as
ch
an impulsive “acting out” of inner tensions resulting from anticipating and
y
dreading separation.
Ps
an
ic
Pynoos & Eth, 1985; Schore, 1994; D. J. Siegel, 2001b). Neural structures
such as the hypothalamus and the prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex, which
ht
occur, the mind of the child may not come to function as a well-integrated
op
Klein and Mahler’s (e.g., Klein & Riviere, 1937; Mahler, 1971; Mahler,
Pine, & Bergman, 1975) psychoanalytic emphasis on the phase of infant
development in which the child first physically separates from the mother and
initiates a separate sense of identity is supported by current research on
A)
neurological development (Dutton, 2006). Thus, rage is exhibited first in the
context of an intimate relationship, usually with mother (Klein & Riviere, 1937),
P
(A
in a phase of life prior to language development and before autobiographical
memory (Schore, 2003a). Such rage is more common in insecure relationships,
n
and the neural and psychological effects of repeatedly experiencing it in early
io
childhood, incoherently and unmediated by language, may make it more
at
likely to erupt later in life, where it seems especially irrational when it leads
ci
to violence against an intimate adult attachment figure.
so
As
al
THE NEUROBIOLOGY OF ATTACHMENT
ic
og
In an extremely detailed analysis, Schore (1994, 2003a, 2003b) showed
how the capacity for attachment matures into a homeostatic self-regulatory
ol
system near the end of the first year of life. This occurs because dyadic com-
ch
munication with a parental figure generates intense positive affective states
y
in the infant and high levels of dopamine and endogenous opiates. These
Ps
dyadic interaction with caregivers during the first year of life (before extensive
er
the problems that abusive individuals have with attachment and emotion
regulation are often grounded in early relationships that are not recalled later
e
in words. This suggests that the most important and long-lasting effects of
th
early family dysfunction may be due not simply to imitation of specific abusive
by
acts but rather to the inability to regulate painful and destructive emotions,
such as panic and rage.
11
with unresolved losses and traumas who are themselves susceptible to dis-
sociative states. These parents generate in their young children “fright without
solution.”
Recent studies have shown that youths with a history of disorganized
A)
attachment are at great risk of expressing hostility with peers and have the
potential for extreme interpersonal violence (Lyons-Ruth, Bronfman, &
P
(A
Atwood, 1999; Lyons-Ruth & Jacobwitz, 2008). This implies that lack of
resolution of trauma in a parent, leading to what Lyons-Ruth and her colleagues
n
call hostile or helpless behavior, can cause parents to create frightening,
io
paradoxical, and unsolvable problematic situations for their children. The
at
adult attachment figure, who, according to Bowlby (1982) is equipped with a
ci
caregiving behavioral system that evolved because it provided a safe haven
so
and secure base for children who were then more likely to live and reproduce,
As
paradoxically becomes also a source of alarm, leaving a dependent child with
no clear strategy for getting attachment needs met.
al
ic
og
EMPIRICAL STUDIES OF ATTACHMENT STYLE, ANGER,
AND INTIMATE PARTNER VIOLENCE ol
y ch
sample and found that experiences and expressions of anger were related to
ic
associates and I had collected from abusive men using the subscales of the
Siegel Multidimensional Anger Inventory (MAI; J. M. Siegel, 1986): anger-in,
11
anger-out, and frequency, duration, and magnitude of anger and hostility. In our
20
higher than other adult attachment patterns with every subscale on the MAI.
ht
In my work (e.g., Dutton, 2006), I have described what I call “the abusive per-
ig
A)
associations between attachment style (measured with the Relationship
Styles Questionnaire [RSQ]; Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994) and emotional,
P
(A
cognitive, and behavioral reactions to intimacy. Specifically, the men completed
a battery of questionnaires including the RSQ and measures of borderline per-
n
sonality traits (Oldham et al., 1985), trauma symptoms (Briere & Runtz, 1989),
io
trait anger (J. M. Siegel, 1986), recalled parental treatment (Perris, Jacobsson,
at
Lindstrom, von Knorring, & Perris, 1980), verbal and physical abusiveness
ci
(Straus, 1992; Tolman, 1989), and jealousy (Mathes & Severa, 1981). The
so
men’s spouses also completed the latter two scales to describe their husbands.
As
We found that the more anxious attachment styles, fearful and preoccupied,
were significantly correlated with most features of intimate abusiveness and
al
with spouses’ reports of abusiveness. The secure attachment style, even in this
ic
self-selected, court-mandated population, was negatively correlated with
og
these features of abusiveness.
ol
A recent study of dating violence in a college sample examined associations
ch
between attachment insecurities and these same features of abusiveness in
y
Dutton et al. (1994) indicated that there is remarkable similarity across gen-
ders in the associations between forms of attachment insecurity and certain
an
features of abusiveness.
ic
& Kwong, 2005) also found attachment anxiety (in this case, preoccupied
Am
and social desirability. The authors used structural equation modeling to test
hypotheses that associations between anxious attachment and physical and
C
A)
physical and psychological abuse, and personality disorders fully mediated the
association between anxious attachment and psychological abuse but only par-
P
(A
tially mediated the link between anxious attachment and psychological abuse.
These studies suggest that attachment insecurity, especially anxious
n
attachment, may be a risk factor for violence, but for it to result in actual
io
violence it has to “crystallize” into something more: a chronically angry
at
temperament or a disturbance of the self that includes angry acting out.
ci
so
As
THE ABUSIVE PERSONALITY:
ATTACHMENT AND BORDERLINE PERSONALITY
al
ic
My early work on the “abusive personality” was completed before the
og
studies reviewed in the previous section were conducted, but it foreshadowed
ol
the importance of both attachment insecurities and personality dysfunctions
ch
(see also Westen & Shedler, 1999). At the time, I focused on borderline per-
y
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., American Psy-
ic
abandonment” (p. 710). However, at the time of our early research no one
Am
petrators in court mandated groups and gathering data from them and their
by
students, working-class control men, clinical outpatients, gay men from urban
yr
communities) have also turned up strong and significant correlations (in the
op
A)
finding replicated by Edwards, Scott, Yarvis, Paizis, and Panizzon (2003) using
an impulsive aggression questionnaire that assessed frequency of losing control,
P
(A
being suicidal, and displaying an extreme temper. Subsequent research with large
samples of men (e.g., Henderson, Bartholomew, & Dutton, 1997; Mauricio
n
et al., 2007) has largely confirmed the associations between attachment
io
insecurity (especially of anxious varieties) and borderline traits, impulsivity,
at
and abusiveness. The same pattern appears in abusive women as well (e.g., Clift
ci
2008; Henning, Jones, & Holford, 2003).
so
We consider severely insecure early attachment to be a likely key to
As
this personality constellation. The “modal family” for our court-mandated
samples was notable for its abusiveness and lack of a safe haven and secure base
al
for the children. Future research should explore the development of abusive-
ic
ness in more detail, and further integrative theorizing should be attempted to
og
bring into a single picture what we have learned and are still learning about early
ol
attachment experiences, neural development, impulsivity and poor emotion
ch
regulation, and clinically significant features of borderline personality and
y
other personality disorders. Some of the pieces of this important story can be
Ps
(Cassidy & Shaver, 2008), which includes many new findings regarding
ic
AND VIOLENCE
11
ing, which sometimes led to abuse. As a pursuit strategy, violence forced one
partner to focus on the other. As a distancing strategy, violence served to push
C
a partner away. Bartholomew et al. (2008) suggested that optimal social distance
rather than physical proximity and contact (the goals of the attachment system
highlighted by Ainsworth et al., 1978) might be considered the state in adult
relationships that lowers or terminates attachment-system activation.
Referring to the frequent reciprocity of negative affect in relationships,
A)
Bartholomew and Allison (2006) analyzed the dyadic nature of interpersonal
violence. Surveys in the United States indicate that bilateral violence is the
P
(A
most common form in intimate relationships (e.g., Stets & Straus, 1989;
Whitaker, Haileyesus, Swahn, & Saltzman, 2007), and in laboratory studies
n
of marital interactions, reciprocal negative affect is a consistent predictor of
io
relationship dysfunction and deterioration (e.g., Cordova, Jacobson, Gottman,
at
Rushe, & Cox, 1993; Leonard & Roberts, 1998; Margolin, John, & Gleberman,
ci
1988). Future research should therefore consider both partners’ attachment
so
insecurities and their connections with both personality disorder symptoms
As
and destructive reciprocal hostility and impulsive violence.
al
ic
OTHER MANIFESTATIONS OF ATTACHMENT THREAT
og
ol
Beyond the issues raised by relationship dynamics, several matters having
ch
to do with attachment insecurity and violence deserve attention. These include
y
data from the U.S. National Morbidity Study, Kposowa (2000) found that
th
to commit suicide. Men were four times more likely than women to commit
suicide under these conditions, and White males had the highest rate of all.
11
should consider why men are more prone than women to enact this form of
aggression following relationship dissolution.
©
ht
A)
of depersonalization, impaired sense of self-worth, impairments in memory
and perceptual functioning, a partial loss of abstract standards including moral
P
(A
and legal rules, and a general decline in intellectual functioning (e.g., loss of
concentration, decline in problem-solving ability).
n
According to Mawson (1987), combat stress produces chronic increases
io
in sympathetic nervous system arousal, which in turn produces “seeking
at
behavior” of the kind Bowlby (1973, 1982) attributed to the attachment
ci
behavioral system. In combat, because no security or familiar source of security
so
is to be found, further increases in sympathetic arousal are generated. Under
As
these conditions, a person’s cognitive map begins to disintegrate; more patterned,
abstract, differentiated mental processes situating the person in a complex of
al
familiar people and places and normative obligations dissolve. According to
ic
Baumeister (1990), the situated identity or individuated self also breaks down
og
and consciousness is altered so that it is completely focused on the present
ol
action, with autonomic arousal remaining very high. Instead of seeking the
ch
familiar (to reduce distress), the soldier is likely to engage in impulsive, enor-
y
This is one way to think about the infamous My Lai massacre, perpetrated
by a U.S. Army platoon during the Vietnam War. It followed weeks of losing
an
a search and destroy mission (Hersh, 1970). The testimony given at the war
er
Lai” although none of the villagers were armed or fired a weapon. This kind
th
launch the First Crusade. The same strategy was used with Japanese kamikaze
pilots in World War II, and it is used now to inspire Islamic suicide bombers
C
(Dutton, 2007). The belief that is central to this kind of activity may be the
most powerful motivating belief for human beings: that they will live with their
loved ones (including an all-powerful symbolic parental figure) in everlasting
bliss (i.e., they will enjoy attachment security in perpetuity). Cheung-Blunden
and Blunden (2008), for example, found that Christian students exposed to
A)
images of the World Trade Center towers in flames on 9/11 were more likely
than atheists to define the enemy in religious terms and to condone bombing
P
(A
and killing as a form of reprisal. Both Christianity and Islam have done this
repeatedly throughout their histories, despite both the Bible and the Koran
n
advocating peace and forgiveness.
io
A further motivating image resides in religious ideology: the notion of
at
Armageddon. Asbridge (2004) describes how the recapturing of Jerusalem
ci
was consistent with the Christian belief that the “last days” before the Second
so
Coming of Christ, foretold in the Bible, could come to pass only after Jerusalem
As
was again in Christian hands. Phillips’ 2006 volume, American Theocracy,
points to the same belief among a sizeable number of fundamentalist Ameri-
al
cans (called “end timers”) today. He sees this belief as supportive of the 2003
ic
invasion of Iraq, which end timers viewed as a way to reduce a threat to Israel
og
and Jerusalem.
ol
Although weaponry has improved immensely in the millennium since the
ch
crusades, the ideological belief structure and psychological need to belong to a
y
2005). Asbridge (2004) describes crusaders, still covered in blood and carrying
ic
booty, kneeling at the Holy Sepulchre to pray. This was not, he assures the
er
“disguise of panic,” for some strange reason attachment research and terror
ht
TMT suggests, and secure attachment is a relief from terror, then attachment
op
and TMT appear to be two sides of the same coin. Developmentally, attachment
has to precede death terror, because attachment, as we described earlier, is a
C
individuation around age 1.5 to 2 years (Schore, 1994, 2003a, 2003b), and
children in the preschool years have little understanding of the universality,
irreversibility, and inevitability of death (Nagy, 1948). However, studies show-
ing that some of a person’s defenses against death awareness are unrelated to
A)
death in any logical or semantic way (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999)
raise the question of whether unconscious awareness of death is prewired or
P
(A
precedes conscious development of notions of death.
Following this reasoning, Mikulincer and Florian (2000) performed five
n
studies examining the contribution of attachment style to the psychological
io
effects of experimentally enhanced mortality salience. Although defending
at
one’s worldview by punishing criminals or dissenters has been portrayed as the
ci
normative or natural defense against mortality salience (Greenberg et al., 1997),
so
Mikulincer and Florian (2000) showed that this reaction is more characteristic
As
of people who score high on measures of attachment insecurity than of those
who score low. Specifically, experimentally induced death reminders produced
al
more severe judgments and punishments of moral transgressors only among
ic
insecurely attached participants, whether they were primarily anxious or
og
avoidant. Secure participants reacted to mortality salience with an increased
ol
sense of symbolic immortality—a transformational, constructive strategy that
ch
leads a person to invest in his or her children’s care and to engage in creative
y
activities whose products will live on after one’s death—and a more intense
Ps
competence.
Am
to shape child killers. In Rwanda, for example, child soldiers were forced to
20
kill a resident of their own village to break attachment to the tribe and foster
attachment to the killer group (Dutton, 2007; Human Rights Watch, 1999).
©
ht
ig
CONCLUSIONS
yr
op
born of fear and the converse, that insecure attachment increases both. When
panic-driven anger is aroused, violence becomes much more likely. This process
is most salient when anger arises in intimate relationships, especially ones
perceived by the perpetrator to be the main or only potential source of security.
It manifests as rage reactions to separation or threats of separation. If we take
A)
one more theoretical step and focus on symbolic attachment in the form of a
tribe or family, then the evolutionary roots of Bowlby’s (1982) formulation of
P
(A
attachment become salient. Threats to the tribe or, by extension, to the tribe’s
ideology or future ability to thrive can generate extreme violence that may
n
have roots in attachment. Attachment and its ability to make us feel safe from
io
death is perhaps the strongest human motive. The lure of secure attachment in
at
perpetuity, unfortunately and tragically, can motivate people to kill themselves
ci
and strangers in a “holy war.”
so
As
REFERENCES
al
ic
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment:
og
Assessed in the Strange Situation and at home. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ol
Allison, C. J., Bartholomew, K., Mayseless, O., & Dutton, D. G. (2008). Love as
ch
a battlefield: Attachment and relationship dynamics in couples identified
y
for male partner violence. Journal of Family Issues, 29, 125–150. doi:10.1177/
Ps
0192513X07306980
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
an
Asbridge, T. (2004). The first crusade: A new history. Oxford, England: Oxford
er
University Press.
Am
Dynamics of romantic love: Attachment, caregiving, and sex (pp. 102–127). New York,
th
couple psychotherapy: The “secure base” concept in practice and research (pp. 43–61).
London, England: Routledge.
20
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. W. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults:
©
Bartholomew, K., Oram, D., & Landolt, M. A. (2008). Correlates of partner abuse in
male same sex relationships. Violence and Victims, 23, 348–364.
yr
op
Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York, NY: Free Press.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger. New
York, NY: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Basic Books.
A)
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. London,
P
England: Routledge.
(A
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes
n
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New York, NY:
io
Guilford Press.
at
ci
Briere, J., & Runtz, M. (1989). The Trauma Symptom Checklist (TSC-33): Early
so
data on a new scale. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 4, 151–163. doi:10.1177/
088626089004002002
As
Carlson, E. A. (1998). A prospective longitudinal study of attachment disorganization/
al
disorientation. Child Development, 69, 1107–1128.
ic
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research,
og
and clinical applications (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
ol
Cheung-Blunden, V., & Blunden, B. (2008). Paving the road to war with group
ch
membership, appraisal antecedents, and anger. Aggressive Behavior, 34, 175–189.
doi:10.1002/ab.20234
y
Ps
Cordova, J. V., Jacobson, N. S., Gottman, J. M., Rushe, R., & Cox, G. (1993). Negative
ic
Dutton, D. G. (2006). The abusive personality (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
e
Dutton, D. G., Starzomski, A., Saunders, K., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Intimacy-
anger and insecure attachment as precursors of abuse in intimate relationships.
11
Edwards, D. W., Scott, C. L., Yarvis, R. M., Paizis, C. L., & Panizzon, M. S. (2003).
©
Ferguson, N. (2006). The war of the world. New York, NY: Penguin.
yr
Follingstad, D. R., Bradley, R. G., Helff, C. M., & Laughlin, J. E. (2002). A model for
op
35.33639
A)
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszcynski, T. (1997). Terror management theory of
self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual
P
refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
(A
(Vol. 29, pp. 61–141). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
n
Griffin, D. W., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). The metaphysics of measurement: The
io
case of adult attachment. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances
at
in personal relationships: Attachment processes in adulthood (Vol. 5, pp. 17–52).
ci
London, England: Jessica Kinsley.
so
Gunderson, J. G. (1984). Borderline personality disorder. Washington, DC: American
As
Psychiatric Press.
Henderson, A. J. Z., Bartholomew, K., & Dutton, D. G. (1997). He loves me, he
al
loves me not: Attachment and separation resolution of abused women. Journal
ic
of Family Violence, 12, 169–192. doi:10.1023/A:1022836711637
og
Henderson, A. J. Z., Bartholomew, K., Trinke, S., & Kwong, M. J. (2005). When loving
ol
means hurting: An exploration of attachment and intimate abuse in a community
ch
sample. Journal of Family Violence, 20, 219–230. doi:10.1007/s10896-005-5985-y
y
Henning, K., Jones, A., & Holford, R. (2003). Treatment needs of women arrested
Ps
Hersh, S. (1970). My Lai: A report on the massacre and its aftermath. New York, NY:
ic
Vintage Books.
er
Human Rights Watch. (1999). Leave none to tell the tale: Genocide in Rwanda. New
Am
Klein, M., & Riviere, J. (1937). Love, hate, and reparation. New York, NY: Norton.
by
doi:10.1136/jech.54.4.254
20
Leonard, K. E., & Roberts, L. J. (1998). The effects of alcohol on the marital inter-
actions of aggressive and nonaggressive husbands and their wives. Journal of
©
Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Atwood, G. (1999). A relational diathesis model
ig
A)
application to borderline phenomena in the psychoanalytic situation. The
Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 26, 403–424.
P
(A
Mahler, M., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (1975). The psychological birth of the human infant.
New York, NY: Basic Books.
n
Main, M. (1995). Attachment: Overview with implications for clinical work. In
io
S. Goldberg, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental,
at
and clinical perspectives (pp. 407–474). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ci
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of a new, insecure disorganized/disoriented
so
attachment pattern: Procedures, findings and implications for the classification
As
of behavior. In T. B. Brazelton & M. W. Yogman (Eds.), Affective development
in infancy (pp. 95–124). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
al
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/
ic
disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation. In M. T. Greenberg,
og
D. Cicchetti, & M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory,
ol
research, and intervention (pp. 121–160). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
ch
Margolin, G., John, R. S., & Gleberman, L. (1988). Affective responses to conflictual
y
Mauricio, A. M., Tein, J. Y., & Lopez, F. G. (2007). Borderline and antisocial person-
er
NY: Praeger.
th
0022-3514.79.2.260
ht
Nagy, M. (1948). The child’s theories concerning death. The Journal of Genetic
ig
Oldham, J., Clarkin, J., Appelbaum, A., Carr, A., Kernberg, P., Lotterman, A., &
op
Perris, C., Jacobsson, L., Lindstrom, H., von Knorring, L., & Perris, H. (1980). Devel-
opment of a new inventory for assessing memories of parental rearing behaviour.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 61, 265–274. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1980.
tb00581.x
A)
Perry, B. (1997). Incubated in terror: Neurodevelopmental factors in the cycle of
violence. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Children, youth and violence: Searching for solutions
P
(pp. 124–149). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
(A
Phillips, K. (2006). American theocracy. New York, NY: Viking.
n
io
Pynoos, R. S., & Eth, S. (1985). Children traumatized by witnessing acts of personal
violence: Homicide, rape, or suicide behavior. In S. Eth & R. S. Pynoos (Eds.),
at
Posttraumatic stress disorder in children (pp. 17–44). Washington, DC: American
ci
Psychiatric Press.
so
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual-process model of defense
As
against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of
terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835–845. doi:10.1037/
al
0033-295X.106.4.835
ic
og
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of
emotional development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ol
Schore, A. N. (2003a). Affect dysregulation and the disorders of the self. New York, NY:
ch
Norton.
y
Ps
Schore, A. N. (2003b). Affect regulation and the repair of the self. New York, NY: Norton.
Siegel, D. J. (2001a). The developing mind: How relationships and the brain interact to
an
Stets, J., & Straus, M. A. (1989). The marriage license as a hitting license: A com-
parison of dating, cohabiting and married couples. Journal of Family Violence, 4,
11
161–180. doi:10.1007/BF01006627
20
Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Findler, L., & Mikulincer, M. (2002). The effects of mortality
ig
ment style. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 419–441. doi:10.1348/
op
014466602760344296
Tolman, R. M. (1989). The development of a measure of physiological maltreatment
C
A)
of Psychiatry, 156, 258–272.
Whitaker, D. J., Haileyesus, T., Swahn, M., & Saltzman, L. (2007). Differences in
P
(A
frequency of violence and reported injury between relationships with reciprocal
and non-reciprocal intimate partner violence. American Journal of Public Health,
n
97, 941–947. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2005.079020
io
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:56 AM
Am
er
ic
Page 276
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-16_CH15-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:57 AM Page 277
15
PA)
RESPECTING OTHERS AND BEING
(A
RESPECTED CAN REDUCE
n
io
AGGRESSION IN PARENT–CHILD
at
ci
RELATIONS AND IN SCHOOLS
so
As
OFRA MAYSELESS AND MIRI SCHARF
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
This chapter examines the role that respect plays in mitigating aggression
ic
respect in reducing aggression and violence has been discussed and examined
Am
(e.g., de Cremer & Tyler, 2005; Gottman, 1994; Janoff-Bulman & Werther,
by
2008). It is interesting that the term respect has rarely been used within
the literature of developmental psychology, which examines the contexts in
11
which children grow and develop. Hence we know little about the expression,
20
277
12051-16_CH15-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:57 AM Page 278
or fostering aggression: (a) respecting others, (b) being respected, (c) being
disrespected or humiliated, and (d) respecting oneself. We then examine the
role of respect in parent–child relationships and at school. Although respect
per se, under that particular name, has rarely been examined in these contexts,
A)
several core characteristics of respect have been considered and discussed,
and their association with aggression and the misuse of power has been inves-
P
(A
tigated. In a final section we advocate an increased emphasis on respect in
parent–child relationships, schools, and other contexts, because of its potential
n
for reducing aggression and violence.
io
at
ci
UNCONDITIONAL AND CONTINGENT FORMS OF RESPECT
so
As
The concept of respect is complex and multifaceted. It refers to several
distinct yet interconnected processes and seems to have somewhat different
al
implications in different contexts and relationships. Following previous
ic
writings (e.g., Frei & Shaver, 2002), we suggest that respect is an attitude rather
og
than an emotion and includes cognitive evaluations, feelings, and behaviors
ol
with possible disparities among these different components. People may, for
ch
example, behave respectfully toward others but internally despise them and view
y
them as immoral and unworthy. Conversely, a person may feel respect toward
Ps
someone but not show it behaviorally. This may be the case, for instance, when
adolescents internally respect their parents but at times behave in a defiant and
an
components of respect (Darwall, 1977; Frei & Shaver, 2002; Hendrick &
Am
types of respect that have been discussed by most researchers, although under
th
different names.
by
Unconditional Respect
11
20
basic rights to freedom, dignity, and autonomy (e.g., Rawls, 1971; see also
ig
the authors call “cultures of dignity”). Some researchers rely on the writings
op
(Lightfoot, 2000) rely on Piaget (1932), who considered the ability to recognize
A)
other points of view.
At a deeper level, such a stance often reflects a spiritual sense or belief
P
(A
that all humans have a common divine origin and share a divine quality. This
belief implies a moral imperative to value, appreciate, and respect others, just
n
as one respects the divine. This moral imperative is shared by many religions
io
and religious movements, old and new, Eastern and Western, monotheistic
at
or not (Smith, 1991).
ci
so
Contingent Respect
As
The second broad meaning of respect, which, following Janoff-Bulman
al
and Werther (2008), we label contingent respect, refers to an attitude toward
ic
an individual who possesses or embodies certain qualities or attributes or
og
who has attained a certain admirable status. This general category of respect
ol
includes several types. For example, respect for social power (Langdon, 2007)
ch
includes respect for people with authority, such as parents or teachers, whose
y
power is bestowed by their role or status regardless of their own specific qualities
Ps
(e.g., warmth, competence, moral behavior; see Chapter 10, this volume, for
a similar analysis of respect in what the author calls “face cultures”). A different
an
and integrity, for example, being hard working, trustworthy, sincere, altruis-
er
honor). Janoff-Bulman and Werther (2008) say that such respect is accorded
th
in their own right and acknowledges that they are entitled to autonomy,
privacy, and dignity and should not be injured, insulted, or humiliated.
C
Such a person is polite and allows others to have a voice; above all, because
A)
people are naturally inclined to nurture and protect an entity (whether a person,
a group, or “nature”) that is valued and not to hurt, damage, or destroy it.
P
(A
Second, there is a moral aspect to contingent respect. A respected individual
or a respected group is considered to be morally worthy and deserving of kind
n
treatment (McCullough, 2008; see also Chapters 10 and 12, this volume).
io
Hence, there is a moral prohibition against aggression toward a respected
at
individual or group. Furthermore, in line with previous conceptual analyses
ci
(Frei & Shaver, 2002; Langdon, 2007), we contend that respecting others
so
typically involves not just a general moral valuing of them but also a commit-
As
ment to nurture, cherish, and support them. Thus, respect calls for a prosocial,
caring orientation rather than an antagonistic or antisocial orientation.
al
Finally, respecting others, particularly close relationship partners, encourages
ic
reciprocation of respect (Kumashiro, Finkel, & Rusbult, 2002). In relation-
og
ships where one feels respected, there are fewer reasons to be frustrated, angry,
or aggressive. ol
y ch
Not just respecting others but being respected by others is likely to reduce
an
feels valued, protected, and secure. In this situation there is little reason to
er
behave aggressively (see Chapters 13 and 14). Further, as discussed more fully
Am
in the next section, being contingently respected fulfills people’s basic needs,
in close relationships and groups, for honor, dignity, and voice. Feeling respected
e
also Chapter 10, this volume). Second, being and feeling respected by others
often implies an expectation on the part of others that one will act morally.
11
Such expectations tend to be fulfilled (e.g., Jussim, 1986), and they can atten-
20
provides a person with greater social influence, so there is less need to resort
ht
On the flip side of respecting others and receiving their respect is the
C
out for respect is not necessarily negative; it may not lead to any particular
feeling. But being disrespected is an extremely negative experience; it often
amounts to being devalued, demeaned, or insulted, and it may include having
one’s rights to autonomy and dignity violated. Researchers have addressed
A)
this kind of disrespect in the context of expectations regarding fair and just
treatment and reactions to perceived injustice (e.g., Heuer, Blumenthal,
P
(A
Douglas, & Weinblatt, 1999; Miller, 2001). According to Miller (2001), peo-
ple generally believe they are entitled to fairness in the allocation of resources
n
(distributive justice) and fairness in procedures used to determine the allocation
io
of these resources (procedural justice). When such fairness—especially with
at
regard to procedural justice—is enacted, individuals tend to feel respected
ci
even if their requests have not been granted. When individuals feel that their
so
right to polite, fair, and respectful treatment has been violated, or that their
As
honor has been violated, they are likely to feel hurt, frustrated, and disrespected,
and therefore to react aggressively to restore self-esteem, save face, or educate
al
the offender (see Chapters 1 and 2, this volume). In line with these contentions,
ic
disrespectful treatment, which denies people what they believe is rightfully
og
theirs, is likely to cause both anger and aggression (Bettencourt & Miller, 1996;
ol
see also Chapter 10, this volume). Moreover, being subjected to disrespectful
ch
treatment is considered to justify aggression. People are less critical of aggressive
y
acts when they are viewed as retaliation for disrespectful treatment (Harvey
Ps
a person’s status (Janoff-Bulman & Werther, 2008; see also Chapter 12, this
er
autonomy, freedom, and dignity (Roland & Foxx, 2003), is less discussed in
the social psychological literature than respect for other people. Some of the
©
urge to retaliate, are said to derive from the damage incurred to one’s self-respect
ig
(e.g., Roland & Foxx, 2003). Maintaining self-respect requires living and
yr
behaving in ways that accord with one’s moral standards and expectations.
op
Respecting oneself makes it more likely that one will not tolerate and accept
disrespectful treatment by others (Roland & Foxx, 2003, p. 250). The relevance
C
A)
with lower levels of aggression and violence (see Chapters 1 and 2, this volume).
Finally, individuals are likely to be less aggressive toward others who exhibit
P
(A
self-respect and dignity (Dillon, 2007).
How and where do people learn self-respect and respect of others? Two
n
developmental contexts in which socialization of these dispositions occurs are
io
(a) parent–child relationships and (b) schools. Both are contexts in which
at
socialization agents are imbued with a moral and legal duty to educate and
ci
raise children to become competent adults and good citizens. How then does
so
respect, with its different forms and facets, develop and function in these
As
contexts?
al
ic
RESPECT IN PARENT–CHILD RELATIONS
og
AND ITS RELATION TO AGGRESSION
ol
ch
Respect, particularly of children toward their parents, has often been
y
discussed. The fifth of the Bible’s Ten Commandments is to “Honor your father
Ps
and your mother, so that your days may be long in the land that the Lord your
God is giving you.” A similar moral and religious mandate is embedded in
an
another ancient tradition, Chinese Confucian thought, which makes filial piety,
ic
love and respect for one’s parents and ancestors, one of the most important
er
power relationships.
Children’s respect for their parents involves demonstrations of honor
e
and esteem toward parents and polite compliance with their instructions and
th
requests (Langdon, 2007; Lightfoot, 2000). A child who disobeys his or her
by
obeyed, esteemed, and honored by their children because of their parental role,
almost without consideration of the parents’ actual expertise, competence,
©
Hendrick, 2006).
op
A)
be seen in the fair and just procedures they implement and in their acknowl-
edgement of their children’s right to have a “voice.” This respect seems to be
P
(A
of the unconditional type, because it relates to the children’s value as human
beings and not to their specific moral character or accomplishments. Conse-
n
quently, although respect in these relationships is expected to be mutual and
io
is often described as such, the ways in which it is expected to be expressed
at
differ somewhat in their qualities.
ci
Both types of respect are related to aggression. When children respect,
so
obey, and honor their parents, and comply with their parents’ requests, they
As
evince capacities for self-control and delay of gratification as well as appreci-
ation of authority, rules, and regulations. Each of these qualities can reduce
al
anger and aggression that children display toward their parents and that parents
ic
display toward their child (see Chapters 1, 2, and 9). Furthermore, children’s
og
respect for their parents usually puts the parents in the position of authority
ol
figures whom their children wish to emulate, thereby minimizing disagreements.
ch
Such a clear and predictable context enhances the child’s sense of confidence
y
and security. Securely attached children, who have a sense of direction and
Ps
(see Chapters 13 & 14). Furthermore, parents’ modeling of respect for their
ic
to respect themselves and the people with whom they interact (see Chapter 7
Am
and less often frustrated (see Chapter 7). Each of these qualities should reduce
by
associated with lower levels of antisocial behavior. Regard for parents further
20
sample of Israeli youth (Scharf & Mayseless, 2005), respect for parents
ig
(e.g., “I respect them”) predicted lower levels of aggression at school, over and
yr
valuation of parents (e.g., “They are wise and have had rich life experiences”).
A)
The effects of parental respect for children have been extensively studied
by researchers examining the topic of disrespect for children’s rights to autonomy
P
(A
and individuation. Parenting attitudes and practices that result in rejection
of the child or invalidation of the child’s autonomy—practices such as guilt
n
induction and aversive psychological control—have clear negative conse-
io
quences for the child’s affect regulation and externalizing behavior problems
at
(Barber, 2002; see also Chapter 7, this volume). Psychological control involves
ci
intrusive parenting practices such as instilling anxiety, inducing guilt, and
so
withdrawing love in order to deny or suppress a child’s thoughts, feelings, or
As
desired actions (Barber, 2002; Mayseless & Scharf, 2009). In many studies,
psychological control has been strongly associated with adverse outcomes,
al
including delinquency and antisocial behavior (Barber, 2002).
ic
Parents too, when they feel disrespected by their children, tend to be
og
aggressive and abusive. Though not explicitly using the term respect, devel-
ol
opmental researchers have examined the ramifications of parents’ sense of
ch
humiliation and lack of power because of disrespect and defiance on the part of
y
their children. In particular, Bugental and colleagues (Bugental & Lewis, 1999;
Ps
Martorell & Bugental, 2006) have proposed a model of the misuse of power
by people who perceive themselves as powerless yet are placed in a position
an
who feel powerless are more stressed by their children’s misbehavior and tend
er
more frequently to resort to coercive and abusive practices than parents who
Am
when children are more challenging (Bugental & Happaney, 2004). We suspect,
th
given these researchers’ descriptions of what they observed, that parents’ sense
by
(Ikels, 2004). In Western cultures, too, there are variations with regard to
ig
the importance of respect for parents. For example, Dixon, Graber, and
yr
African American and Latina girls compared with European American girls.
Furthermore, low levels of respect were associated with more intense arguments
C
in ethnic groups that placed higher value on respect (e.g., African American
A)
esting example of cultural differences. The study included a videotaped inter-
action in which parents and their adolescent child were asked to discuss a
P
(A
conflictual issue. Based on the 10-minute interaction with each parent, several
aspects of respect were coded. Autonomy-inhibiting behaviors included over-
n
personalizing the disagreement and pressuring the other person to agree, which
io
were ways of expressing disrespect for the other’s space, boundaries, and views;
at
whereas relatedness-inhibiting behaviors included expressing hostility and rudely
ci
interrupting the other, showing disrespect for the other’s right to be heard.
so
Israeli girls were more autonomy-inhibiting toward both parents than U.S. girls.
As
In addition, Israeli girls were more relatedness-inhibiting toward their mothers
compared with the U.S. girls. These observations accord well with claims for
al
low levels of respect toward authority in general, and for parents in particular,
ic
in Israeli society (Scharf & Mayseless, 2005; Golden & Mayseless, 2008).
og
ol
ch
RESPECT IN SCHOOLS AND ITS RELATION TO AGGRESSION
y
Ps
the importance of respect in the school context, suggesting that respecting each
Am
student and his or her potential for growth and development should be a core
aspect of education and a central component of school climate (Noddings,
e
students with special needs, and students from high-risk backgrounds or envi-
by
moral perspective that advocates caring for students as valued human beings
20
and ensuring that they flourish and actualize their potential (Noddings, 2005).
Respecting individual differences in this context often means that authority
©
figures are expected to create a school climate in which each student feels
ht
valued and competent and is helped to realize his or her potential. Such a
ig
ation and benevolence (e.g., Mulcahy & Casella, 2005). Although past research
op
(e.g., Benbenishty & Astor, 2005; Kasen, Berenson, Cohen, & Johnson, 2004)
In one of the few studies that directly focused on the expression of respect,
LaRusso, Romer, and Selman (2008) found, in a representative sample of U.S.
high schools, that greater perceived teacher support and regard for students’
A)
perspectives was associated with students’ perception of their schools as
having respectful climates and with positive outcomes, such as lower levels
P
(A
of drug use.
Students are generally expected to have and show respect for peers and
n
teachers. Showing respect for peers often means not being aggressive toward
io
them either physically or relationally, being polite to them, and refraining
at
from damaging their belongings and blemishing their reputations (DioGuardi
ci
& Theodore, 2006) Within such an egalitarian context, mutual respect proved
so
to be important in promoting friendships (Zongkui, Chumei, & Hsueh, 2006).
As
On the flip side, disrespect for peers who have low social power has been
considered one of the most serious causes of aggression at school: peer victim-
al
ization and bullying. Peer victimization has been defined as repeated exposure
ic
to physical and verbal aggressive actions by peers (Olweus, 1997). Several
og
studies have identified peer bullying as one of the major problems in U.S. schools
ol
(Espelage & Swearer, 2004). It is interesting that when discussing the factors
ch
in a school that affect peer bullying, Batsche and Knoff (1994) suggested that
y
lack of respect for peers and the presence of teachers who seem to overlook the
Ps
problem and not intervene are important. It is not surprising that some of
the best-known intervention programs mention respect in their titles, although
an
the focus on respect in the actual interventions is not strong (e.g., the Expect
ic
Robertson, 2003; the Respect Program described by Ertesvåg & Vaaland, 2007).
Am
Students are clearly expected to respect their teachers, and this type
of respect closely resembles that expected from children in parent–child
e
ity figure who is responsible for the children’s acquisition of culturally valued
by
expected to obey the teachers and show respect by abiding by the teachers’
20
rules and meeting their expectations (e.g., Hsueh, Zhou, Cohen, Hundley, &
Deptula, 2005). Thus, in teacher–student relations, students are expected to
©
In line with the similarity between parents’ and teachers’ roles, several
op
researchers have likened the teacher to a parent (e.g., Wentzel, 2002). For
example, teachers’ respect for students’ autonomy, as in parent–child relation-
C
Bugental and her colleagues (Bugental, Lyon, Lin, McGrath, & Bimbela, 1999)
have similarly likened teachers and parents in their authority roles and found
that a teacher’s low perception of power is associated with intrusive and
coercive educational practices.
A)
As children grow older, respect is expected to be more reciprocal; that
is, both teachers and students are expected to show respect of the unconditional
P
(A
type although, in addition, students have contingent respect for their teachers
based on meritorious qualities, such as expertise, knowledge, or moral integrity
n
(e.g., Chunmei, Zongkui, & Hse, 2005). In this context, students often respect
io
their teachers in response to the respect they receive from them (Noddings,
at
1996). All types of respect—of teachers for students and of students for teachers
ci
and peers—are expected to be negatively associated with aggression. For
so
example, victims of violence (i.e., both pure victims and bullies who are also
As
victims) report feeling less respected at school than other students feel
(Morrison, 2006). Langdon and Preble (2008) examined both adult respect
al
(e.g., “Most of my teachers treat students with fairness and respect”) and peer
ic
respect (e.g., “Students treat each other with fairness and respect at this school”)
og
in school using a large sample of 5th- through 12th-grade students. Each type of
ol
respect uniquely predicted lower levels of bullying at the school after controlling
ch
for background variables such as gender and ethnic origin.
y
parents and respect for teachers on the one hand and various outcomes on
er
the other. We found that respect for teachers was negatively associated with
Am
violence and aggression at school. Though they did not specifically use the
20
term respect, they assessed values that are closely related to facets of respect.
For example, the value called universalism, defined as “understanding, appre-
©
ciation, tolerance, and protection of the welfare of all people and of nature”
ht
(p. 654), reflects the unconditional type of respect, whereas the value called
ig
conformity, defined as “limiting actions and urges that might violate social
yr
expectations and norms” (p. 654), reflects the contingent type of respect for
op
authority. Using a large sample of Jewish and Arab students in Israel, Knafo
et al. found that youth in both ethnic groups who endorsed these values were
C
in which violent behavior was more frequent, these values, and in particular
universalism, were strongly negatively associated with self-reported violence
and provided a stronger protective effect against violence than in schools
where violent behavior was less frequent. It thus appears that values that
A)
reflect respect, either unconditional or contingent, are associated with lower
levels of aggression and violence, and such values seem to be able to counteract
P
(A
contextual factors that favor aggression. In sum, although there are currently
only a handful of studies examining respect in schools directly, there are
n
promising empirical indications that respect of various kinds—teachers for
io
students, students for teachers, and peers for peers—in addition to internalized
at
values that foster respect are associated with lower levels of aggression and
ci
violence at school.
so
As
WHY IS RESPECT SO IMPORTANT AND HOW IS IT DIFFERENT
al
FROM OTHER ATTITUDES OR EMOTIONS?
ic
og
Respect is often conceptually and empirically associated with other atti-
ol
tudes and emotions. For example, examining respect in close relationships,
ch
Frei and Shaver (2002) found that when participants were asked to identify
y
(2006) devised a scale to assess respect for a romantic partner, and it focused
ic
with love and caring or with general power and value within a group. First,
th
unlike empathy or caring, which are “warm” emotions that incline a person
by
caring are strong buffers against aggression, but they do not always have this
beneficial effect. In fact, a large number of clinical case studies of marital vio-
©
lence and child abuse demonstrate that love and caring can sometimes lead
ht
to violence and aggression when they are not associated with respect for the
ig
other’s autonomy and his or her value as a separate and unique human being
yr
(see Chapters 19 and 20). Similarly, viewing another person as having high
op
status or great power is not synonymous with respect and may not be associated
with lower levels of aggression toward the person. In fact, history is full of
C
they valued them and wanted to possess them or tried to destroy them because
of their unjustifiably high status or undeserved power (see Chapter 4). Respect
for others is quite different from this love and status envy. Respect includes a
moral imperative not to harm the other. The value accorded to the other
A)
entails consideration and acceptance of the other’s right to dignity and
autonomy.
P
(A
n
HOW CAN RESPECT BE FOSTERED?
io
at
In every context in which children are reared and educated, a general
ci
attitude of respect can be demonstrated and taught. One of the most influential
so
ways in which respect can be taught and transmitted is by modeling it in daily
As
life. Respect breeds respect, and values and attitudes in particular are inter-
nalized by emulating authority figures (Grusec & Kuczynski, 1997). To foster
al
and teach respect, parents, teachers, and other authority figures should behave
ic
respectfully toward others. Behaving respectfully means honoring and enabling
og
the other’s autonomy, showing interest and care, allowing the other to have
ol
a voice even when not approving its message, and appreciating the other’s
ch
natural goodness. Contingent respect is learned when the unique qualities of
y
context, this requires becoming better acquainted with each student’s unique-
ness and allowing each student to be successful in his or her own way and in
an
behavior toward oneself or others; that is, to foster respect, authority figures
Am
Children and adults learn respect or disrespect partly from the general climate
th
personal and moral standards (Roland & Foxx, 2003). Authority figures can
ig
discussions of values and the importance of good character and moral conduct.
Articulating clear expectations for behaviors that are consistent with respect,
as discussed in this chapter, provides children with concrete and much needed
direction. Following through with helpful scaffolding (e.g., reminders, obser-
vations, examples) helps them internalize these standards and make them
A)
their own. This can be done by helping children articulate and discuss their
feelings, hesitations, and decision processes with regard to their values.
P
(A
Contemporary parents and teachers sometimes shy away from a directive
approach because they see themselves as living in a child-centered world. But
n
good directions, as can be seen in athletic coaching situations, can be beneficial
io
to novices.
at
ci
so
CONCLUSION
As
In sum, the explicit study of respect within contexts in which children
al
are raised, socialized, and educated is still fairly rare, and its results are not
ic
conceptually well integrated. Although constructs related to respect have been
og
examined (e.g., psychological control by parents, acknowledgement of ethnic
ol
diversity in schools), research efforts to conceptualize and measure different
ch
types of respect in home and school contexts, their associations with various
y
hope that the conceptual framework elucidated in this chapter will open
new possibilities to study respect and its vicissitudes, including reductions
an
REFERENCES
e
Barber, B. K. (Ed.). (2002). Intrusive parenting: How psychological control affects children
th
Batsche, G. M., & Knoff, H. M. (1994). Bullies and their victims: Understanding a
pervasive problem in the schools. School Psychology Review, 23, 165–174.
11
Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Watson, M., & Schaps, E. (1997). Caring school commu-
nities. Educational Psychologist, 32, 137–151. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3203_1
20
Benbenishty, R., & Astor, R. A. (2005). School violence in context: Culture, neighborhood,
©
family, school, and gender. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
ht
0033-2909.119.3.422
op
Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. S. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication criteria for
fairness. In B. Sheppard (Ed.), Research on negotiation in organizations (pp. 43–55).
C
A)
Bugental, D. B., & Lewis, J. C. (1999). The paradoxical misuse of power by those who
see themselves as powerless: How does it happen? Journal of Social Issues, 55,
P
(A
51–64. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00104/
Bugental, D. B., Lyon, J. E., Lin, E. K., McGrath, E. P., & Bimbela, A. (1999). Children
n
“tune out” in response to the ambiguous communication style of powerless adults.
io
Child Development, 70, 214–230. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00016
at
ci
Chunmei, Z., Zongkui, Z., & Hse, Y. (2005). The conception of respect and its devel-
so
opment in childhood. Psychological Science (China), 28, 337–341.
As
Darwall, S. L. (1977). Two kinds of respect. Ethics, 88, 36–48. doi:10.1086/292054
de Cremer, D., & Tyler, T. R. (2005). Am I respected or not? Inclusion and reputation
al
as issues in group membership. Social Justice Research, 18, 121–153. doi:10.1007/
ic
s11211-005-7366-3
og
Dillon, R. S. (2007). Arrogance, self respect, and personhood. Journal of Consciousness
Studies, 14, 101–126. ol
ch
DioGuardi, R. J., & Theodore, L. A. (2006). Understanding and addressing peer
y
of school violence and school safety: From research to practice (pp. 339–352). Mahwah,
NJ: Erlbaum.
an
Dixon, S. V., Graber, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2008). The roles of respect for parental
ic
ican, Latino, and European American families. Journal of Family Psychology, 22,
Am
1–10. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.22.1.1
Ertesvåg, S. K., & Vaaland, G. S. (2007). Prevention and reduction of behavioral
e
Frei, J., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Respect in close relationships: Prototype definition,
20
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between marital processes
yr
Grusec, J. E., & Kuczynski, L. (1997). Parenting and children’s internalization of values:
C
Harvey, M. D., & Enzle, M. E. (1978). Effects of retaliation latency and provocation
level on judged blameworthiness for retaliatory aggression. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 4, 579–582. doi:10.1177/014616727800400417
Hendrick, S. S., & Hendrick, C. (2006). Measuring respect in close relationships.
A)
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 881–899. doi:10.1177/0265407
506070471
P
(A
Heuer, L., Blumenthal, E., Douglas, A., & Weinblatt, T. (1999). A deservingness
approach to respect as a relationally based fairness judgment. Personality and
n
Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1279–1292. doi:10.1177/0146167299258009
io
Hsueh, Y., Zhou, Z., Cohen, R., Hundley, R. J., & Deptula, D. P. (2005). Knowing
at
and showing respect: Chinese and U.S. children’s understanding of respect and
ci
its association to their friendships. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 6,
so
229–260.
As
Ikels, C. (Ed.). (2004). Filial piety: Practice and discourse in contemporary East Asia.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
al
Janoff-Bulman, R., & Werther, A. (2008). The social psychology of respect: Implications
ic
for delegitimization and reconciliation. In A. Nadler, T. Malloy, & J. D. Fisher
og
(Eds.), The social psychology of inter-group reconciliation (pp. 145–170). New York,
ol
NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195300314.003.0008
ch
Jussim, L. (1986). Self-fulfilling prophecies: A theoretical and integrative review.
y
Kasen, S., Berenson, K., Cohen, P., & Johnson, J. G. (2004). The effects of school
ic
Knafo, A., Daniel, E., & Khoury-Kassabri, M. (2008). Values as protective factors against
th
violent behavior in Jewish and Arab high schools in Israel. Child Development,
79, 652–667. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01149.x
by
Kumashiro, M., Finkel, E. J., & Rusbult, C. E. (2002). Self-respect and prorelationship
11
JRLP.141.5.469-484
ht
Langdon, S. W., & Preble, W. (2008). The relationship between levels of perceived
ig
respect and bullying in 5th through 12th graders. Adolescence, 43, 485–503.
yr
LaRusso, M. D., Romer, D., & Selman, R. L. (2008). Teachers as builders of respectful
op
school climates: Implications for adolescent drug use norms and depressive symp-
toms in high school. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 386–398. doi:10.1007/
C
s10964-007-9212-4
A)
Implications for harsh parenting practices with very young children. Journal of
Family Psychology, 20, 641–647. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.20.4.641
P
(A
Mayseless, O., & Scharf, M. (2009). Too close for comfort: Inadequate boundaries
with parents and individuation in late adolescent girls. American Journal of
n
Orthopsychiatry, 79, 191–202. doi:10.1037/a0015623
io
at
McCullough, M. (2008). Beyond revenge: The evolution of the forgiveness instinct. San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
ci
so
Meraviglia, M. G., Becker, H., Rosenbluth, B., Sanchez, E., & Robertson, T. (2003).
The Expect Respect Project: Creating a positive elementary school climate. Jour-
As
nal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 1347–1360. doi:10.1177/0886260503257457
al
Miller, D. T. (2001). Disrespect and the experience of injustice. Annual Review of
ic
Psychology, 52, 527–553. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.527
og
Morrison, B. (2006). School bullying and restorative justice: Toward a theoretical
understanding of the role of respect, pride, and shame. Journal of Social Issues,
ol
62, 371–392. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2006.00455.x
ch
Mulcahy, D. G., & Casella, R. (2005). Violence and caring in school and society:
y
es3703_3
an
Noddings, N. (2005). What does it mean to educate the whole child? Educational
Leadership, 63, 8–13.
Am
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. Oxford, England: Harcourt, Brace.
Pitt-Rivers, J. (1965). Honour and social status. In J. G. Peristiany (Ed.), The nature
by
of human society: Honor and shame (pp. 19–78). London, England: Weidenfeld
11
& Nicholson.
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
20
Rubin, K. H., & Chung, O. B. (2006). Parenting beliefs, behaviors, and parent–child
ig
Scharf, M., & Mayseless, O. (2005). Parental authority and its associations with social and
op
A)
HarperCollins.
Steinberg, L. (2004). The 10 basic principles of good parenting. New York, NY: Simon
P
(A
& Schuster.
Wentzel, K. R. (2002). Are effective teachers like good parents? Teaching styles
n
and student adjustment in early adolescence. Child Development, 73, 287–301.
io
doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00406
at
Zongkui, Z., Chumei, Z., & Hsueh, Y. (2006). Relations between conception of
ci
respect and peer relationships in childhood. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 38, 232–239.
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
IV
A)
P
(A
AGGRESSION AT
n
io
THE SOCIETAL LEVEL
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
12051-17_PT4_CH16-rev2.qxd
11
by
5/25/10
th
e
Am
11:58 AM
er
ic
an
Page 296
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-17_PT4_CH16-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:58 AM Page 297
16
PA)
AN EXISTENTIAL PERSPECTIVE
(A
ON VIOLENT SOLUTIONS TO
n
io
ETHNO–POLITICAL CONFLICT
at
ci
so
GILAD HIRSCHBERGER AND TOM PYSZCZYNSKI
As
al
ic
og
War would end if the dead could return.
ol
—Stanley Baldwin, British Prime Minister
y ch
Ps
an
The outbreak of violence between Israel and Hamas on the eve of the
ic
year 2009 surprised hardly anyone. The fragile ceasefire had come to an end,
er
and the renewed missile attacks on towns and villages in the south of Israel
Am
were the appetizer preceding the inevitable main course of massive violent
retribution. But, although the ebbing and flowing of violence in this region
e
intractable conflict? The dictum that “war itself is the enemy,” attributed to
20
the Prussian philosopher von Clausewitz, has never resonated more strongly.
The violent clashes between Israel and the Palestinians have resulted in
©
significant losses to both sides with little gain, if any, to justify the price. Every
ht
round of violence ends with a new shaky agreement that differs ever so slightly
ig
from the shaky agreement that preceded the most recent violent outbreak.
yr
Thus, time and again when the dust settles from the futile attempt to subdue
op
the other, Israelis and Palestinians find themselves with no viable option
but to find a way to live peacefully with one another. But no one seems capable
C
297
12051-17_PT4_CH16-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:58 AM Page 298
Even more perplexing is the fact that since the 1993 peace accord
between Israel and the Palestine Liberation Organization (PLO), relations
between the groups have significantly deteriorated and violence has reached
an unprecedented level. The Israeli disengagement from Gaza in the summer
A)
of 2005 was intended to reduce friction between Israelis and Palestinians, but
it failed to live up to its promise of breaking the cycle of violence and may
P
(A
have inadvertently contributed to the recent escalation of violent conflict.
How can we explain that despite mounting evidence of the futility of violence,
n
and the obvious need for reconciliation among the various factions, there
io
seems to be no end in sight to perpetual warfare? And why has every step
at
taken toward peace paradoxically resulted in a spiral of bitter violence?
ci
One possible answer that many Israelis and Palestinians seem to adhere
so
to is that the other side has proven to be duplicitous, inhumane, and ruthless,
As
using peace as a cover for malevolent intentions (see Chapter 17). In the
present chapter we provide an alternative answer to these questions and
al
argue that powerful psychological forces underlying ethno–political conflict
ic
hamper the ability to achieve peace, even when peace seems to be a rational
og
solution that would benefit all. We base our analysis on terror management
ol
theory (e.g., Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1997) and demonstrate, with
ch
a body of research conducted recently in Iran, Israel, Europe, and the United
y
ical reasoning provides insight into ways to move beyond violence and to
Am
promote peace.
e
th
that the instinctive animal desire for continued life juxtaposed with the
20
attain a sense of personal value, and the promise of literal or symbolic immor-
op
tality to those who live up to these standards; (b) self-esteem, which is acquired
by believing in the cultural worldview and living up to its standards; and
C
A)
these structures is bolstered by others sharing one’s beliefs and diminished
when others adhere to a different system of values and beliefs. According to
P
(A
TMT, because these terror-management processes protect people from deeply
rooted existential fears, much social thought and behavior is oriented toward
n
maintaining them and defending them against threats.
io
Because of the fragile nature of culturally derived forms of defense,
at
threats to these symbolic constructions undermine the emotional security that
ci
they provide and motivate people to protect their death-denying mechanisms
so
to ward off the threats. When others hold beliefs that conflict with one’s own
As
basic conceptions and proclaim their culture’s superiority or moral righteousness,
they imply that one’s worldview is incorrect and one’s culture is inferior, which
al
undermines one’s sense of value and meaning. Such threats to worldviews and
ic
self-esteem strip away an individual’s symbolic defensive shield and then people
og
may express anger and derogate the source of the threats or choose to demon-
ol
strate their group’s superiority by subduing, defeating, or even annihilating
ch
groups that challenge their worldview.
y
propositions of TMT. For example, studies have found that MS increases the
er
imagining physical pain, or worrying about life after college do not produce the
same reactions as MS (e.g., Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Solomon, Simon, & Breus,
11
mental structures often results in extreme reactions toward people who uphold
A)
ferent political orientations than their own (McGregor et al., 1998), White
Americans to express sympathy for a White racist (Greenberg, Schimel,
P
(A
Martens, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 2001), Italians to view their own nation
as superior to other European countries (Castano, 2004), and even Israeli chil-
n
dren as young as 11 to react more negatively to an immigrant child from Russia
io
and more positively toward a child from Israel (Florian & Mikulincer, 1998).
at
These studies have consistently shown that brief, unobtrusive reminders
ci
of mortality lead people to view their group in a more positive light and view
so
other groups in a more negative light to the extent of derogating and even
As
aggressing against other groups. Thus, it may seem that reminders of mortality
instantly turn people into ethnocentric, prejudiced, and potentially violent
al
automatons. Fortunately, TMT views the effects of MS on intergroup attitudes
ic
as more complex and maintains that different people may react to MS in dif-
og
ferent ways, depending on individual differences and the situational context.
ol
For example, research has shown that whereas people with a conservative
ch
political orientation respond to MS with greater intolerance, people with a
y
Simon, Pyszczynski, Solomon, & Chatel, 1992). Other research has shown that
MS leads to heightened ingroup affiliation only when the ingroup is portrayed
an
as strong and successful but leads to disaffiliation from elements of the worldview
ic
In the realm of political conflict, additional variables may enter into the
equation and further complicate the effect of existential concerns on violent
e
outcomes depends on three major conditions: (a) social and national consensus
20
on the use of violence, (b) a sense that violence is justified and necessary, and
(c) a sense that violence is imminent and unavoidable. In the next step, we
©
outline conditions that may disrupt the link between death concerns and
ht
moderate and even reverse the effects of MS on violent inclinations. In the final
op
PA)
The hypothesis that death awareness motivates violent solutions to
(A
political conflict is the most straightforward application of TMT to the realm
of political psychology. War and terrorism simplify conflict and dichotomize
n
groups into “us” and “them,” “good” and “evil.” Violence also offers hope for a
io
clear-cut and long-lasting resolution of the conflict, the potential (or illusion)
at
of pronouncing winners and losers, and a better future following victory. These
ci
so
attributes of political violence make it particularly attractive when death is
salient, because under these conditions people are motivated to promote the
As
triumph of their group and the thorough defeat of the opposition.
Indeed, research has revealed that among conservative Americans MS
al
leads to greater support of extreme violence against countries or organizations
ic
that pose a threat to the United States (Pyszczynski et al., 2006, Study 2). It
og
has led to Israeli settlers in the Gaza Strip, and their supporters who refused
ol
to accept the 2005 disengagement plan, to support more violent resistance
ch
(Hirschberger & Ein-Dor, 2006). Research has also shown that MS led par-
y
& Arndt, 2008) to express greater willingness to sacrifice their life for their
country. However, despite the seemingly clear link between mortality concerns
an
and support for intergroup violence, we contend that this link is neither auto-
ic
Consensus
e
th
extent on the broad support of the populations for whom these actions are pur-
portedly undertaken. When leaders receive the support of their people, they feel
11
less restrained in sending young men and women to the battlefield, and they are
20
under less pressure from their publics to end the war. However, consensus is
fragile, and often the price of war and the inability to achieve the expected
©
cerns about the costs of such policies (in terms of loss of life, resources, and
ig
international respect), and the legitimacy and wisdom of the leader and his
yr
can transform into disillusionment, frustration, and anger. Such was the
case in the American war in Vietnam and more recently in the war in Iraq,
C
where initial consensus and hope dissipated when the war failed to deliver
its promise. In Israel, the Lebanon War of 1982 and, to some extent, the
Lebanon War of 2006 were launched following missile attacks on Northern
Israel and enjoyed public support until the death toll rose, and it became evident
that the declared goals of the war were not attainable by violent means.
A)
From a terror management perspective, consensus is a necessary ingredient
for the functioning of the cultural worldview as an effective anxiety buffer.
P
(A
Because worldviews are symbolic social constructions that are fragile and
susceptible to disconfirming information, they require constant consensual
n
validation. For consensus to be established and maintained it is necessary to
io
believe that the threat is of such magnitude that massive use of force is the
at
only effective response.
ci
Consensus is clearly a requirement for military action in democratic
so
societies, where leaders receive their legitimacy from the support of their
As
constituents. There is good reason to believe that consensus is important
for violent behavior in nondemocratic societies as well. For example, in the
al
Palestinian territories, suicide bombers are exalted as martyrs. Their pictures
ic
are posted on walls and buildings, and they are admired by children who view
og
them as role models. Such idolization of people who are ready to kill themselves
ol
and others for what is considered a holy cause is necessary for the propagation
ch
of suicidal terrorism because in return for a shortened life, terrorists gain
y
fame, adoration, respect, and honor (not to mention several afterlife virgins).
Ps
feeling that certain aspects of the self will survive physical death.
ic
sus regarding violent responses to conflict as well as the contention that con-
Am
2004) demonstrated that shortly after the beginning of the Iraq War, reminders
th
of death increased support for American president George W. Bush and his
by
political issues. Half of the participants read a description in which the student
ht
expressed support for martyrdom attacks against Western targets, and the
ig
A)
support for suicidal terrorism, participants had to feel that there was consensus
among their peers supporting such violence.
P
(A
Justice
n
io
Consensus for violent solutions to conflict may be established when
at
people feel that they are fighting for a just and noble cause. However, just
ci
because a cause is noble does not mean it will be achieved using violent means
so
or that violence is the most effective route to attain the desired goal. Early
As
scholars of war and conflict viewed the use of violence as a rational option that
could advance a country’s interests, as Clausewitz (1832/1976) contended,
al
“War is not a mere act of policy but a true political instrument, a continuation
ic
of political activity by other means,” implying that the decision to engage in
og
war is the product of a rational cost–benefit analysis. From this perspective,
ol
war is waged when leaders conclude that war is a more efficient way to achieve
ch
political goals than other available means.
y
assumption and posits that by delineating the costs and benefits of conflict,
political scientists may better understand the motives for going to war.
an
However, over the years scholars of conflict resolution have concluded that
ic
rational factors alone do little to explain the outburst of violent conflict and
er
revenge play a pivotal role in the decision to engage in war (e.g., Baumeister
& Butz, 2005).
e
the two world wars), Welch (1993) concluded that the motivation to achieve
justice or restore justice has been a major factor in most global conflicts. Welch
©
further contended that the sense of injustice involves powerful emotions that
ht
often hinder a rational analysis of costs and benefits, and may lead to decisions
ig
argue that when death is salient, justice motives gain prominence and may
A)
and Pyszczynski et al. (2003) argued that part of the appeal of President Bush
and his counter-terrorist polices immediately following 9/11 was that existential
P
(A
fear increased the desire to vanquish evil and restore justice.
In a series of four studies conducted in 2008 (Hirschberger, Pyszczynski,
n
& Ein-Dor, 2009b), we examined whether MS would increase justice motives
io
for violence and whether the motivation to achieve or restore justice would
at
be greater than rational utilitarian considerations. In Study 1, Israeli partici-
ci
pants were randomly assigned to MS and control conditions and then read a
so
description of a missile attack from the Gaza Strip on an Israeli town. Some
As
participants were told that security experts believed that a military incursion
into Gaza was likely to significantly diminish Hamas’s ability to fire more
al
missiles (utility condition). Other participants were told that security experts
ic
believed that an incursion into Gaza would not reduce Hamas’s ability to fire
og
more missiles, nor would it effectively deter Hamas, but it would restore a
ol
sense of justice to the Israeli public (justice condition). All participants were
ch
asked to indicate their support for a military incursion into Gaza. The results
y
revealed that in both the justice and utility conditions, MS led to greater
Ps
seem to imply that people still desire violence even when they understand
Am
for both justice and utility reasons, but they did not indicate whether justice
th
motives are more prominent than utility motives when death is salient. In the
by
next study we developed a scale to measure justice and utility motives for
violence, which would enable us to measure these motives within each subject
11
developed the Justice, Utility, and Peace Inventory (JUPI), which consists of
questions favoring violence to restore justice (justice factor; e.g., “A military
©
strike on Gaza will make the Palestinians pay for their crimes”), favoring
ht
violence for utilitarian reasons (utility factor; e.g., “A military strike on Gaza
ig
(peace factor; e.g., “A military strike on Gaza hurts the chances for peace”).
op
A factor analysis confirmed the factorial structure of the JUPI, and correlations
between the JUPI and other relevant instruments, such as the Right-Wing
C
Authoritarianism Scale (Altemeyer, 1981) and the Need for Cognitive Closure
A)
description of a missile attack from Gaza on an Israeli town, as in the first
study. However, half of the participants read that the attack resulted only in
P
(A
some minor damage and no casualties (mild outcome condition), and the
other half read the description used in the first study wherein several people
n
were killed or wounded during the attack (severe outcome condition). Then
io
all participants completed the JUPI. Results revealed that in the mild outcome
at
condition MS had no significant impact on the JUPI. However, in the severe
ci
outcome condition MS led to greater endorsement of the justice factor
so
compared with the control condition. There was no significant effect of MS
As
on the utility and peace items.
The results of this study indicate that when participants are given the
al
opportunity to endorse both justice and utility items, MS has a significant
ic
effect only on the justice items and not on the utility items. Furthermore, the
og
impact of MS on justice motives for violence was evident only when the
ol
outcome of the attack was severe and not when it was mild. Perhaps for MS
ch
to increase justice motives one needs to be in an enraged state of mind that
y
or an essay that argued that decisions are best made on an emotional, intuitive
ic
full-scale attack against Gaza. Following this description, participants were asked
th
to indicate whether, given the choice, they would favor a limited attack, as
by
recommended, or a full scale attack, and they were asked to indicate to what
extent they felt confident about their choice. Thus, MS, cognitive mode, and
11
The level of confidence they expressed in the decision they made served as
the dependent variable. Results revealed that participants who favored a
©
emotions and intuitions. The results of this study suggest that confidence in
op
are induced to think with their gut rather than their mind.
A)
grief Dr. Abu El-Aish pleaded in a press conference that the violence be
stopped and that Israelis and Palestinians find a way to live in peace. He also
P
(A
insisted that there was no reason to target his house, as there were no terrorists
shooting from it. A mother of an Israeli soldier angrily interrupted his speech
n
and accused him of harboring weapons or terrorists; otherwise why would
io
anyone bomb his house?
at
At first sight, this rude and insensitive interruption could be seen as the
ci
epitome of coldheartedness. However, from a terror management perspective
so
this behavior is different only in style, but not in essence, from other desper-
As
ate attempts to defend the cultural worldview at all costs. From this point of
view, Dr. Abu-Aish represented for two major reasons a severe threat to
al
the predominant Israeli worldview that supported the war. First, he insisted
ic
that there were no hostile activities taking place in his house, undermining the
og
position that all the casualties of the war were justified. For the accusing
ol
woman (a mother of a soldier), the possibility that some of the killing in Gaza
ch
could not be explained or justified posed an unbearable threat to her belief
y
system. Second, in spite of his devastating loss, the doctor remained steadfast
Ps
in his belief in peace and coexistence, threatening the need to believe that the
other side of the conflict consists only of inhumane, cruel, and savage terrorists.
an
In her almost instinctive attack on the doctor (which she later regretted), the
ic
Israeli woman defended her worldview from the possibility that the war was
er
anything but a just, moral, and necessary clash between the forces of good
Am
and evil. This small episode in a larger war demonstrates the resistance of a
population at war to any information that might undermine consensus that
e
workings of the justice motive and the mental acrobatics people will perform to
by
Rallying public support for a war not only requires that people perceive the
©
cause as just and view their group as representing forces of good fighting against
ht
evil, but people also need to believe that there is no alternative and that war
ig
is imminent and unavoidable (see Chapter 17). For example, the American
yr
public perceived the September 11th attacks as the beginning of a violent and
op
inevitable clash with radical Islam, and following the terrorist attacks support
for President Bush and his war on terrorism was high. In contrast, the 2004
C
terrorist attacks on Madrid had the opposite effect: Support for the ruling Partido
A)
changed its foreign policy and withdrew its support from the war in Iraq.
The difference between an imminent and inevitable war from an
P
(A
American perspective, and an undesirable and unnecessary war from a Spanish
perspective, led to diametrically opposed reactions to the attacks in the two
n
countries. For Americans, the seemingly inevitable route to violent conflict
io
left the impression of no alternative options and perhaps elevated terror
at
management mechanisms in the form of a symbolic war against evil. For the
ci
Spanish, who perceived the terrorist attacks as the price they were paying for
so
the ill-considered policy of their government, the elevated death awareness
As
following the attacks made their own vulnerability to harm salient, and
they reacted by replacing their leadership with a more peaceful one. The fact
al
that the Madrid attacks occurred several days before the election probably
ic
contributed to the Spanish sentiment that power was in their hands to avoid
og
an unnecessary violent confrontation.
ol
Recent research lends support to this analysis and indicates that when
ch
violence seems inevitable, MS leads to greater support for violent solutions
y
to conflict. In one study (Hirschberger et al., 2009c, Study 1), MS led to greater
Ps
of the State of Israel and for the continued development of Iran’s nuclear
ic
program. In a similar study (Hirschberger et al., 2009c, Study 3), Israelis who
er
lived in a region attacked by missiles during the 2006 Lebanon War expressed
Am
the fact that real-life conflict has not only symbolic but also real implications.
yr
That is, as much as people engaged in conflict are concerned about protect-
op
ing their symbolic death-denying structures, so too are they concerned about
their own physical safety. Reminders of personal mortality have been shown
C
to engage motivation to defend the worldview, but it is also likely that they bring
to mind the possibility of dying in a war. What determines people’s choice of war
or peace when mortality is salient? So far, we have demonstrated that MS leads
to greater support for political violence when social consensus is high, when the
cause is perceived as just, and when conflict seems inevitable. In this section we
A)
argue that MS will lead to less violent intentions when (a) perceived personal
vulnerability to conflict-related injury or death is high, (b) adversary rhetoric
P
(A
raises the possibility of a nonviolent solution, (c) experts advise that violence is
counterproductive, and (d) people are induced to think rationally.
n
To examine the role of perceived vulnerability and adversary intent
io
in moderating the link between MS and political violence, we (Hirschberger
at
et al., 2009c) conducted a series of studies to examine the dynamic interplay
ci
between symbolic terror management defenses, concrete self-protection from
so
physical danger, and their effects on support of violent solutions to political
As
conflict. In Study 1, we focused on the growing tensions between Israel and
Iran over the development of Iran’s nuclear program. Participants completed
al
the typical MS procedure and were then randomly assigned to read either a
ic
conflict-escalating speech by an Iranian leader against Israel and the West or
og
a conflict-de-escalating speech that implies that violence is not inevitable.
ol
Participants then rated their support of extremely violent reactions against
ch
Iran, including a preemptive nuclear strike. Results revealed that in the esca-
y
against Iran, but in the de-escalating scenario the opposite pattern was observed,
and MS decreased support of extreme preemptive violence relative to the
an
control condition.
ic
violent motives. However, because MS makes salient not only the need for a
symbolic worldview but also the fact of personal vulnerability to harm and to
e
death, when there were reasons to believe that violence might be averted, the
th
need for personal safety overrode the defense of the symbolic worldview, and
by
of tensions with Iran, participants were randomly assigned to two groups. The
first group was asked to reflect on the possibility that they or their loved ones
©
might be hurt in a future conflict between Iran and Israel. The second group
ht
was asked to reflect on the content of the passage. All participants then
ig
who reflected only on the content of the passage responded to MS with increased
op
A)
groups of participants that differed in their level of exposure to war-related
violence (matched on other potentially confounding variables). The first group
P
(A
consisted of participants who lived in Northern Israel during the Second
Lebanon War against Hezbollah (summer, 2006) and had directly experienced
n
missile attacks. The second group consisted of participants who lived in other
io
parts of Israel and had never been directly exposed to conflict-related violence.
at
All participants completed the MS procedure and then read either a conflict-
ci
escalating speech or a conflict-de-escalating speech by a leader of Hezbollah,
so
and answered questions on support of a preemptive attack against Hezbollah.
As
Results revealed that for participants in the no-war-exposure group,
MS led to greater support of violence regardless of Hezbollah rhetoric. However,
al
among participants in the war-exposure group, MS led to greater support of
ic
violence in the escalation scenario but led to a reduced support of violence
og
among participants in the de-escalation scenario. These findings suggest that
ol
among persons who feel less vulnerable to conflict-related violence, MS leads
ch
to increased support of violence regardless of whether the adversary’s rhetoric
y
safety. However, among participants who had experienced the war firsthand, the
influence of MS on support of violence was contingent on adversary rhetoric:
an
When it seemed that war was imminent, it increased support of violence, but
ic
when war seemed avoidable it had the opposite effect and reduced support of
er
violence. When one’s own life and that of one’s family is on the line, people are
Am
more likely to consider nonviolent options when they perceive that violence
can be averted.
e
participants were told that security experts believed that an incursion into
20
Gaza would be counterproductive and was likely only to increase attacks against
Israel (futility of violence condition). Counter to the other conditions, in this
©
only found that when participants were induced to decide intuitively and
op
A)
and may lead to a reduction in violent motivations under MS conditions,
are encouraging. But they still suggest that humans are defensive violence-prone
P
(A
creatures in one way or another. In this section of the chapter we demonstrate
that focusing on common humanity with others, on compassionate religious
n
values, and on feelings of psychological security enables people to transcend
io
their fear and respond to MS with greater tolerance of others.
at
In a study examining support of political violence in a sample of Israelis
ci
(Hirschberger et al., 2009a), participants were assigned to either an MS con-
so
dition, a pain salience condition, a “Holocaust as a crime against the Jewish
As
people” condition, or a “Holocaust as a crime against humanity” condition
(based on Wohl & Branscombe, 2005). They then answered questions tapping
al
support of violent solutions to conflict. In the MS condition and the “Holocaust
ic
as a crime against the Jewish people” condition, support of violence was signifi-
og
cantly higher compared with the pain salience condition and the “Holocaust
ol
as a crime against humanity” condition. These results suggest that describing
ch
the Holocaust as a crime against the Jewish people led to effects similar to MS.
y
text and even after reading about nonbiblical compassionate values. However,
th
reduce support for violence (see Chapters 13 and 14). On this basis, Weise and
his colleagues (2008) primed American participants with thoughts of death
©
and then asked them to visualize a warm and accepting personal relationship.
ht
measures was reduced after participants thought about close personal relation-
yr
Arias Ben-Tal, Pyszczynski, & Ein-Dor, 2009), participants had to write a brief
comment following MS about a sketch of a woman breastfeeding her child
C
A)
security condition.
P
(A
CONCLUSIONS
n
io
The conflict in the Middle East that currently involves Israel, Arab
at
nations, Iran, Europe, and the United States is concerned with disputes over
ci
land, water, oil, terrorism, occupation, historical rights, and religious promises.
so
These important reasons notwithstanding, we have suggested in this chapter
As
that human existential concerns also contribute to the perpetuation of this
conflict and the inability to reach a peaceful solution. We have reviewed
al
research that sheds light on the underlying mechanisms that transform
ic
unobtrusive, brief reminders of personal mortality into powerful motives to
og
subdue an enemy. We demonstrated that perceived consensus, belief in a just
ol
cause, and belief that war is inevitable are powerful catalysts that amplify
ch
the impact of mortality concerns on support for violent solutions to conflict.
y
However, we also demonstrated that when people focus on the personal price
Ps
they might pay in a war, when they perceive the adversary as harboring less
malevolent intentions, and when they are induced to think rationally MS has
an
Moreover, when people think of their common humanity with others, focus
er
make painful compromises on the concrete issues at stake, but they will also
th
have to change the way they think about their adversaries and transform the
by
violent conflicts and render them resistant to change. However, our research
20
also suggests that the path to real change and to peace requires that, as people
and as nations, we confront our deepest fears with courage rather than denying
©
them. Although many political plans for peace have been proposed over the
ht
years, none has been successful. Perhaps in addition to diplomacy and com-
ig
changing the psychological forces that promote hatred to those that promote
op
peace are needed to involve in the process of peacemaking people on all sides
of the conflict.
C
REFERENCES
Abdollahi, A., Henthorn, C., & Pyszczynski, T. (2009). Experimental peace psychol-
ogy: Priming consensus mitigates aggression against out-groups under mortality
A)
salience. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression, 2, 30–37.
Altemeyer, B. (1981). Right-wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg, Canada: University of
P
(A
Manitoba Press.
Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Schimel, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2002). To belong
n
or not to belong, that is the question: Terror management and identification
io
with gender and ethnicity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 26–43.
at
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.1.26
ci
so
Baumeister, R. F., & Butz, J. (2005). Roots of hate, violence, and evil. In R. J. Sternberg
(Ed.), The psychology of hate (pp. 87–102). Washington, DC: American Psy-
As
chological Association. doi:10.1037/10930-005
Bueno de Mesquita, B. (1988). The contribution of expected utility theory to the study
al
of international conflict. The Journal of Interdisciplinary History, 18, 629–652.
ic
doi:10.2307/204818
og
Castano, E. (2004). In case of death, cling to the ingroup. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 34, 375–384. doi:10.1002/ejsp.211
ol
ch
Clausewitz, C. V. (1976). On war (M. Howard & P. Paret, Eds. &Trans.). Princeton, NJ:
y
Dechesne, M., Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., & Schimel, J. (2000). Terror management
an
and the vicissitudes of sports fan affiliation: The effects of mortality salience on
optimism and fan identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 813–835.
ic
doi:10.1002/1099-0992(200011/12)30:6<813::AID-EJSP17>3.0.CO;2-M
er
Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1998). Terror management in childhood: Does death
Am
1104–1112. doi:10.1177/01461672982410007
th
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1997). Terror management theory
by
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S.,
& Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence for terror management theory II: The effects of
©
0022-3514.58.2.308
yr
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Simon, L., & Breus, M. (1994). The role
op
0022-3514.67.4.627
Greenberg, J., Schimel, J., Martens, A., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (2001). Sym-
pathy for the devil: Evidence that reminding whites of their mortality promotes
more favorable reactions to white racists. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 113–133.
doi:10.1023/A:1010613909207
A)
Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Porteus, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1995). Evidence
of a terror management function of cultural icons: The effects of mortality salience
P
on the inappropriate use of cherished cultural symbols. Personality and Social
(A
Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1221–1228. doi:10.1177/01461672952111010
n
Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Chatel, D. (1992). Terror
io
management and tolerance: Does mortality salience always intensify negative
at
reactions to others who threaten one’s worldview? Journal of Personality and
ci
Social Psychology, 63, 212–220. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.212
so
Hirschberger, G. (2006). Terror management and attributions of blame to innocent
As
victims: Reconciling compassionate and defensive responses. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 91, 832–844. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.832
al
Hirschberger, G., Arias Ben-Tal, K., Pyszczynski, T., & Ein-Dor, T. (2009). Peace and
ic
psychological security: Evidence that security primes moderate the impact of mortality
og
salience on violent inclinations. Unpublished manuscript, Interdisciplinary Center
(IDC) Herzliya, Israel. ol
ch
Hirschberger, G., & Ein-Dor, T. (2006). Defenders of a lost cause: Terror management
y
and violent resistance to the disengagement plan. Personality and Social Psychology
Ps
Hirschberger, G., Pyszczynski, T., Canetti, D., & Ein-Dor, T. (2009). Never again:
Evidence that reminders of the holocaust elicit support for violent solutions to contem-
ic
Israel.
Am
Hirschberger, G., Pyszczynski, T., & Ein-Dor, T. (2009a). The commonalities and differ-
ences between group mortality and individual mortality: A terror management perspec-
e
Ramat-Gan, Israel.
by
Hirschberger, G., Pyszczynski, T., & Ein-Dor, T. (2009b). Death and the deliverance
of justice: Mortality salience increases support for vindictive yet inefficient violent
11
Israel.
Hirschberger, G., Pyszczynski, T., & Ein-Dor, T. (2009c). Vulnerability and vigilance:
©
Threat awareness and perceived adversary intent moderate the impact of mortality
ht
597–607. doi:10.1177/0146167208331093
yr
Landau, M. J., Johns, M., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Martens, A., Goldenberg, J. L.,
op
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.190
Landau, M. J., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., . . .
Cook, A. (2004). Deliver us from evil: The effects of mortality salience and
reminders of 9/11 on support for president George W. Bush. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1136–1150. doi:10.1177/0146167204267988
A)
McGregor, H., Lieberman, J. D., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Simon, L., &
Pyszczynski, T. (1998). Terror management and aggression: Evidence that
P
mortality salience motivates aggression against worldview threatening others.
(A
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 590–605. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.
n
74.3.590
io
Pyszczynski, T., Abdollahi, A., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Cohen, F., & Weise, D.
at
(2006). Mortality salience, martyrdom, and military might: The great Satan
ci
versus the axis of evil. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 525–537.
so
doi:10.1177/0146167205282157
As
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual process model of defense
against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: An extension of
al
terror management theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835–845. doi:10.1037/
ic
0033-295X.106.4.835
og
Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2003). In the wake of 9/11: The psychology
ol
of terror. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/
10478-000
ch
Rothschild, Z. K., Abdollahi, A., & Pyszczynski, T. (2008). Does peace have a prayer?
y
Ps
increases efforts to affirm a symbolic immortal self at the expense of the physical
er
Webster, D., & Kruglanski, A. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive
closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062. doi:10.1037
e
/0022-3514.67.6.1049
th
Weise, D. R., Pyszczynski, T., Cox, C., Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., &
by
448–455. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02108.x
20
Welch, D. (1993). Justice and the genesis of war. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511521805
©
0022-3514.88.2.288
op
C
17
PA)
THE EMOTIONAL ROOTS
(A
OF INTERGROUP AGGRESSION:
n
io
THE DISTINCT ROLES OF ANGER
at
ci
AND HATRED
so
As
ERAN HALPERIN
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
dimensional, biased lens and therefore perceive the other group’s actions
by
be the reasons for their current actions or statements (Bar-Tal & Halperin,
2010).
©
is a pivotal emotion in every conflict (see Chapters 1 and 9 for cognitive analy-
ig
ses of anger arousal). Think for example of the emotional experiences of U.S.
yr
citizens who watched the televised 9/11 terrorist attacks on the World Trade
op
Center almost as they were happening. Surely rage (among other emotions)
was central to their experience. Similarly, it is not hard to imagine the rapid
C
heartbeats, the sweaty palms and faces, and the extreme anger felt by Jewish
315
12051-18_CH17-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:58 AM Page 316
Israelis who sat down to a Passover dinner on March 27, 2002, and suddenly
heard about the destructive suicide bombing at the Park Hotel in Netanya,
which resulted in the deaths of 30 citizens dressed in festive holiday clothing.
The main issue in this chapter is the role played by public anger in the U.S.
A)
decision to invade Iraq or in Israel’s decision to initiate a large-scale military
action against the Palestinians in March of 2002.
P
We know that emotions in general, and anger in particular, play a
(A
pivotal role in driving people to aggressive behavior (see Chapters 6 and 12).
n
It has long been assumed in social psychology that anger is the most pow-
io
erful emotional determinant of aggressive behavior (e.g., Berkowitz, 1993).
at
Accordingly, there is evidence that flare-ups of public anger automatically
ci
lead to widespread support for vengeful aggression (Huddy, Feldman, &
so
Cassese, 2007; Skitka, Bauman, Aramovich, & Morgan, 2006). In reality,
As
however, despite the prevalence of ingroup anger following an outgroup
provocation, public opinion is often divided about the best response (Maoz
al
& McCauley, 2008). Some people clearly advocate an aggressive response,
ic
but others counsel self-restraint. These differences in opinion can be explained,
og
at least in part, by differences in rational calculations concerning the costs
ol
and benefits of aggressive reactions. But I argue in this chapter that rational
ch
analyses are not sufficient to explain public opinion; in addition, a deeper
y
is needed.
Specifically, I contend that in the context of prolonged intergroup
an
the opposing group. In other words, in the absence of long-term hatred for
Am
the outgroup (and especially when a degree of empathy exists), anger will not
necessarily lead to support for aggression, and in some cases it may even lead
e
long-term hatred for an opponent will not necessarily cause aggression in the
by
absence of an event that triggers explosive anger. That is, only the combined
influences of short-term anger and long-term hatred are likely to produce
11
intergroup violence. I then review recent findings concerning the roles of anger
yr
and hatred in the context of the Middle-East conflict. Finally, I present new
op
data gathered in that context to illustrate the joint effects of anger and hatred
C
A)
aggressive action in intergroup conflicts, we need a working definition of
emotion. For this purpose, I accept William James’s (1884) perspective
P
(A
on emotions as response tendencies. According to this view (as elaborated, for
example, by Frijda, 1986, and Scherer, 1984), emotions are flexible response
n
sequences evoked when a person evaluates a situation as offering important
io
challenges or opportunities (Tooby & Cosmides, 1990). In other words,
at
emotions transform a substantive event into a motive to respond to it in a
ci
particular manner (Zajonc, 1998).
so
Core components of emotion include subjective feelings, bodily changes,
As
facial expressions, and other physiological reactions (Shaver, Schwartz,
Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987). These components help to distinguish emo-
al
tions from other psychological phenomena, such as attitudes or beliefs
ic
(Cacioppo & Gardner, 1999). Following Averill (1994), I view emotions as
og
also comprising stories that help people interpret events and guide their
ol
behavioral reactions. This perspective highlights two additional compo-
ch
nents, cognitive appraisals and emotional goals that guide response tenden-
y
cies, which I view as central to the links between emotions and support for
Ps
intergroup aggression.
Extensive research has illuminated the role of cognitive appraisal in the
an
2004; Shaver et al., 1987; Smith & Ellsworth 1985). It is now well established
er
behavior.
The emotional behavior that one can observe from outside is thought
11
to be the expression of what Frijda (1986; Frijda, Kuipers, & ter Schure, 1989)
20
and emotional goals associated with particular emotions are usually present
yr
(Frijda et al., 1989; Roseman, 2002). In this view, a discrete emotion such as
op
the emotion.
Emotions are just one of several kinds of affective responses (Gross, 2007).
For the purposes of this chapter, it is important to distinguish between emo-
tions and sentiments. As I have just explained, emotions are multicomponent
responses to particular kinds of events. Sentiments, by contrast, are enduring
A)
configurations of emotions or emotional predispositions (Arnold, 1960;
Frijda, 1986; Lazarus, 1994). According to this view, an emotional sentiment
P
(A
is a relatively stable emotional disposition toward a person, group, or symbol
(Halperin, 2008a). Emotional sentiments also differ from another kind of
n
affective state, moods, in that moods typically do not have well-defined objects
io
(Oatley, Keltner, & Jenkins, 2006), whereas emotional sentiments do. In my
at
view, as I explain in subsequent sections of this chapter, only a combination
ci
of a specific long-term sentiment and a momentarily triggered discrete emotion
so
toward an outgroup will lead to support for intergroup aggression.
As
The emotional sentiments and emotions relevant to the present discussion
reflect intergroup and not interpersonal phenomena. In recent years, there
al
has been growing interest in the concept of intergroup emotions, emotions that
ic
are felt by individuals as a result of their membership in or identification with
og
a certain group or society and are targeted toward another group (Iyer & Leach,
ol
2008; Mackie, Devos, & Smith, 2000; Smith & Mackie, 2008). Research has
ch
shown that people may experience emotions not only in response to personal
y
experiences and activities but also in response to events that affect other
Ps
Seger, & Mackie, 2007; Wohl, Branscombe, & Klar, 2006; Yzerbyt, Dumont,
ic
Wigboldus, & Gordin, 2003). I want to focus here on ways in which events
er
tudes, and actions of individuals and groups influence the course of a conflict.
by
Kriesberg, 1993).
ht
established that extend way beyond any immediate provocative event. The
yr
continuing conflict (Kelman & Fisher, 2003). The interaction between these
A)
EMOTIONS AND AGGRESSION IN INTERGROUP CONFLICT:
P
(A
AN APPRAISAL-BASED MODEL
n
The model in Figure 17.1 shows how exposure to a particular conflict-
io
related event is processed and transformed into support for certain political
at
and military actions. The model has been tested among Israeli Jews and found
ci
to be valid and analytically useful (Halperin, 2008a). According to the model,
so
the link between exposure to a conflict-related event and a person’s attitude
As
concerning the required response to the outgroup is mediated by a discrete
emotional response elicited by the event. In turn, this emotional response is
al
influenced by the individual’s long-term emotional sentiments toward the
ic
outgroup (Halperin, Sharvit, & Gross, 2010). The model is sufficiently
og
general to capture processes initiated by various conflict-related events, includ-
ol
ing positive events (e.g., a new peace proposal) or negative ones (e.g., a terror
ch
attack). In the current chapter, however, I use the model to explain how a
y
Ps
an
long-term
ic
emotional
sentiment
er
about the
adversary
Am
e
th
cognitive
provocative/ emotions— for
appraisals of
aggressive personal or aggressive
the events
11
non-affective
factors (e.g.,
ig
ideology,
yr
personality)
op
C
A)
personally, in most cases they are experienced directly by only a few group
members and transmitted to other group members through leaders, the mass
P
(A
media, or one or more social networks. If a person identifies with the same
group as the directly affected individuals, he or she will experience group-based
n
emotions (Mackie et al., 2000; Smith, 1993; Yzerbyt et al., 2003).
io
Such events will elicit individual and group-based emotions and the
at
ensuing political response tendencies, depending on how the events are
ci
appraised. For example, if a violent act committed by outgroup members against
so
one or more members of the ingroup is appraised as unjust and is accompanied
As
by appraisal of the ingroup as strong, it will induce anger (Halperin, 2008b;
Huddy et al., 2007; Shaver et al., 1987). Hence, the cognitive appraisal of an
al
event is a crucial factor in determining the kind of emotion that will be evoked
ic
by the event.
og
When an event occurs in the context of a long-term conflict, the appraisal
ol
of the event is influenced by three sets of factors: (a) the way in which the event
ch
is presented to the public (i.e., the framing of the event by leaders and media
y
ity, moral values, and implicit schemas or theories (e.g., Halperin, 2008a;
Sharvit, Halperin, & Rosler, 2008); and (c) most important, a set of long-term
an
long-term external threats to one’s group are likely to cause bouts of fear and
th
strong anger in all members of the victimized society, the nature of that anger
ht
A)
tion of a long-term threat posed by the outgroup (Maoz & McCauley, 2008;
Struch & Schwartz, 1989). Yet in the majority of cases, conflict of interest or
P
(A
even perceived threat alone will not bring about violence in the absence of
an immediate provocation.
n
In terms of the more immediate antecedents of intergroup aggression, it
io
seems that public support for aggression and violence results, in most cases,
at
from a shared belief that the outgroup committed a grievous, unjust action
ci
(or made a provocative, unfair statement) aimed at the ingroup (see Chapter 16).
so
To be an adequate trigger of violence, the action or statement must usually be
As
perceived as an extreme deviation from moral norms and a notable deviation
from the routine (hence acceptable) reciprocal intergroup hostility.
al
A second precondition for supporting aggressive action is perceiving the
ic
ingroup as strong enough to overpower the outgroup in a future military
og
battle. (See Chapter 3 for a similar analysis at the interpersonal level, and
ol
Chapter 16 for a similar analysis at the intergroup level.) This perception of
ch
strength and controllability provides the confidence necessary to undertake
y
Finally, given the high probable costs (in resources, human lives, and
er
support them only as a last resort (see Chapter 16). In other words, most
members who believe that their group is capable of correcting a perceived wrong
e
the use of military power, at least for the time being. Moreover, in most cases,
by
character. If this is not the case, other paths of correction may be perceived
as equally beneficial and much less costly than the aggressive one.
©
ht
ig
A)
member) appraised the event. They were then asked to name the emotions
experienced by the protagonist in relation to the event.
P
(A
Both anger and hatred were associated with blaming the outgroup
for the conflict-related event and with appraising the outgroup’s behavior
n
as unpleasant, hurtful, and contrary to the ingroup’s interests and goals.
io
Nevertheless, in line with previous theoretical writings about anger and hatred
at
(e.g., Ben-Zeev, 1992; Royzman, McCauley, & Rozin, 2005; Sternberg, 2003),
ci
there was also a clear distinction between the appraisals associated with each
so
of these phenomena. The negative appraisals associated with anger focused
As
solely on unfairness of the outgroup’s specific action, whereas the appraisals
associated with hatred focused on the nature of the outgroup itself, suggesting
al
that its actions were not aimed at achieving instrumental goals but stemmed
ic
from a malevolent disposition to hurt the ingroup.
og
Important as they may be, cognitive appraisals are just one part of the
ol
emotion process. It is well established that the appraisals associated with each
ch
emotion underpin the unique content of its corresponding motivational
y
of the problematic situation inherent in the appraisals (e.g., Frijda et al., 1989).
For example, fear aroused by seeing a tiger about to attack leads to running
an
away or hiding behind a barrier of some kind; but fear that one is about to miss
ic
the last train home leads to running toward the door of the train. Therefore, in
er
another study (Halperin, 2008b, Study 3) I examined the emotional goals and
Am
response tendencies that are associated with anger and hatred in the context
of intergroup conflict.
e
people to rank the level of each of several emotional reactions (i.e., hatred,
anger), emotional goals, and response tendencies as reactions to the scenario.
11
In a subsequent analysis of the data, I regressed each of two reactions (i.e., anger,
20
hatred) on the emotional goals and response tendencies while controlling for
sociopolitical variables (e.g., political position, educational level).
©
hatred. Hatred was the only emotion associated with the goals of exclusion
ig
(i.e., removing outgroup members from one’s life) and attack (i.e., hurting
yr
the outgroup members). However, anger was associated with the goal of cor-
op
13, and 14, for similar analyses of anger), hatred reflects avoidance of any deal-
ings with the outgroup based on having given up on the outgroup’s capacity
for change.
The findings regarding the response tendencies associated with hatred
and anger were complex. Naturally, participants who reported relatively high
A)
levels of hatred were more likely to support “denying Palestinians’ basic
political and social rights” as well as “physical and violent actions toward the
P
(A
Palestinians.” It is interesting that although anger was also associated with
support for “physical and violent actions toward the Palestinians,” it was
n
associated as well with “support for educational channels to create perceptual
io
change among Palestinians.” This latter response was obviously a more con-
at
structive approach to changing the Palestinians’ (perceived) unjust behavior.
ci
It seems, based on my research and previous studies by other researchers,
so
that people who feel angry have one key emotional goal: They wish to correct
As
and redirect behavior that they perceive to have been unfair and unjustified.
Researchers have documented that when people feel angry, they believe they
al
have (or at least deserve to have) high control over the situation, are more
ic
willing to take risks, and believe they have the ability to create beneficial
og
changes (Lerner, Gonzalez, Small, & Fischhoff, 2003; Lerner & Keltner, 2001;
ol
Mackie et al., 2000; see also Chapters 3, 4, and 12, this volume). These com-
ch
ponents of anger correspond with the first three psychological preconditions
y
Swartz, 1994).
ic
well as findings from a study by Fischer and Roseman (2007), indicate that
Am
tive response. As I explain in the next section, the most important factor
moderating the effect of anger on aggressive behavior is the level of long-term
11
2007; Lerner et al., 2003; Skitka et al., 2006). However, the findings I have
summarized here suggest that anger interacts with hatred to fuel aggression.
This conclusion is supported by recent studies of associations between inter-
group emotions and political intolerance. Political intolerance is usually defined
A)
as the willingness to deny the political rights of out-group members (Sullivan,
Piereson, & Marcus, 1979). This willingness is commonly viewed as a political
P
(A
version of aggressive intentions toward the outgroup, so it is important for
understanding the emotional underpinnings of intergroup aggression.
n
Two studies, one conducted in the post-9/11 period in the United States
io
(Skitka, Bauman, & Mullen, 2004) and the other in the midst of the second
at
Palestinian uprising in the Middle East (Halperin, Canetti-Nisim, & Hirsch-
ci
Hoefler, 2009), reveal similar patterns of political intolerance related to anger
so
and hatred. In a two-wave national field study (N = 550), Skitka et al. (2004)
As
examined the effects of anger and fear on political intolerance toward Arab
Americans, Muslims, and first-generation immigrants 4 months after the terror
al
attacks of 9/11. They found that anger had no direct influence on political
ic
intolerance and that most of the indirect effect of anger on intolerance was
og
mediated by moral outrage and outgroup derogation. Unfortunately, hatred
ol
of the outgroup was not measured as a separate variable in this study, but
ch
arguably, moral outrage and outgroup derogation might be proxies of hatred.
y
surveys varied in their design (two panel surveys and two cross sectional surveys),
ic
their context (some of the surveys were conducted during war and others
er
at times of relative calm in the conflict between Israelis and Arabs), and the
Am
the same: Anger toward the outgroup led to political intolerance only if it was
th
the interplay of anger and hatred, I examined survey-based data sets in which
20
explicit measures of support for military action were among the dependent
variables. The first analysis was based on a nationwide representative telephone
©
survey conducted among Israeli Jews in March of 2008. The sample consisted of
ht
781 Jewish-Israeli respondents (403 women, 378 men). The reported political
ig
society at the time: 37.3% were identified as rightists, 45.2% as centrists, and
op
17.6% as leftists.
Among other items, the questionnaire included one assessing long-term
C
hatred of Palestinians, two items (i.e., anger and rage) capturing anger about
Palestinian actions, and two items concerning support for aggressive actions
against Palestinians (i.e., “In a time of significant Palestinian threat, Israel should
use unconventional warfare”; “Only by using force can you achieve anything
in the Middle East”). For all of these items, participants used a 6-point response
A)
scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 6 (very much).
Support for aggressive action toward Palestinians was regressed on hatred,
P
(A
anger, and the interaction of the two while controlling for sociopolitical variables
(e.g., political position, educational level). No main effect of anger (β = −.10)
n
or hatred (β = −.17) was found, but their interaction was significant (β = .40,
io
p < .05). As shown in Figure 17.2, anger was associated with heightened
at
support for aggressive actions only in the presence of long-term hatred.
ci
Given the provocative nature of the findings, replication of the results
so
was called for. Therefore, I examined data from another nationwide survey
As
conducted in Israel during August and September of 2008. This sample
included 500 respondents, of whom 48.4% were men and 51.6% women.
al
The mean age was 45.5 years (SD = 16.49), and 41% of the respondents
ic
defined themselves as rightists, 28.9% as centrists, and 18.3% as leftists. Socio-
og
demographic measures and scales assessing anger and hatred were similar to
ol
the ones used in the previous survey. Support for military action was measured
ch
in a slightly different way, with two items: support for initiating a large military
y
operation of the Israeli army and support for using severe military action, even
Ps
5
ic
er
4.5
Am
4.17
4
e
Aggressive Action
th
3.5 3.27
3.24
by
Low Hatred
3 3.09 High Hatred
11
2.5
20
2
©
ht
1.5
ig
yr
1
op
Figure 17.2. Regression lines depicting the anger × hatred interaction affecting sup-
C
port for aggressive action toward Palestinians in the first nationwide survey (N = 781).
A)
for aggression, but their interaction was significant (β = .45, p < .01). Again, only
P
(A
the combination of anger and hatred was significantly associated with support
for aggressive action.
n
io
at
CONCLUSION
ci
so
Protracted intergroup conflicts inherently include mutual provocations,
As
violent responses, and diverse transgressions by both parties to the conflict.
As a result, members of societies involved in such conflicts experience frequent
al
episodes of anger toward the outgroup, its leaders, and its representatives.
ic
This anger, in various degrees, is an inevitable element of every intergroup
og
conflict. The research literature on emotions occurring in the context of conflict
ol
shows that anger evoked by an antagonist’s behavior is highly associated with
ch
support for retaliatory aggression (Huddy et al., 2007; Skitka et al., 2006).
y
In turn, widespread public support for aggression may legitimize and even
Ps
violence that causes a tragic loss of life on both sides of the conflict. However,
ic
the theoretical model and the bulk of empirical data presented in this chapter
er
suggest that the seemingly direct link between anger and the cycle of violence
Am
is not unavoidable.
The link between anger and support for aggression is dependent on the
e
level of hatred one feels for the outgroup. Anger in the absence of hatred, even
th
painful and unjust, can arouse a desire to correct the wrongdoing without
necessarily harming or destroying the outgroup (Fischer & Roseman, 2007).
11
of the outgroup.
ht
large-scale aggression if their goals are not shifted in that direction by a spike
op
justified.
The analysis of anger’s role that I have sketched here suggests a need for
studies that identify moderating factors that can reduce the destructive effects
of hatred and anger. Possibilities include other emotional sentiments such as
despair, empathy, or compassion, but also relevant are personality factors
A)
(e.g., authoritarianism, need for structure, attachment security), implicit theo-
ries about individuals and groups, adherence to moral values, socioeconomic
P
status, and political ideology.
(A
I hope I have made readers more aware of the role of intergroup emotions
n
as influences on public opinion, political decision making, and intergroup vio-
io
lence. More research is needed on ways to down-regulate “destructive emo-
at
tions” (Goleman, 2003; Halperin, Sharvit, & Gross, in press), up-regulate
ci
more prosocial emotions (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2009), and identify other
so
emotional sentiments and dispositions that might be used to foster more
As
favorable interactions with various emotions. In the long run, basic and
applied research along these lines may contribute to a reduction in retalia-
al
tory aggression.
ic
og
REFERENCES ol
ch
Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality (Vols. 1 & 2). New York, NY: Columbia
y
Ps
University Press.
Averill, J. R. (1994). In the eyes of the beholder. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.),
an
The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 7–15). New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
ic
er
Bar-Tal, D. (1998). Societal beliefs in times of intractable conflict: The Israeli case.
The International Journal of Conflict Management, 9, 22–50. doi:10.1108/eb022803
Am
Bar-Tal, D., & Halperin, E. (2009). Overcoming psychological barriers to peace mak-
ing: The influence of mediating beliefs about losses. In M. Mikulincer & P. R.
e
Shaver (Eds.), Prosocial motives, emotions, and behavior The better angels of our
th
Ben-Zeev, A. (1992). Anger and hate. Journal of Social Philosophy, 2, 85–110. doi:10.1111/
j.1467-9833.1992.tb00295.x
©
Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. Philadelphia, PA:
ht
Cacioppo, J. T., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 50,
yr
191–214. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.191
op
Cheung-Blunden, V., & Blunden, B. (2008). The emotional construal of war: Anger,
fear, and other negative emotions. Peace and Conflict, 14, 123–150. doi:10.1080/
C
10781910802017289
Fischer, A. H., & Roseman, I. J. (2007). Beat them or ban them: The characteristics
and social functions of anger and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 93, 103–115. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.103
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
A)
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal,
P
and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
(A
212–228. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212
Goleman, D. (2003). Destructive emotions: How can we overcome them? New York, NY:
n
Bantam Dell.
io
at
Gross, J. J. (Ed.). (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. New York, NY: Guilford
ci
Press.
so
Halperin, E. (2008a). Emotional barriers to peace: Negative emotions and public opinion
about the peace process in the Middle East. Paper presented at the 31st annual meet-
As
ing of the International Society of Political Psychology, Paris, France.
al
Halperin, E. (2008b). Group-based hatred in intractable conflict in Israel. The Journal
ic
of Conflict Resolution, 52, 713–736. doi:10.1177/0022002708314665
og
Halperin, E., Canetti-Nisim, D., & Hirsch-Hoefler, S. (2009). Emotional antecedents
ol
of political intolerance: The central role of group-based hatred. Political Psychology,
30, 93–123. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00682.x
ch
Halperin, E., Sharvit, K., Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotions and emotion regulation in
y
Ps
Huddy, L., Feldman, S., & Cassese, E. (2007). On the distinct political effects of
ic
(Eds.), The dynamics of emotion in political thinking and behavior (pp. 202–230).
Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.
Am
Iyer, A., & Leach, C. W. (2008). Emotion in inter-group relations. European Review
of Social Psychology, 19, 86–125. doi:10.1080/10463280802079738
e
th
Kelman, H. C., & Fisher, R. J. (2003). Conflict analysis and resolution. In D. O. Sears,
11
L. Huddy, & R. Jervis (Eds.), Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 315–353).
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
20
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
ht
Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects of fear and
anger on perceived risk of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychological
C
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-
specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 473–493.
doi:10.1080/026999300402763
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social
A)
Psychology, 81, 1 146-1159.
Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: Explaining
P
(A
offensive actions in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 79, 602–616. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.602
n
Maoz, I., & McCauley, C. (2008). Threat, dehumanization and support for retaliatory-
io
aggressive policies in asymmetric conflict. The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 52,
at
93–116. doi:10.1177/0022002707308597
ci
so
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (2009). Prosocial motives, emotions, and
behavior: The better angels of our nature. Washington, DC: American Psychological
As
Association.
Oatley, K., Keltner, D., & Jenkins, J. M. (2006). Understanding emotions (2nd ed.).
al
Maldin, MA: Blackwell.
ic
og
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotions: A structural theory. In
P. R. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 11–36).
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. ol
ch
Roseman, I. J. (2002). Dislike, anger, and contempt: Interpersonal distancing, attack,
y
Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and
an
Royzman, E. B., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (2005). From Plato to Putnam: Four
er
(Eds.), Feeling and emotions: The Amsterdam Symposium (pp. 136–157). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press.
©
Sharvit, K., Halperin, E., & Rosler, N. (2008). Forces of stability and change in prolonged
ht
occupation: Image threat, emotions, and justifying beliefs. Paper presented at the
ig
France.
op
Shaver, P. R., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge:
Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social
C
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W., Aramovich, N. P., & Morgan, G. C. (2006). Confronta-
tional and preventative policy responses to terrorism: Anger wants a fight and
fear wants “them” to go away. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 375–384.
doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2804_11
A)
Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W., & Mullen, E. (2004). Political tolerance and coming
to psychological closure following the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks: An
P
integrative approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 743–756.
(A
doi:10.1177/0146167204263968
n
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotions.
io
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.
at
48.4.813
ci
Smith, E. R. (1993). Social identity and social emotions: Toward a new conceptual-
so
ization of prejudice. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition
As
and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 297–315). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
al
Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2008). Intergroup emotions. In M. Lewis, J. M.
ic
Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 428–439).
og
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
ol
Smith, E. R., Seger, C. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2007). Can emotions be truly group level?
ch
Evidence for four conceptual criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
y
doi:10.1037/1089-2680.7.3.299
ic
Struch, N., & Schwartz, S. H. (1989). Intergroup aggression: Its predictors and
er
distinctness from in-group bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
Am
364–373. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.3.364
Sullivan, J. L., Piereson, J., & Marcus, G. E. (1979). An alternative conceptualization
e
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations
and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 375–424.
11
doi:10.1016/0162-3095(90)90017-Z
20
White, R. K. (1970). Nobody wanted war: Misperception in Vietnam and other wars.
Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.
©
Wohl, M. J. A., Branscombe, N. R., & Klar, Y. (2006). Collective guilt: Emotional
ht
reactions when one’s group has done wrong or been wronged. European Review
ig
Yzerbyt, V., Dumont, M., Wigboldus, D., & Gordin, E. (2003). I feel for us: The
op
014466603322595266
A)
Bennett & D. L. Paletz (Eds.), Taken by storm: The media, public opinion, and U.S.
foreign policy in the Gulf War (pp. 250–276). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
P
Press.
(A
n
io
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
11:58 AM
Am
er
ic
Page 332
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-19_CH18-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:59 AM Page 333
18
A)
P
TENSION AND HARMONY
(A
IN INTERGROUP RELATIONS
n
io
at
ci
TAMAR SAGUY, NICOLE TAUSCH, JOHN F. DOVIDIO,
so
FELICIA PRATTO, AND PURNIMA SINGH
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
group tension (see Chapters 16 and 17). This form of tension can result in
er
reduction (see Paluck & Green, 2009, for a review), and intergroup contact
(Pettigrew, 1998). The primary goal in these areas of research has typically
©
intergroup relations (Jackman, 1994), and not all forms of intergroup tension
yr
op
C
333
12051-19_CH18-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 11:59 AM Page 334
A)
systematic inequality (e.g., Jews and Arabs in Israel, Blacks and Whites in the
United States or South Africa, Muslims and Hindus in India, Muslims and
P
(A
non-Muslims in the United Kingdom). As exemplified by historical changes,
societal tension can be quite constructive in creating pressure for changing
n
unequal structures in the direction of equality. Social struggles such as those
io
initiated by the U.S. civil rights movement or by the African National Congress
at
in South Africa were aimed at disturbing apparent amity in order to “open
ci
the eyes of blind prejudice” (King, 1964, p. 35) and incite action for social
so
justice (see Sharp, 2005).
As
Because the disruption of harmony is often necessary for stimulating
change toward social equality, efforts to create harmony between groups may in
al
fact work to undermine these potential changes (see Wright & Lubensky, 2009).
ic
Thus, interventions to create societal harmony can potentially have the ironic
og
effect of sustaining negative patterns of intergroup relations, particularly
ol
those pertaining to group-based inequality. In the present chapter, we consider
ch
research on the effects of harmony-inducing strategies on psychological factors
y
that are unequal in the social system. Although it is well established that
intergroup contact relates to improved attitudes toward outgroup members
e
(Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006), little is known about the effect of contact and
th
focus on intergroup tension and discuss factors that can bring about change
ht
a strategy that aims to increase harmony, can work to undermine these factors.
yr
contact may reduce the potential for change. We discuss the implications of
these results for intergroup relations in general and for intergroup hostility
C
A)
considering antecedents of social change, we focus on psychological processes
that predict group members’ motivations to resist the status quo. We begin by
P
(A
explicating who the likely agents of change are in a hierarchical system and
then turn to consider the more specific psychological processes involved in
n
the development of social actions that can increase equality.
io
at
Who Are the Likely Agents of Change? The Psychologies
ci
of Advantaged and Disadvantaged Group Members
so
As
Without exception, societies are hierarchically organized such that at least
one group controls a greater share of valued resources (e.g., political power,
al
land, economic wealth, educational opportunities, access to health care) than
ic
do other groups (Jackman, 2001; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Group-based
og
hierarchy is reflected in almost every aspect of social life, from poverty rates
ol
and school attrition rates to prison sentences and mortality rates—favoring
ch
members of advantaged groups over those in disadvantaged groups (Feagin,
y
2006; Jackman, 2001; Smooha, 2005; Ulmer & Johnson, 2004). Moreover,
Ps
domains, such as interviewing for jobs and being quoted a price for a house or
ic
time climbing the social ladder, and experience legal authorities as a source
by
Thus, although the world may seem fair, hospitable, and inviting to members of
20
2009). These different realities and divergent perspectives form the basis for
ht
different motives and goals that advantaged and disadvantaged group members
ig
need to protect the dominance of one’s group, which in turn can account for
A)
advantaged groups are motivated to defend the existing social arrangements that
benefit them, members of disadvantaged groups are motivated to gain more
P
(A
resources, and thus to change the status quo so that their groups’ position in the
social hierarchy can improve. In line with these theories, research on social dom-
n
inance theory (Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth, & Malle, 1994; Sidanius & Pratto,
io
1999) demonstrates that individuals who hold more power in society tend to
at
view the social hierarchy as natural and even necessary, whereas members of dis-
ci
advantaged groups are more likely to see the hierarchy as in need of change.
so
Taken together, these views suggest that because members of advantaged
As
groups benefit both practically (Bobo & Hutchings, 1996) and psychologically
(Tajfel & Turner, 1979) from hierarchical social arrangements, they are not
al
likely to oppose the status quo. Undoubtedly, because they hold positions of
ic
power there is much value in the attempts to reduce potential discriminatory
og
behavior, which can occur in a variety of interpersonal contexts (e.g., in hiring
ol
decisions, when shopping in retail stores). Nevertheless, expecting advantaged
ch
group members to mobilize for structural-level change fails to consider the
y
they can benefit from changes toward greater equality, members of disadvantaged
groups are generally more likely to be motivated to challenge the status quo
an
and to raise related societal tension so that their group position can improve
ic
(Saguy, Dovidio, & Pratto, 2008; Scheepers, Spears, Doosje, & Manstead,
er
2006; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). Thus, for both practical and psychological rea-
Am
sons, collective action aimed at advancing more equality typically arises from
disadvantaged groups.
e
refers to more violent types of protest such as destroying property and, in extreme
20
Spears, 2008, for a meta-analysis). Whereas there are parallels between predic-
ig
(Lemieux, 2006; Moghaddam & Marsella, 2004). We next consider the con-
ditions that can increase or attenuate tendencies to partake in normative
C
A)
motivation is relatively subdued. A large body of research suggests that a key
(but not sole) determinant of collective action on the part of disadvantaged
P
(A
group members is their recognition that intergroup inequality exists and that
they are unjustly disadvantaged within the hierarchical system (see van Zomeren
n
et al., 2008, for a meta-analysis). This awareness of unjust inequality depends
io
on group members engaging in intergroup comparisons (Tajfel & Turner, 1979;
at
Wright & Lubensky, 2009); that is, they must perceive themselves as members
ci
of a group and compare their group’s standing with that of other groups. In
so
the context of social inequality, intergroup comparisons are likely to result in
As
a sense of relative disadvantage or unjust deprivation within the social system
(Walker & Smith, 2002).
al
What makes a sense of deprivation a powerful motivator for action is
ic
the emotions associated with it, such as anger and resentment (van Zomeren,
og
Spears, Fischer, & Leach, 2004). Beyond the recognition of deprivation that
ol
can incite these emotions, members of disadvantaged groups need to perceive
ch
an external source as responsible for their situation, typically the privileged
y
outgroup and/or the social system (Simon & Klandermans, 2001; Smith &
Ps
emotions such as anger can be directed toward it and can also arouse and guide
er
can also affect the way group members view their social standing. Research on
th
that reduce the emphasis on group boundaries and on unjust social structures
reduce disadvantaged group members’ perceptions of discrimination. For
11
example, when primed with beliefs about individual merit, women who were
20
despite their good performance were significantly less likely to act on behalf of
ig
their group when the illegitimacy of the decision was not emphasized (compared
yr
with when it was emphasized; Wright, 1997). Thus, although relative depriva-
op
tion is often an objective marker of any social hierarchy (Jackman, 1994, 2001),
factors that blur group boundaries can reduce the extent to which disadvantaged
C
group members are aware of the inequality and of their position in it.
A)
based hierarchy has been theorized to remain stable insofar as disadvantaged
group members deny their disadvantage or make internal attributions for their
P
(A
subordination (Jackman, 1994; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). As we argue next,
the emphasis on commonalities that is part of positive contact can reduce
n
group members’ awareness of group-based inequality. Because members of dis-
io
advantaged groups are likely agents of social change, their reduced awareness
at
can ultimately stabilize the existing social hierarchy.
ci
so
As
INTERGROUP CONTACT AND SOCIAL CHANGE
al
The fundamental premise of contact theory is that intergroup relations can
ic
improve by bringing members of opposing groups together under conditions
og
that involve institutional support, cooperation, equal status, and potential for
ol
personal acquaintance (Allport, 1954; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). The theory
ch
has stimulated a large body of research and has been applied in a wide variety
y
and Protestants in Northern Ireland (Hughes, 2001) and between Jews and
ic
policies, yet the majority of this work has focused on stated support for such
by
chief instigator of social change toward equality (Simon & Klandermans, 2001),
has rarely been examined.
©
between the groups (e.g., Gaertner, Mann, Murrell, & Dovidio, 1989) or as
op
salience of the psychological distinction between the ingroup and the outgroup
A)
goal is to redefine original group boundaries so that members of both groups are
perceived as part of a single more inclusive category (Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000).
P
(A
Either way, weakening the salience of original group boundaries is likely
to reduce the extent to which group members are focusing on group differences,
n
including those pertaining to differences in resources and power. Thus, the
io
focus on commonalities is likely to reduce awareness of structural inequality,
at
which is a crucial component in motivating disadvantaged group members to
ci
advance change toward equality. The fact that this reduced awareness is likely
so
to be coupled with positive attitudes toward the outgroup, makes the reduced
As
motivation for change even more likely, because disadvantaged group members
are less likely to attribute unfair or unjust acts to members of the outgroup
al
(see Smith & Walker, 2009). In the next section we present empirical evidence
ic
suggesting that intergroup contact can undermine the conditions necessary
og
for social change toward equality (see Wright & Lubensky, 2009, for a similar
argument). ol
y ch
Ps
RESEARCH SUPPORT
an
nature of intergroup contact can also affect the way group members view
er
social inequality. Past research has established that forms of contact that are
Am
boundaries, we propose that these forms of contact can also reduce awareness
by
optimism about the prospects of equality between the groups. Because recog-
nition of inequality and external attribution of disadvantage are necessary
©
A)
commonality-focused contact would reduce awareness of inequality in gen-
eral and of relative disadvantage more specifically. In the subsequent two field
P
(A
studies we examined whether a similar effect would occur among members of
naturally occurring disadvantaged groups: Arabs in Israel and Muslims in
n
India. In these field studies, we further tested whether perceptions of group-based
io
inequality predict reduced motivation for social change, our main outcome
at
of interest.
ci
In the laboratory study (Saguy, Tausch, Dovidio, & Pratto, 2009, Study 1),
so
power between two groups to which members were randomly assigned was
As
manipulated by giving the advantaged group the opportunity to assign extra
course credits to the two groups. Before the advantaged group members
al
allocated the credits, members of both groups interacted under the influence
ic
of instructions to focus on either intergroup commonalities or differences.
og
Consistent with prior research, commonality-focused interactions, compared
ol
with differences-focused interactions, produced more positive intergroup
ch
attitudes in both advantaged and disadvantaged group members. In addition,
y
compared with the advantaged group’s actual allocations, there was a sig-
by
motivated to maintain their power (Blumer, 1958; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999).
yr
act to create equality in practice. Thus, the results from the laboratory experi-
A)
the intergroup relations were situation-based and short-lived. They may
therefore not reflect processes that occur in more naturalistic intergroup
P
(A
contexts. For instance, members of disadvantaged groups might initially be
overly optimistic regarding outgroup fairness but not show the same effect
n
following repeated intergroup experiences. Our goal in the next set of studies
io
was to examine whether among members of real groups, intergroup contact
at
relates to reduced awareness of inequality and increased perceptions of out-
ci
group fairness. The naturalistic contexts also enabled us to explore whether these
so
outcomes were related to reduced support for social change among members
As
of disadvantaged groups.
Participants in the second study (Saguy et al., 2009, Study 2) were Arabs
al
in Israel, a national minority that suffers notable disadvantage compared with
ic
Jews (e.g., in academic achievement, income, political power; Smooha, 2005).
og
We examined the statistical associations among friendships with Jews (a type
ol
of positive contact that is particularly likely to involve a focus on common-
ch
alities; Aron et al., 2004), attitudes toward Jews, awareness of inequality, and
y
change toward equality (e.g., by asking them the degree to which they support
legislation guaranteeing equal work opportunities for Arabs). Drawing on col-
an
lective action research (Simon & Klandermans, 2001; van Zomeren et al., 2008)
ic
orientations (i.e., improved attitudes toward Jews and viewing Jews as fair)
Am
contact with Jews was associated with more positive attitudes toward Jews and
by
Moreover, and consistent with our theorizing, both perceptions of Jews as fair
20
members viewed the social inequality and members of the other group, contact
ht
was associated with a decrease in support for social change. The overall model
ig
testing the proposed links between variables fit the data well and better than
yr
Results of a third study (Tausch, Saguy, & Singh, 2009) replicated these
findings, this time among Muslims in India. Muslims in India are disadvantaged
C
compared with other minorities and, most notably, compared with Hindus, the
Outgroup
β = .31** attitude β = .13
β = .38**
b=.67** Support
β = .14† Outgroup β = –.24**
Positive contact for
A)
fairness
social change
b=.67** β = .11
P
β = 7**
β = –.18*
Inequality
(A
as just
n
io
Figure 18.1. Path model examining the mediated relationships among contact and
support for social change. The beta value between “positive contact” and “outgroup
at
fairness” = .14†; the beta value between “positive contact” and “inequality as
ci
just” = .22**.
so
*p < .05. **p < .01. †p < .09
As
dominant majority group. Having more Hindu friends was related to improved
al
attitudes toward Hindus but also to reduced awareness of inequality between
ic
Muslims and Hindus. In addition, these outcomes predicted stronger percep-
og
tions of Hindus as fair, which in turn were related to weaker collective action
ol
tendencies (measured as intentions to participate in various actions that could
ch
improve the position of Muslims in India).
y
Taken together, results from the field studies and the laboratory exper-
Ps
inequality and their own disadvantage. These perceptions, which are parts of
ic
challenge existing social inequality. These effects are consistent with our
Am
of Black and White respondents in South Africa, found that more positive
op
Africans who reported more positive contact with Whites were less support-
A)
These recent findings, coupled with our own studies reported here, suggest
that the harmony created by intergroup contact can undermine tension that
P
(A
might bring about change toward equality. This suggests that commonality-
focused contact can be used as a strategy, or a tool, for maintaining the hierarchy
n
that favors advantaged groups (see Jackman, 1994). If indeed harmony reduces
io
the potential for change, advantaged group members might use it to appease
at
members of disadvantaged groups. An additional implication of the current
ci
findings, which is highly relevant to the issue of intergroup aggression, is
so
that in the long term, the effect of commonality-focused contact might not
As
necessarily lead to a reduction in intergroup conflict, particularly if (or when)
disadvantaged group members are disillusioned and realize that their expec-
al
tations of fairness are not met. In the final two sections, we discuss these issues,
ic
among others, and offer potential solutions to some of the problems raised by
og
the reported findings.
ol
y ch
can foster social change that brings about greater equality and justice between
Am
inequality.
th
In general, our research supports this view by showing that the harmony
by
encouraged by contact may not translate into greater equality between groups.
The orientations of both disadvantaged and advantaged groups can contribute
11
inducing strategies can turn attention away from social inequities and cause
members of the advantaged group to be perceived as fair. Both of these outcomes
©
can lead disadvantaged group members to relax their efforts to promote change
ht
A)
system. Their knowledge that the disadvantaged group members are content
can reduce concerns about social inequality and make actual egalitarian
P
(A
behavior seem less necessary. A related possibility is that members of advantaged
groups may strategically try to create forms of positive social relations to sustain
n
the status quo (Jackman, 1994). Indeed, the threat of change in the system is
io
reduced if the likely agents of change (i.e., members of the disadvantaged
at
group) see less need for action. Therefore, from the point of view of those who
ci
wish to sustain the hierarchy, commonality-focused contact can be functional,
so
if somewhat deceptive.
As
Future research might also examine the effects of commonality-focused
contact over time, especially the effects on members of the disadvantaged group.
al
As we have demonstrated, disadvantaged group members may at first become
ic
optimistic, through positive contact, about intergroup relations and the
og
advantaged group’s benevolence. In the longer term, however, disadvantaged
ol
group members may become “disillusioned,” particularly if they are repeatedly
ch
appeased without achieving any real change. In addition, their situation might
y
actually get worse rather than improve if members of the advantaged group
Ps
ventions that would help achieve a more stable and genuine improvement in
Am
intergroup relations.
e
th
suggests that these benefits can turn out to be superficial and may even impede,
in the long run, constructive societal changes. Practical efforts to improve
©
about how to create harmony and eliminate tension to studying ways to cre-
ig
approach that views harmony and tension between groups as processes that
op
can coexist. Rather than overlooking the fact that groups operate in a system
of asymmetrical power, group-based differences, particularly those pertain-
C
can allow the pressure for social change to continue while providing an
avenue for communication and exchange (see Halabi, 2004). In addition, to
the extent that recognizing both commonality and group-based differences
and inequalities helps people to extend moral principles across group lines,
A)
advantaged group members may more readily recognize the illegitimacy of
group-based disparities and become motivated to respond fairly in a way that
P
(A
supersedes separate group interests.
n
io
REFERENCES
at
ci
so
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Aron, A., McLaughlin-Volpe, T., Mashek, D., Lewandowski, G., Wright, S. C., &
As
Aron, E. N. (2004). Including others in the self. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone
al
(Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 15, pp. 101–132). Hove, England:
ic
Psychology Press.
og
Ayres, I. (1991). Fair driving: Gender and race discrimination in retail car negotiations.
Harvard Law Review, 104, 817–872. doi:10.2307/1341506
ol
ch
Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than
Lakisha and Jamal? The American Economic Review, 94, 991–1013. doi:10.1257/
y
0002828042002561
Ps
Review, 1, 3–7.
ic
Bobo, L., & Hutchings, V. L. (1996). Perceptions of racial group competition: Extend-
er
toward the principle and practice of racial equality. Psychological Science, 18,
11
867–872. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01993.x
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Kawakami, K. (2003). The contact hypothesis: The
20
past, present, and the future. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 6, 5–21.
©
doi:10.1177/1368430203006001009
ht
Durrheim, K., & Dixon, J. A. (2004). Attitudes in the fiber of everyday life:
Desegregation and the discourse of racial evaluation. American Psychologist, 59,
ig
626–636. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.626
yr
op
Feagin, J. R. (2006). Systematic racism: A theory of oppression. New York, NY: Routledge.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup
C
Gaertner, S. L., Mann, J., Murrell, A., & Dovidio, J. F. (1989). Reducing intergroup bias:
The benefits of recategorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
239–249. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.239
Halabi, R. (Ed). (2004). Israeli and Palestinian identities in dialogue: The school for peace
A)
approach. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Hewstone, M., & Cairns, E. (2001). Social psychology and intergroup conflict. In
P
(A
D. Chirot & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Ethnopolitical warfare: Causes, consequences,
and possible solutions (pp. 319–342). Washington, DC: American Psychological
n
Association. doi:10.1037/10396-020
io
Hughes, J. (2001). Constitutional reform in Northern Ireland: Implications for
at
community relations policy and practice. The International Journal of Conflict
ci
Management, 12, 154–173. doi:10.1108/eb022854
so
Jackman, M. R. (1994). The velvet glove. Berkeley, CA: University of California
As
Press.
Jackman, M. R. (2001). License to kill: Violence and legitimacy in expropriative social
al
relations. In J. T. Jost & B. Major (Eds.), The psychology of legitimacy: Emerging
ic
perspectives on ideology, justice, and intergroup relations (pp. 437–467). New York,
og
NY: Cambridge University Press.
ol
Jones, J. M., Engelman, S., Turner, C., & Campbell, S. (2009). Worlds apart: The
ch
universality of racism leads to divergent social realities. In S. Demoulin, J. P. Leyens,
y
encounter interventions between Jews and Arabs in Israel. Journal of Social Issues,
by
Zanna (Eds), Relative deprivation and social comparison: The Ontario Symposium
20
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.04.009
ig
Miller, N. (2002). Personalization and the promise of contact theory. Journal of Social
yr
ciation. doi:10.1037/10621-000
Paluck, E. L., & Green, D. P. (2009). Prejudice reduction: What works? A critical look
at evidence from the field and the laboratory. Annual Review of Psychology, 60,
339–367. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163607
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49,
A)
65–85. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.65
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact
P
(A
theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751–783. doi:10.1037/
0022-3514.90.5.751
n
Pettigrew, T. F., Wagner, U., & Christ, O. (2007). Who opposes immigration?
io
Comparing German with North American findings. Du Bois Review, 4, 19–39.
at
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance
ci
orientation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes.
so
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741–763. doi:10.1037/0022-
As
3514.67.4.741
Saguy, T., Dovidio, J. F., & Pratto, F. (2008). Beyond contact: Intergroup contact in the
al
context of power relations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 432–445.
ic
doi:10.1177/0146167207311200
og
Saguy, T., Tausch, N., Dovidio, J. F., & Pratto, F. (2009). The irony of harmony:
ol
Intergroup contact can produce false expectations for equality. Psychological
ch
Science, 20, 114–121. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02261.x
y
Scheepers, D., Spears, R., Doosje, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2006). Diversity in
Ps
in-group bias: Structural factors, situational features, and social functions. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 944–960. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.6.944
an
Sharp, G. (2005). Waging nonviolent struggle: 20th century practice and 21st century
ic
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy
Am
56.4.319
by
Smith, H. J., & Walker, I. (2009). Feeling relative deprivation: The rocky road
from comparisons to actions. In U. Wagner, L. R., Tropp, G. Finchilescu, &
11
Smooha, S. (2005). Index of Arab–Jewish relations in Israel 2004. Haifa, Israel: The
ht
A)
sociopsychological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 504–535. doi:10.1037/
0033-2909.134.4.504
P
(A
van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., Fischer, A. H., & Leach, C. W. (2004). Put your money
where your mouth is! Explaining collective action tendencies through group-based
n
anger and group efficacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 649–664.
io
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.649
at
Varshney, A. (2002). Ethnic conflict and civic life: Hindus and Muslims in India. New
ci
Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
so
Walker, I., & Smith, H. J. (2002). Relative deprivation: Specification, development, and
As
integration. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Wright, S. C. (1997). Ambiguity, social influence, and collective action: Generating
al
collective protest in response to tokenism. Personality and Social Psychology
ic
Bulletin, 23, 1277–1290. doi:10.1177/01461672972312005
og
Wright, S. C., & Lubensky, M. (2009). The struggle for social equality: Collective
ol
action versus prejudice reduction. In S. Demoulin, J. P. Leyens, & J. F. Dovidio
ch
(Eds.), Intergroup misunderstandings: Impact of divergent social realities (pp. 291–310).
y
A)
P
(A
CONSEQUENCES
n
io
OF AGGRESSION:
at
ci
THE VICTIM PERSPECTIVE
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
20
12051-20_PT5-CH19-rev2.qxd
11
by
5/25/10
th
e
Am
12:00 PM
er
ic
an
Page 350
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-20_PT5-CH19-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 12:00 PM Page 351
19
A)
P
INFLUENCE OF VIOLENCE AND
(A
AGGRESSION ON CHILDREN’S
n
io
PSYCHOLOGICAL DEVELOPMENT:
at
ci
TRAUMA, ATTACHMENT,
so
AND MEMORY
As
al
SHEREE L. TOTH, LATONYA S. HARRIS, GAIL S. GOODMAN,
ic
AND DANTE CICCHETTI
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ory, a topic that has recently received attention from researchers. Scores of
th
ened risk of poor mental health outcomes. Only more recently have the
20
memory is important for clinical, legal, and societal interventions and for
ht
This material is based in part on work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grants
No. 0545413 and 0851420 to Dr. Gail S. Goodman.
C
Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of
the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.
351
12051-20_PT5-CH19-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 12:00 PM Page 352
A)
In this chapter, we consider how violence can affect children’s socio-
emotional and cognitive development, including their memories. Rather than
P
(A
providing a comprehensive review, we direct our attention to investigations
guided by a developmental psychopathology perspective. In so doing, we pro-
n
vide an overview of the sequelae of child maltreatment in relation to emotion
io
regulation, attachment, and psychopathology. We describe how these psycho-
at
social phenomena provide important perspectives from which to understand
ci
so
maltreated individuals’ memories for emotional or traumatic events, an issue
of special importance to both the field of developmental psychology and the
As
legal system.
al
ic
og
PATHOGENIC RELATIONAL ENVIRONMENTS
AND CHILD MALTREATMENT ol
ch
Child maltreatment, in the form of aggression and violence against chil-
y
Ps
however, that maltreatment does not automatically doom all children to neg-
ative outcomes. In fact, some children are surprisingly resilient even under
e
th
with a person who believes in the child and offers support when needed
(Werner, 1993). This places attachment theory at the heart of theorizing about
20
A)
tors by which emotional arousal is redirected, controlled, modulated, and mod-
ified so that an individual can function adaptively in emotionally challenging
P
(A
situations (Cicchetti, Ganiban, & Barnett, 1991). The ability to regulate emo-
tions develops within the context of early parent–child interactions and rela-
n
tionships (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Thompson, 1999, 1994).
io
Child maltreatment poses a significant threat to the optimal develop-
at
ment of affect regulation. Although there are important individual differences,
ci
maltreated children as a group exhibit numerous deficits in the recognition,
so
expression, and understanding of emotions. Even in the early months of life,
As
distortions in affect differentiation can occur in children with a history of mal-
treatment (Gaensbauer & Hiatt, 1984; Gaensbauer, Mrazek, & Harmon,
al
1981). Specifically, either excessive amounts of negative affect or blunted
ic
affect may be observed in such cases.
og
Early problems in emotion regulation can lay a foundation for future dif-
ol
ficulties in modulating affect. For example, in one study, physically abused
ch
preschool boys who witnessed an angry simulated interaction involving their
y
Physically abused boys also reported greater fear in response to angry interac-
Am
tions between adults, especially when the conflicts were not resolved (Hen-
nessy, Rabideau, Cicchetti, & Cummings, 1994). Finally, in an investigation
e
(Pollak, Cicchetti, Hornung, & Reed, 2000). Pollak and Sinha (2002) reported
ig
that physically abused children required less sensory input than comparison
yr
children to accurately identify facial displays of anger (see also Pollak & Kistler,
op
(2003) discovered that such children notice such expressions readily and find
it difficult to disengage attention from them. These results suggest that early
adverse experiences influence maltreated children’s selective attention to
threat-related emotional signals.
Deviations in understanding negative affect and in processing negative
A)
affective signals are also evident in children who have experienced forms of
abuse other than physical, including sexual abuse, emotional maltreatment,
P
(A
and neglect. Such difficulties are associated with undercontrolled and aggres-
sive behavior in school settings (Rogosch, Cicchetti, & Aber, 1995). In an
n
investigation of attributional processes, Toth, Cicchetti, and Kim (2002)
io
found that children’s attributions for positive and negative events moderated
at
externalizing behavior problems (e.g., aggression) and that children’s percep-
ci
tions of their mothers mediated both internalizing and externalizing symp-
so
toms. Maltreated children had higher levels of internalizing and externalizing
As
behavior problems than did nonmaltreated children. However, maltreated
children with higher positive attribution scores had lower levels of external-
al
izing symptomatology, and those with lower levels of negative attribution
ic
scores had lower levels of externalizing behavior problems. These findings
og
suggest that children’s positive attributional styles exert a protective force
ol
against the damaging effects of child maltreatment. Negative perceptions of
ch
mothers served as an independent risk factor for children’s internalizing and
y
leagues (Dodge, Pettit, Bates, & Valente, 1995; see also Chapter 9, this vol-
Am
ume) reported that children who were physically abused during the first 5 years
of life were later defensively hypervigilant to hostile cues and failed to attend
e
ity to others in situations where most people would not make such attributions.
by
likely.
A number of cross-sectional investigations have examined associations
©
sive actions. Maltreated toddlers tend to react to peer distress with poorly reg-
op
ulated and situationally inappropriate affect and behavior, including anger, fear,
and aggression, as opposed to the more normatively expected responses of
C
empathy and concern (Klimes-Dougan & Kistner, 1990). Shields and Cicchetti
(1998) found that maltreated children were more likely than nonmaltreated
children to be aggressive, with physically abused children at heightened risk of
reactive aggression (i.e., reacting to perceived provocations). Maltreated chil-
dren also evidenced attention deficits, and subclinical or nonpathological dis-
A)
sociation was more likely among children with histories of physical or sexual
abuse. A history of abuse also predicted emotion dysregulation, affective labil-
P
(A
ity and negativity, and socially inappropriate emotional expressions. In that
study, emotion dysregulation was the mechanism whereby maltreatment
n
resulted in reactive aggression (see Chapters 2, 6, and 7, this volume).
io
Overall, the effects of maltreatment on emotion regulation are profound,
at
and they have important effects on socioemotional development including
ci
aggressive behavior. As we describe later, the resulting emotion-regulation dif-
so
ficulties may also have important effects on memories of trauma, which are
As
important clinically and forensically.
al
Attachment and Child Maltreatment
ic
og
Establishing a secure attachment relationship between an infant and his
ol
or her primary caregiver is an important developmental task during the first
ch
year of life (Sroufe, 1979; Thompson, 1999). Attachment theorists have
y
vides a secure base from which to explore the environment and, ultimately,
contributes to the integration of cognitive, affective, and behavioral capacities
an
(Bowlby, 1973, 1980, 1982; Chapters 13, 14, and 18, this volume). Not
er
rity reaching as high as 95% (Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2006; Crittenden,
th
1988; Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Zoll, & Stahl, 1987). Furthermore, in both cross-
by
sectional and longitudinal studies, maltreated infants and toddlers have ele-
vated rates of disorganized/disoriented (Type D) attachment, an atypical form of
11
ment continues to be important across the life course, because internal work-
op
ing models (i.e., mental representations) of self, others, and close relationships
are carried forward into later stages of development (Chapters 4, 13, and 14).
C
For example, children maltreated in infancy and toddlerhood are more likely
A)
There is evidence (summarized, e.g., in the comprehensive volume edited
by Grossmann, Grossmann, & Waters, 2005) that child–parent attachment lays
P
(A
a foundation for later “coherence of mind” with respect to attachment (Hesse,
2008) as well as emotion-regulation strategies relevant to attachment issues
n
(Chapters 13 and 14, this volume). As such, children’s attachment security is
io
likely to affect memory and other cognitive processes in ways that are impor-
at
tant to close relationships, clinical assessment and intervention, and forensic
ci
interviews.
so
As
Psychopathology and Child Maltreatment
al
Although not all maltreated children will have serious mental health
ic
problems, disruptions in psychological development during the early years of
og
life can certainly contribute to the emergence of psychopathology later on. In
ol
general, the literature on maltreatment reveals a greater preponderance of psy-
ch
chiatric symptoms and diagnoses in maltreated than in nonmaltreated chil-
y
dren, and the range of related disorders is broad. Physical abuse and neglect are
Ps
Toth & Cicchetti, 1996; Toth, Manly, & Cicchetti, 1992). In addition, mal-
ic
(Widom, DuMont, & Czaja, 2007). Moreover, the risk of adult depression
Am
increases even further for adults who were maltreated in the first 5 years of life
(Kaplow & Widom, 2007). The same is true for conduct disorder and delin-
e
quency (Smith & Thornberry, 1995). Higher rates of attention deficit hyper-
th
iety, and dissociation (Johnson, Cohen, Brown, Smailes, & Bernstein, 1999;
Luntz & Widom, 1994; Macfie, Cicchetti, & Toth, 2001a, 2001b; Malinosky-
©
Rummell & Hansen, 1993; McLeer, Callaghan, Henry, & Wallen, 1994; Yates,
ht
trauma (Rubin, Berntsen, & Bohni, 2008). These memory issues are addressed
in the following section.
A)
Increasing evidence suggests that being maltreated in childhood not
P
(A
only affects emotion regulation, attachment, and mental health but also
affects cognitive functioning (Harris et al., 2008), in particular, memory. In
n
fact, emotion regulation, attachment, psychopathology, and maltreatment all
io
come together to influence memory processes.
at
Research reviewed earlier in this chapter indicates that victims of mal-
ci
treatment often overattend to trauma-related information, as in the case,
so
mentioned earlier, of children with histories of physical abuse having diffi-
As
culty disengaging attention from anger cues while experiencing heightened
arousal and emotional dysregulation. One consequence is that many maltreat-
al
ment victims, especially if they suffer from the trauma-related psychopathol-
ic
ogy of PTSD, may have unusually accurate memories of childhood traumas
og
(Alexander et al., 2005), because they focused great attention on the events
ol
in question and have remembered them in detail ever since. In other words,
ch
the oversensitization to trauma cues, mentioned earlier, may make trauma-
y
differences in the ways in which aggression and violence toward children may
Am
Faunce, Riddlesberger, & Kuhn, 1996). According to Bowlby (1980; see also
th
Main, 1990), insecure–avoidant children, whose bids for care have been repeat-
by
the processing of stressful information, with the goal of preventing the intense
20
to recall their abusive experiences. Goodman et al. (1992) first examined the
op
1992). About 13 years after their court cases, the victims were interviewed
about their abusive experiences (Edelstein et al., 2005; Goodman et al., 2003).
The results indicated that victims who scored higher on a measure of avoidant
attachment had poorer (i.e., less detailed and less accurate) memories the more
severe their assaults had been, a pattern apparently reflecting defensive exclu-
A)
sion. In sharp contrast, victims with lower scores on avoidant attachment were
actually more accurate about their more severe abuse experiences.
P
(A
Coping strategies, which are likely to be affected by child maltreatment,
may explain, at least in part, the connection between avoidant attachment,
n
avoidant coping (sometimes called distancing coping), and memory. In a recent
io
study of autobiographical memory, adults and adolescents with or without
at
childhood sexual abuse histories were asked to recount childhood events
ci
(Harris et al., 2008). Their accounts were scored for the degree of specificity of
so
the memories (e.g., specificity as to time and place of the incidents described;
As
see Williams, 1996). In line with Bowlby’s (1980) conception of defensive
avoidance, Harris et al. (2008) predicted that survivors of childhood sexual
al
abuse would adopt a nonspecific memory retrieval style as a way of avoiding
ic
unpleasant and intrusive specific memories (e.g., Hermans, Defranc, Raes,
og
Williams, & Eelen, 2005). Harris et al. found that memory nonspecificity was
ol
associated with distancing coping processes (i.e., cognitive efforts to detach
ch
oneself from and minimize the significance of the situation, as measured by
the Ways of Coping Questionnaire; Folkman & Lazarus, 1988), which was
y
Ps
expected based on attachment theory and research (Hazan & Shaver, 1987;
Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). The relation between distancing coping and
an
to trauma cues, memory for trauma may be robust and accurate. In other cases,
such as with children and adults with an avoidant attachment style and who
11
may be suppressed or weakened, and for that reason may also be less accurate.
©
ht
METHODOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS
ig
yr
design and conduct of this research. We are especially interested here in how
C
A)
ulated approaches to defining maltreatment (Barnett, Manly, & Cicchetti,
1993; National Research Council, 1993) and by a long history of disagreement
P
among researchers, lawmakers, and clinicians (Aber & Zigler, 1981; Barnett
(A
et al., 1993). Many researchers have maintained that the definition of mal-
n
treatment should focus on specific acts that endanger a child (Barnett et al.,
io
1993; Cicchetti & Barnett, 1991b; Zuravin, 1991). However, because mal-
at
treatment is largely determined by legal considerations, it is often identified by
ci
social service workers. Yet there is still not a broad consensus among them
so
regarding acceptable disciplinary practices (e.g., spanking) and what counts as
As
child maltreatment (Barnett, Manly, & Cicchetti, 1993; Black & Dubowitz,
1999). There is also a lack of agreement on whether child maltreatment should
al
be defined based on the actions of the perpetrator, the effects on the child, or
ic
a combination of the two (Barnett et al., 1993).
og
Operationalizations of the maltreatment variable have ranged from inves-
ol
tigator observations of “poor parenting” through reliance on parent and/or child
ch
reports, to utilization of Child Protective Services records. These issues are not
easily resolved, but they need to be considered when evaluating studies of the
y
Ps
Internal Validity
ic
er
CONCLUSION
©
ht
outcomes are affected when children experience aggression and violence, par-
yr
current and later mental health. It is now well documented that violence
affects children’s emotional memories as well and their feelings and behavior.
Further research should examine the extent to which violence adversely
affects other cognitive domains such as executive functions, problem solving,
A)
planning, and learning. Although research in the area of child maltreatment
is not without major challenges and complexities, the potential theoretical,
P
clinical, and social policy implications of such research provide compelling
(A
reasons to increase its sophistication and applicability in the future.
n
io
at
REFERENCES
ci
so
Aber, J. L., & Zigler, E. (1981). Developmental considerations in the definition of
As
child maltreatment. New Directions for Child Development, 1981(11), 1–29.
doi:10.1002/cd.23219811103
al
Alexander, K. W., Quas, J. A., & Goodman, G. S. (2002). Theoretical advances in
ic
understanding children’s memory for distressing events: The role of attachment.
og
Developmental Review, 22, 490–519. doi:10.1016/S0273-2297(02)00004-7
Alexander, K. W., Quas, J. A., Goodman, G. S., Ghetti, S., Edelstein, R. S., Redlich,
ol
A. D., . . . Jones, D. P. H. (2005). Traumatic impact predicts long-term memory
ch
for documented child sexual abuse. Psychological Science, 16, 33–40. doi:10.1111/
y
j.0956-7976.2005.00777.x
Ps
Barnett, D., Ganiban, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Maltreatment, negative expressiv-
an
doi:10.1111/1540-5834.00035
er
Barnett, D., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Defining child maltreatment: The
Am
interface between policy and research. In D. Cicchetti & S. L. Toth (Eds.), Child
abuse, child development, and social policy (pp. 7–74). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
e
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depression. New York, NY:
Basic Books.
©
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
ht
Basic Books.
ig
Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K. (1989). Disorganized/
yr
A)
of child maltreatment. In W. Grove & D. Cicchetti (Eds.), Thinking clearly about
psychology: Essays in honor of Paul E. Meehl: Vol. 2. Personality and psychopathology
P
(pp. 346–377). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
(A
Cicchetti, D., Ganiban, J., & Barnett, D. (1991). Contributions from the study of
n
high-risk populations to understanding the development of emotion regulation.
io
In J. Garber & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and
at
dysregulation (pp. 15–48). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
ci
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001). The impact of child maltreatment and psycho-
so
pathology upon neuroendocrine functioning. Development and Psychopathology,
As
13, 783–804.
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Fostering secure attachment in
al
infants in maltreating families through preventive interventions. Development
ic
and Psychopathology, 18, 623–649. doi:10.1017/S0954579406060329
og
Crittenden, P. M. (1988). Relationships at risk. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.),
ol
The clinical implications of attachment (pp. 136–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ch
Cummings, E. M., Hennessy, K. D., Rabideau, G. J., & Cicchetti, D. (1994).
y
processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later
ic
0021-843X.104.4.632
Am
Edelstein, R. S., Ghetti, S., Quas, J. A., Goodman, G. S., Alexander, K. W., Redlich,
A. D., & Cordón, I. M. (2005). Individual differences in emotional memory:
Adult attachment and long-term memory for child sexual abuse. Personality and
e
th
treated children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi-
11
Gaensbauer, T. J., Mrazek, D., & Harmon, R. (1981). Emotional expression in abused
yr
Goodman, G. S., Ghetti, S., Quas, J. A., Edelstein, R. S., Alexander, K. W., Redlich,
C
A)
ences of age, anatomical dolls, knowledge, and parental attachment. Applied
Developmental Science, 1, 54–75. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads0102_1
P
(A
Goodman, G. S., Taub, E. P., Jones, D., England, P., Port, L., Rudy, L., & Prado, L.
(1992). Testifying in court. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Devel-
n
opment, 57 (Serial no. 229).
io
Grossmann, K. E., Grossmann, K., & Waters, E. (Eds.). (2005). Attachment from
at
infancy to adulthood: The major longitudinal studies. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
ci
so
Harris, L. S., Block, S. D., Augusti, E. M., Larson, R. P., Culver, M. D., Pineda,
R. R., . . . Goodman, G. S. (2008, May). The relation between coping style and
As
autobiographical memory specificity in maltreated and nonmaltreated adolescents and
adults. Poster session presented at the meeting of the annual convention of the
al
Association for Psychological Science, Chicago, IL.
ic
og
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment
process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511–524. doi:10.1037/
0022-3514.52.3.511 ol
ch
Hennessy, K. D., Rabideau, G. J., Cicchetti, D., & Cummings, E. M. (1994).
y
Hermans, D., Defranc, A., Raes, F., Williams, J. M. G., & Eelen, P. (2005). Reduced
autobiographical memory specificity as an avoidant coping style. The British Jour-
ic
Guilford Press.
th
Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Brown, J., Smailes, E. M., & Bernstein, D. P. (1999). Child-
by
hood maltreatment increases risk for personality disorders during early adulthood.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 600–606. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.7.600
11
Kaplow, J. B., & Widom, C. S. (2007). Age of onset of child maltreatment predicts
20
00004583-199103000-00014
yr
1649.26.4.599
Luntz, B. K., & Widom, C. S. (1994). Antisocial personality disorder in abused and
neglected children grown up. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 670–674.
Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Patterns of relatedness in maltreated and non-
maltreated children: Connections among multiple representational models.
A)
Development and Psychopathology, 3, 207–226. doi:10.1017/S0954579400000080
P
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk:
(A
Relationships among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant
attachment behavior. Developmental Psychology, 23, 223–232. doi:10.1037/
n
0012-1649.23.2.223
io
Macfie, J., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2001a). The development of dissociation
at
in maltreated preschool-aged children. Development and Psychopathology, 13,
ci
233–254. doi:10.1017/S0954579401002036
so
Macfie, J., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2001b). Dissociation in maltreated versus
As
nonmaltreated preschool-aged children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25, 1253–1267.
doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(01)00266-6
al
Main, M. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of attachment organization: Recent studies,
ic
og
changing methodologies, and the concept of conditional strategies. Human
Development, 33, 48–61. doi:10.1159/000276502
ol
Malinosky-Rummell, R., & Hansen, D. (1993). Long-term consequences of child-
ch
hood physical abuse. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 68–79. doi:10.1037/0033-
y
2909.114.1.68
Ps
Manly, J. T., Kim, J. E., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Dimensions of child
an
Maughan, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). The impact of child maltreatment and
er
sexually abused children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
th
National Research Council. (1993). Understanding child abuse and neglect. Washington,
20
Pollak, S. D., & Kistler, D. (2002). Early experience alters the development of categor-
yr
pnas.142165999
Pollak, S. D., & Sinha, P. (2002). Enhanced perceptual sensitivity for anger among
physically abused children. Developmental Psychology, 38, 784–791. doi:10.1037/
0012-1649.38.5.784
Pollak, S. D., & Tolley-Schell, S. A. (2003). Selective attention to facial emotion
in physically abused children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 323–338.
A)
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.323
P
Rogosch, F. A., Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1995). The role of child maltreatment in
(A
early deviations in cognitive and affective processing abilities and later peer rela-
n
tionship problems. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 591–609. doi:10.1017/
io
S0954579400006738
at
Rubin, D., Berntsen, D., & Bohni, M. (2008). A memory based model of posttraumatic
ci
stress disorder. Psychological Review, 115, 985–1011. doi:10.1037/a0013730
so
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated children:
As
The contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Clinical
Child Psychology, 27, 381–395. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2704_2
al
Smith, C. A., & Thornberry, T. P. (1995). The relationship between childhood
ic
maltreatment and adolescent involvement in delinquency. Criminology, 33,
og
451–481. doi:10.1111/j.1745-9125.1995.tb01186.x
ol
Sroufe, L. A. (1979). The coherence of individual development: Early care, attach-
ch
ment, and subsequent developmental issues. American Psychologist, 34, 834–841.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.834
y
Ps
1166137
ic
Toth, S. L., & Cicchetti, D. (1996). Patterns of relatedness and depressive symp-
tomatology in maltreated children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
e
Toth, S. L., Cicchetti, D., & Kim, J. E. (2002). Relations among children’s percep-
by
doi:10.1023/A:1019868914685
20
Toth, S. L., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1992). Child maltreatment and vulnera-
bility to depression. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 97–112. doi:10.1017/
©
S0954579400005587
ht
Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, and recovery: Perspectives from the Kauai
ig
S095457940000612X
op
Widom, C. S., DuMont, K., & Czaja, S. J. (2007). A prospective investigation of major
depressive disorder and comorbidity in abused and neglected children grown up.
C
A)
Yates, T. M., Carlson, E. A., & Egeland, B. R. (2008). A prospective study of child
maltreatment and self-injurious behavior in a community sample. Development
P
and Psychopathology, 20, 651–671. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000321
(A
Zigler, E. (1998). A place of value for applied and policy studies. Child Development,
n
69, 532–542.
io
Zuravin, S. J. (1991). Research definitions of child physical abuse and neglect: Cur-
at
rent problems. In R. H. Starr & D. A. Wolfe (Eds.), The effects of child abuse and
ci
neglect (pp. 100–128). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
11
by
5/25/10
th
e
Am
12:00 PM
er
ic
an
Page 366
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-21_CH20-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 12:00 PM Page 367
20
PA)
THE PARADOX OF PARTNER
(A
AGGRESSION: BEING COMMITTED
n
io
TO AN AGGRESSIVE PARTNER
at
ci
so
XIMENA B. ARRIAGA AND NICOLE M. CAPEZZA
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
for example, approximately, 1.3 million women and 835,000 men are physi-
er
acts (about 75%) being mild forms of aggression, such as pushing or slapping
(Tjaden & Theonnes, 2000). Each year, about 5% of physically abused
e
women (approximately 44,000) suffer serious injuries from being beaten up,
th
choked, or assaulted with a weapon. (Such severe acts are significantly more
by
likely to be perpetrated by men against women than the reverse; Rennison &
Welchans, 2000; see also Chapters 2, 13, and 14, this volume.)
11
expectations for a close relationship. Targets of partner aggression who are com-
ht
mitted to their relationship are left to make sense of a paradox: The presumed
ig
source of love and intimacy is also the source of pain. This paradox often causes
yr
outsiders to wonder why the victims of partner aggression remain with their
op
aggressive partner.
In this chapter, we suggest that perceptions or interpretations of the part-
C
ner’s acts play a central role in continuing the relationship, and we consider
367
12051-21_CH20-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 12:00 PM Page 368
the possibility that individuals perceive aggressive partner acts as less severe to
the extent that they are committed. Victims who downplay their partner’s
aggression fail to protect themselves and thus run the risk of being hurt further.
Moreover, when members of a society fail to recognize that partner aggression
A)
has negative consequences and should not be tolerated, partner aggression
becomes more likely to continue as perpetrators avoid punishment and targets
P
(A
fail to get needed protection and support.
We begin by defining partner aggression and delineating the different
n
forms it takes. We then review our own and other research on what is known
io
about perceptions of partner aggression in general, and about how victims’ per-
at
ceptions compare with others’ perceptions. We then shift the focus from per-
ci
ceptions of partner aggression in general to how victims perceive their own
so
partner’s acts. We use concepts from interdependence and consistency theories
As
to suggest that the closer and more committed the victim feels to the perpetra-
tor, the more likely she is to downplay his aggressive acts. We end by discussing
al
how a victim’s well-being is affected by continuing a relationship in which she
ic
downplays her partner’s aggressive acts.
og
ol
ch
WHAT DO WE MEAN BY PARTNER AGGRESSION?
y
Ps
acts, such as hitting, punching, and kicking. They also include psychologically
ic
aggression. For instance, over 70% of physically abused women report that
20
emotional abuse had a more damaging effect on their self-esteem and health
than physical aggression (Follingstad, Rutledge, Berg, Hause, & Polek, 1990).
©
mental health (e.g., Arias & Pape, 1999), but it has also been linked to a num-
ig
are just as likely as victims of physical aggression to suffer from chronic neck
op
Bethea, King, & McKeown, 2000). Thus, psychological aggression has clear
mental and physical health outcomes for victims.
Yet, as we describe later in this chapter, psychological aggression is not
perceived to be as serious as physical aggression, despite the growing literature
A)
on the severe consequences of psychological aggression suggesting otherwise.
This may make victims themselves and people in general less likely to counter
P
(A
partner psychological aggression than physical aggression.
Much of our analysis focuses on commitment, a strong subjective force
n
that keeps people in relationships (Rusbult, 1983). A victim’s feeling tied to
io
a partner, imagining a long-term future with the partner, and intending to
at
remain in the relationship—all of which are aspects of commitment (Arriaga
ci
& Agnew, 2001)—create a subjective state that motivates more benign inter-
so
pretations of negative partner acts (Rusbult, Olsen, Davis, & Hannon, 2001).
As
To date, we have examined commitment and type of aggression (i.e., physi-
cal vs. psychological) as important factors predicting perceptions of partner
al
aggression, but there are likely many other personality, relational, and social
ic
circumstances that affect perceptions. We begin our analysis by establishing
og
how partner aggression is generally perceived.
ol
y ch
in that perceptions are more negative today than they used to be. A second
ic
considered.
Am
may have been tolerated 40 years ago, but they are now generally considered
unacceptable (Gelles, 1993). Representative U.S. samples have shown that
11
domestic violence and are thus unlawful. For instance, 98.8% of respondents
in one study (Carlson & Worden, 2005) reported that a husband punching his
©
Although these U.S. norms against physical abuse have become stable
yr
A)
logically aggressive behaviors (e.g., belittling, humiliating, threatening).
On one hand, this may not seem surprising given that physical aggression
P
(A
can leave visible injuries whereas psychological aggression leaves no physical
marks. But on the other hand, U.S. society’s failure to condemn psycho-
n
logical aggression is surprising given that its effects are as or more damag-
io
ing to the victim’s long-term well-being than physical aggression. It is as if
at
American adults adhere to the familiar childhood retort that “Sticks and
ci
stones may break my bones, but words will never hurt me.” Unfortunately,
so
that long-lived maxim is untrue.
As
Research that attempts to identify individual characteristics that make a
person prone to tolerate domestic violence has yielded mixed findings. For
al
example, several studies examining the sex of the perceiver suggest that
ic
women find a perpetrator’s aggressive behavior more blameworthy and less
og
acceptable than do men (e.g., Cauffman, Feldman, Jensen, & Arnett, 2000;
ol
Pierce & Harris, 1993), but not all studies have found similar sex differences
ch
(e.g., Capezza & Arriaga, 2008a). Although there may be some variation
y
among ethnic groups and subcultures, the belief that acts of partner aggression
Ps
Our own research (Capezza & Arriaga, 2008a, 2008b) suggests that per-
Am
ceptions are strongly influenced by the nature of the partner acts and that not
all forms of partner aggression are perceived equally. In one study (Capezza &
e
Arriaga, 2008b), college students who had previously been or were currently
th
argument and the husband becomes aggressive. The levels of physical aggres-
sion (i.e., none, low, high) and psychological aggression (i.e., low, high) were
11
regarding the event, with mean ratings beyond the midpoint and close to the
ig
scale anchor indicating negative perceptions. The main effect for physical
yr
the perpetrator and conflict in more negative ways with increasing levels of
physical aggression, confirming that norms exist among the college students we
C
sampled against using physical force in a relationship. The effect for psycholog-
ical aggression was not as robust, however: One dependent variable failed to
show any effect, one showed a more negative perception for high (vs. low) psy-
chological aggression, and one showed the effect only when physical aggression
was absent (i.e., a simple effect within the no physical aggression condition). A
A)
similar study (Capezza & Arriaga, 2008a) revealed that participants’ percep-
tions did not vary between a vignette depicting verbal aggression (e.g., yelling
P
(A
and swearing) versus one depicting severe emotional aggression (e.g., ridicul-
ing, degrading, highly threatening behaviors), despite the documented serious
n
consequences of emotional aggression.
io
People generally see physical partner aggression as negative. Views of
at
psychological aggression, although not positive or neutral, are not uniformly
ci
negative. It stands to reason that victims of partner aggression should share
so
these perceptions; that is, they should view their partner’s behavior negatively,
As
particularly when the acts qualify as physical aggression. Social psychological
theories, however, suggest that this commonsense prediction may be incorrect,
al
as we explain in the next section outlining our theoretical analysis of victim
ic
perceptions.
og
ol
ch
WHY MIGHT VICTIM PERCEPTIONS DIFFER
y
ner aggression might downplay their partner’s aggressive acts. One might
ic
expect victims to seek help and sympathy and to complain that their partners’
er
acts are severe and damaging. Why and how, then, might victims downplay
Am
Consistency Theories
by
their partner’s aggressive acts less negatively than victims who are less com-
mitted to their partners. Heider’s (1958) balance theory, for example, suggests
©
what he called a positive unit relation between the two partners and a positive
ht
unit relation between the perpetrator and his aggressive act. At the outset, the
ig
sentiment relation between the victim and the perpetrator’s aggressive act
yr
(i.e., two positive and one negative relation). If the unit relation linking the
two partners is strong, that is, if the ties keeping them together are strong and
C
the victim is committed, then this would create pressure to shift the sentiment
relation between the victim and the perpetrator’s aggressive act from negative
to neutral or positive. In short, the victim would adopt a less negative view of
the partner’s aggressive act.
In addition to commitment, the nature of the aggressive behavior may
A)
also make a difference. Overtly aggressive acts (e.g., hitting, punching) are more
difficult to reinterpret as being benign than are less physical forms of aggression
P
(A
(e.g., belittling, degrading). Regardless of commitment level, an individual may
be more likely to adopt a neutral sentiment relationship toward an act of ver-
n
bal aggression than toward an act of physical aggression.
io
Festinger’s (1957) cognitive dissonance theory would make a similar pre-
at
diction. The theory suggests that accepting two opposing beliefs results in
ci
mental discomfort that motivates a person to change one of the beliefs to be
so
consistent with the other. Less committed individuals faced with an aggressive
As
(and thus negative) partner act might come to feel more negative about the
relationship. More committed individuals, however, are motivated to feel pos-
al
itive about the partner and therefore would adopt a less negative perception
ic
of the partner act. Similarly, less physical forms of aggression would not create
og
as much dissonance as would overtly physical forms of aggression.
ol
ch
Interdependence Theory
y
Ps
interaction situation (e.g., a partner interaction) and the person’s broad goals
er
what the partner does; they are “dependent” on the partner to the extent that
their interaction experience is strongly affected by what the partner does or
e
and determines the actual response is a person’s broader goals for the relation-
20
ship. Actual behavior reflects any of several broad goals, such as wanting to help
the partner, promote the relationship, or be slightly ahead of the partner
©
downplay or justify the aggressive act) or reduce their motivation to save the
op
A)
aggressive may be perceived as less contentious than physical aggression situa-
tions. Victims may find it easier to respond in benign ways toward less rather
P
(A
than more overt forms of aggression, irrespective of their commitment level.
In addition to predicting responses to contentious partner situations,
n
interdependence theory suggests that people become acclimated to events in
io
their relationship by forming expectations of typical behavior, namely a stan-
at
dard of comparison or “comparison level” (Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). Individ-
ci
uals gauge their satisfaction level based on whether events in their relationship
so
exceed their expectations (causing high satisfaction) or fall short of their
As
expectations (causing low satisfaction). As such, victims who repeatedly expe-
rience partner aggression may come to expect the occurrence of aggression in
al
their current and future intimate relationships. The more aggression individ-
ic
uals experience, the more likely they are to perceive the acts as normal rather
og
than severely negative.
ol
Taken together, these theoretical frameworks call into question the idea
ch
that a target of partner aggression would immediately take a negative stance
y
toward the partner’s aggressive actions. It depends on factors such as the indi-
Ps
vidual’s level of commitment, the nature of the aggressive acts, and the current
comparison level or level of expectations.
an
ic
er
to an aggressive partner, they may have a vested interest in holding more tol-
20
pervasive among women who had experienced such aggression than among
yr
Much (but not all) of our own research on this issue has primarily
involved female college students. We focus on women mainly to narrow our
C
analysis at the outset, with the intention of expanding it to male victims once
A)
We focus on college students for several reasons. From a practical stand-
point, it has been convenient to collect data from college samples. More
P
(A
important, however, on theoretical grounds, we have been interested in the
forces that keep victims connected to an aggressive partner when there are no
n
legally binding reasons for remaining with the partner (which is the case in
io
marriage), and when victims reside in an environment where there are many
at
other potential partners (which interdependence theory conceptualizes in
ci
terms of the comparison level for alternatives; Kelley & Thibaut, 1978).
so
Because the objective circumstances are such that leaving one’s abusive part-
As
ner should not be difficult, it stands to reason that the subjective circumstances
keeping victims in the relationship must be strong.
al
ic
Research Comparing General Perceptions of
og
Current Victims, Past Victims, and Nonvictims
ol
ch
We sought to examine further the link between being victimized and
y
dicted that current or past victims would be more tolerant than nonvictims,
based on the idea in interdependence theory that individuals shift their point
an
of reference for expected behavior based on their own experiences. The more
ic
aggression one has experienced, the more aggression might come to be expected
er
in relationships, and thus the less likely it would be viewed as grounds for end-
Am
ing a relationship.
Our critical prediction, however, was with respect to differences between
e
current and past victims. If these two groups do not differ in their perceptions
th
one’s current relationship goals. They are influenced only by the victim’s expe-
riences and expectations. Consistency and interdependence theories, how-
11
ever, suggest that current victims should be more tolerant of partner aggression
20
than past victims. Current victims remain involved with their partner and thus
have an interest in holding perceptions that are consistent with this involve-
©
rent victims would be more tolerant in general of partner aggression than past
ig
Across two studies, we recruited female college students who had pre-
viously been or currently were in a relationship (Study 1, n = 186; Study 2,
op
tionship status (i.e., currently dating or not), and their victimization status
A)
ically aggressive acts (e.g., partner insulted or swore at you, partner grabbed
and shook you).
P
(A
Participants also completed a scale that asked about the same and addi-
tional specific partner aggressive acts; on this scale, they were instructed to indi-
n
cate the degree to which having a partner commit that act would be grounds
io
for ending a relationship. We also included one nonaggressive conflict act as a
at
comparison point. The scale anchors were definitely would not end a relationship
ci
versus definitely would end a relationship. Responses were recoded and averaged
so
to create three dependent variables, with higher numbers indicating more tol-
As
erance for a particular type of act: (a) a nonaggressive act (e.g., refused to talk
about an issue with you), (b) psychologically aggressive acts (e.g., insulted or
al
swore at you, intentionally destroyed your belongings), and (c) physically
ic
aggressive acts (e.g., grabbed and shook you). Study 2 also assessed the partici-
og
pant’s perpetration status and eliminated participants who had been perpetra-
ol
tors more than victims. All analyses controlled for whether the participant was
ch
currently in a relationship. Study 1 also controlled for the format of the survey
y
(paper and pencil vs. online). Study 2 exclusively relied on online surveys, but
Ps
sion than physical aggression and were less uniform (i.e., more variable) in their
er
nonaggressive act. In short, not all aggressive acts are perceived similarly, and
there is more tolerance of psychological aggression than of physical aggression.
e
tion status was significantly and positively associated with perceptions of what
by
would be grounds for ending a relationship. This provides evidence that stan-
dards shift based on one’s experience, consistent with interdependence theory’s
11
erant than nonvictims. In short, the more connected a victim is, the more moti-
ht
of acts that have already occurred, rather than perceptions of what might be
grounds for ending a relationship if they were to occur. That is, we shift our
C
focus from hypothetical, possible acts to ones that have actually occurred.
A)
those who are not. However, these theories have not provided precise ways to
conceptualize and measure variations in closeness or connection among rela-
P
(A
tionship partners. In that respect, interdependence theory has been useful.
Rusbult’s concept of relationship commitment (Rusbult, 1983; Rusbult et al.,
n
2001)—thoughts and feelings that reflect wanting to continue the relation-
io
ship and being attached to the partner, thoughts and feelings that stem from
at
being strongly affected by a partner—is useful for predicting variations in per-
ci
ceptions among current daters. In several lines of work, our basic prediction
so
has been the same: The more committed a victim is to a partner, the more
As
likely the victim will be to avoid maintaining strongly negative views of the
partner’s aggressive acts.
al
One study (Arriaga, 2002) testing this basic prediction examined whether
ic
victims of partner aggression “spin” (reinterpret) their partner’s aggressive acts
og
as “just joking around.” Students at a community college and a large university
ol
were recruited for a survey study. Of the 82 who reported currently being in a
ch
romantic relationship, 54 (18 males and 36 females) reported that their current
y
partner had engaged in at least one aggressive act. Participants completed var-
Ps
cate the number of times their current partner had been aggressive, that is, to
ic
Participants also completed a scale modeled after the CTS, listing the
Am
same aggressive acts but varying the instructions to elicit instances when the
partner was joking around. Some of the acts listed—the same as those used
e
earlier in the survey—might well have occurred when the partner was joking
th
around (e.g., pushing) but other acts were unequivocally aggressive (e.g.,
by
kicking, beating up, striking with a weapon). Participants who reported these
severely aggressive acts in a joking context were deemed to be reinterpreting
11
really were. The number of times these severe acts occurred provided an indi-
cator of joking violence.
©
ing that highly committed individuals did not share the same pattern of percep-
op
much more likely to have their reports of joking violence associated with their
reports of conflict violence, r(32) = .59, p < .01, suggesting that they reinter-
preted their partner’s aggression in ways that would make it easier to accept
while continuing the relationship.
A)
The highest levels of conflict violence were reported by individuals who
were highly committed and who reported joking violence. For highly commit-
P
(A
ted individuals, accepting partner aggression and reinterpreting it go hand in
hand. As would be predicted from cognitive dissonance theory, individuals
n
who are strongly tied (i.e., committed) to their partner feel a sense of entrap-
io
ment (Rusbult & Martz, 1995) and find it difficult to report even particularly
at
difficult instances of aggression (Arriaga, 2002).
ci
so
As
PERCEPTIONS OF THE FUTURE: HOW EMOTIONALLY AFFECTED
DO VICTIMS ANTICIPATE BEING FROM A BREAKUP?
al
ic
So far we have focused on victims’ motivated perceptions of their part-
og
ner’s past aggressive behaviors. Interdependence theory suggests that victims
ol
might suffer from motivated, distorted perceptions of a range of interpersonal
ch
situations with their partner, not just those involving aggression. A person who
y
has no overarching goals for interactions with the partner would perceive things
Ps
2003) shows that people overestimate how emotionally affected they are likely
Am
demics predict they would be much less happy if denied tenure than is actually
th
the case among those who actually were denied tenured. In one of several stud-
by
ies discussed by Wilson and Gilbert (2003), Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg,
and Wheatley (1998) examined the bias in forecasting future happiness among
11
relationship partners. They found that partners thought they would feel much
20
less happy following a breakup than was actually the case among those who had
recently experienced a breakup.
©
Would victims of partner aggression show the same bias, whereby they
ht
overestimate how unhappy they might feel if their relationship with the
ig
aggressive partner ended? The commonsense view is that victims should not
yr
relationships than are nonvictims, and ending the relationship would provide
an opportunity to undo the negative consequences of being a victim of part-
C
ner aggression.
A)
written) suggests that victims exhibit the same bias as nonvictims in predicting
their (un)happiness following a breakup, even though victims report less hap-
P
(A
piness overall and less satisfaction in their relationships as compared with non-
victims. In an initial cross-sectional survey study (n = 165), dating participants
n
were asked to forecast their happiness immediately following, 6 months after,
io
and 1 year after the dissolution of their relationship. Their responses were com-
at
pared with current happiness ratings of individuals who had been in a past
ci
aggressive relationship and who experienced the aggression either within the
so
last month, 6 months, or 1 year.
As
Several interesting findings emerged for victims of partner aggression.
First, current victims forecasted they would feel much less happy if their rela-
al
tionship were to end than was actually the case among past victims (i.e., those
ic
whose aggressive relationship had already ended). This was the case for every
og
time frame (i.e., immediately after, 6 months after, and 1 year after the dissolu-
ol
tion). Second, current victims reported significantly lower current happiness
ch
and relationship satisfaction than individuals whose current partners were not
y
aggressive (i.e., nonvictims). In short, current victims were less happy and less
Ps
satisfied than their nonvictim counterparts but just as likely to overestimate the
negative impact on their well-being of relationship dissolution. This finding
an
by nonvictims.
Third, regardless of current victimization status, level of commitment
e
to end, r(76) = −.58, p < .001. We are currently collecting longitudinal data
by
to compare with the findings from the cross-sectional study. In short, regard-
less of whether perceptions concern past partner aggression or the future of a
11
help victims manage the paradox that arises from choosing to remain with an
aggressive partner (Dunham & Senn, 2000) and is one way of coping with a
severely troubling situation. As Dunham and Senn (2000) noted, this coping
strategy may have benefits and costs. In the short term, victims can find a
A)
“mental space” within which to sustain their relationships. In the long term,
however, denying or justifying a partner’s aggression may keep a victim from
P
(A
recognizing the acts as aggressive, detecting their negative consequences, and
seeking help.
n
An obvious way to avoid the devastating effects of partner aggression is
io
to prevent it from occurring. Major advances have been made over the last
at
decades in understanding perpetrators of partner aggression (Arriaga &
ci
Capezza, 2005; see also Chapters 2 and 14, this volume). Preventing the nega-
so
tive consequences of partner aggression, however, is not limited to ending the
As
violence itself. It also involves helping victims of violence who are at risk of
negative mental and/or physical health outcomes (Arriaga & Capezza, 2005).
al
A major challenge in helping victims is simply finding them. Victims who
ic
downplay aggression often do not perceive a problem and thus fail to seek help.
og
Unfortunately, aggressive partners often convince victims that the situation is
ol
not so bad, that other relationships would be worse, or that things will get bet-
ch
ter. At the start of their relationship, the perpetrator may have had qualities to
y
attract the victim, and she may hope that those qualities will prevail and replace
Ps
the aggressive tendencies. It should come as no surprise, then, that a victim may
decline over time—feel more depressed, anxious, and uncertain—and yet not
an
Other victims recognize a problem but cannot leave their partner. They
er
may be financially dependent on the partner (Rusbult & Martz, 1995). Alter-
Am
that she is to blame for the aggression. As she comes to feel increasingly respon-
th
sible and intent on repairing the relationship, she becomes further exposed to
by
spiral of more victim self-blame, self-loathing, and loss of self, which increases
dependence and susceptibility to future aggression (Kirkwood, 1993). Many
©
immediate and sustained financial assistance (e.g., providing shelter, job train-
op
partner acts as destructive and understanding how they exert their nefarious
effects on the victim; and (c) restoring self-esteem, self-confidence, and a sense
that one is capable of happiness alone or in another relationship. Denying
aggression might best be addressed by a similar approach, especially by identi-
A)
fying destructive partner acts.
Would reducing commitment and dependence be the basis of an effec-
P
(A
tive intervention? It is violent partners, not commitment or dependence, that
causes harm to victims. Commitment and dependence are states that charac-
n
terize a multitude of close relationships, are crucial to relationship well-being,
io
and predict a vast array of relationship-maintenance behaviors. Indeed,
at
healthy, committed relationships may be a source of physical and emotional
ci
well-being (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001). It is not commitment per se
so
that needs to change but personal and societal norms that are conducive
As
to the acceptance of violence. As such, interventions might address find-
ing nurturing partners and sustaining a strong commitment to them rather
al
than mistakenly suggesting that commitment or dependence per se is the
ic
source of distress.
og
Reducing the toll of partner aggression on mental and physical health
ol
requires changing perceptions of partner aggression. As victims adopt less
ch
embellished, more negative perceptions of the partner’s behavior, they
y
become more emotionally ready to pursue their own goals with or without the
Ps
(a) help and, importantly, support victims; (b) ostracize those who engage in
ic
partner aggression; and (c) support policies that will eradicate all forms of
er
REFERENCES
11
Arias, I., & Pape, K. T. (1999). Psychological abuse: Implications for adjustment and
commitment to leave violent partners. Violence and Victims, 14, 55–67.
20
Arriaga, X. B., & Agnew, C. R. (2001). Being committed: Affective, cognitive, and
conative components of relationship commitment. Personality and Social Psy-
C
Arriaga, X. B., & Capezza, N. M. (2005). Targets of partner violence: The impor-
tance of understanding coping trajectories. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20,
89–99. doi:10.1177/0886260504268600
Capezza, N. M., & Arriaga, X. B. (2008a). Factors associated with acceptance of psy-
chological aggression against women. Violence Against Women, 14, 612–633.
A)
doi:10.1177/1077801208319004
P
Capezza, N. M., & Arriaga, X. B. (2008b). You can degrade but you can’t hit: Differ-
(A
ences in perceptions of psychological versus physical aggression. Journal of Social
n
and Personal Relationships, 25, 225–245. doi:10.1177/0265407507087957
io
Carlson, B. E., & Worden, A. P. (2005). Attitude and beliefs about domestic vio-
at
lence: Results of a public opinion survey: 1. Definitions of domestic violence,
ci
criminal domestic violence, and prevalence. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20,
so
1197–1218. doi:10.1177/0886260505278530
As
Cauffman, E., Feldman, S. S., Jensen, L. A., & Arnett, J. J. (2000). The (un)accept-
ability of violence against peers and dates. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15,
al
652–673. doi:10.1177/0743558400156003
ic
Coker, A. L., Davis, K. E., Arias, I., Desai, S., Sanderson, M., Brandt, H. M., &
og
Smith, P. H. (2002). Physical and mental health effects of intimate partner vio-
lence for men and women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 23, 260–268.
ol
doi:10.1016/S0749-3797(02)00514-7
ch
Coker, A. L., Smith, P. H., Bethea, L., King, M. R., & McKeown, R. E. (2000). Phys-
y
doi:10.1177/088626000015003002
er
Ferraro, K. J., & Johnson, J. M. (1983). How women experience battering: The
Am
sity Press.
by
Follingstad, D. R., Rutledge, L. L., Berg, B. J., Hause, E. S., & Polek, D. S. (1990).
The role of emotional abuse in physically abusive relationships. Journal of Fam-
11
Garcia-Moreno, C., Heise, L., Jansen, H. A. F. M., Ellsberg, M., & Watts, C. (2005,
November). Violence against women. Science, 310, 1282–1283. doi:10.1126/
©
science.1121400
ht
Gelles, R. J. (1993). Through a sociological lens: Social structure and family vio-
ig
Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. (1998).
Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of
C
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal perception. New York, NY: Wiley.
doi:10.1037/10628-000
Johnson, M. P., & Ferraro, K. P. (2000). Research on domestic violence in the 1990s:
Making distinctions. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 948–963. doi:10.1111/
A)
j.1741-3737.2000.00948.x
P
Kelley, H. H. (1979). Personal relationships: Their structures and properties. Hillsdale,
(A
NJ: Erlbaum.
Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. W. (1978). Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdepen-
n
dence. New York, NY: Wiley
io
at
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: His and hers.
ci
Psychological Bulletin, 127, 472–503. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472
so
Kirkwood, C. (1993). Leaving abusive partners: From the scars of survival to the wisdom
for change. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
As
McClintock, C. G. (1972). Social motives: A set of propositions. Behavioral Science,
al
17, 438–454. doi:10.1002/bs.3830170505
ic
Murphy, C. M., & Hoover, S. A. (1999). Measuring emotional abuse in dating rela-
og
tionships as a multifactorial construct. Violence and Victims, 14, 39–53.
ol
O’Leary, K. D. (1999). Psychological abuse: A variable deserving critical attention
ch
in domestic violence. Violence and Victims, 14, 3–23.
y
Pierce, M. C., & Harris, R. J. (1993). The effect of provocation, race, and injury
Ps
1993.tb01006.x
ic
Rennison, C. M., & Welchans, S. (2000). Intimate partner violence (Report No. NCJ
er
Justice.
Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment model: The development
e
Rusbult, C. E., Martz, J. M., & Agnew, C. R. (1998). The Investment Model Scale:
©
Rusbult, C. E., Olsen, N., Davis, J. L., & Hannon, P. A. (2001). Commitment and
op
Erlbaum.
Sorenson, S. B., & Taylor, C. A. (2005). Female aggression toward male intimate
partners: An examination of social norms in a community-based sample. Psychol-
ogy of Women Quarterly, 29, 78–96. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00170.x
Straus, M. A., & Gelles R. J. (1990). Physical violence in American families: Risk factors
A)
and adaptations to violence in 8,145 families. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. New York,
P
(A
NY: Wiley.
Tjaden, P., & Theonnes, N. (2000). Prevalence, incidence, and consequences of violence
n
against women: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey (Report
io
No. NCJ 183781). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice, U.S. Depart-
at
ment of Justice.
ci
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. Zanna (Ed.),
so
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 345–411). San Diego,
As
CA: Academic Press.
Yovetich, N. A., & Rusbult, C. E. (1994). Accommodative behavior in close rela-
al
tionships: Exploring transformation of motivation. Journal of Experimental Social
ic
Psychology, 30, 138–164. doi:10.1006/jesp.1994.1007
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
12:00 PM
Am
er
ic
Page 384
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-22_CH21-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 12:01 PM Page 385
21
PA)
THE PSYCHOLOGICAL TOLL OF
(A
EXPOSURE TO POLITICAL VIOLENCE:
n
io
THE ISRAELI EXPERIENCE
at
ci
so
ZAHAVA SOLOMON AND KARNI GINZBURG
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
The proclivity of human beings for aggression, which too often culmi-
ic
nates in war, entails a high price in human suffering. Combatants, mostly men,
er
are naturally the ones who suffer most severely. In constant danger themselves,
Am
they witness the injury and death of friends and enemies and are repeatedly
exposed to the gruesome sights and sounds of slaughter. They struggle with fear
e
and loneliness as well as the more tangible deprivations of food, drink, and
th
sleep. The enormous destructive power of modern weapons and the uncertain-
by
ties of modern guerrilla warfare add to the already massive stress of war.
People have recognized the inevitability of psychological wounds of war
11
disorders receive public attention during and immediately after a war, little
attention is devoted to the long-term psychological tolls of war. Hence, until
©
recently, knowledge about this topic has been quite fragmented, and lessons
ht
learned in one war have easily been forgotten before the next one begins
ig
(Mangelsdorff, 1985).
yr
385
12051-22_CH21-rev2.qxd 5/25/10 12:01 PM Page 386
(IDF) reserves, which means that they are more or less continually exposed to
military violence or training for war. Thus, the postwar experiences of Israeli
veterans differ from those of veterans in other countries, where soldiers usu-
ally return to a stable civilian society after a war ends. Given that the context
A)
of postwar recovery has a substantial effect on traumatized veterans’ mental
health, one cannot necessarily extrapolate to Israeli veterans from findings of
P
(A
studies conducted in other parts of the world.
In this chapter, we bring together findings from 3 decades of Israeli
n
studies of male combatants designed to shed light on the immediate and
io
long-term effects of recurrent and prolonged exposure to military violence.
at
In particular, the chapter is based on our own prospective longitudinal
ci
assessment of two cohorts: (a) veterans of the First Lebanon War (1982) and
so
(b) ex-prisoners of war (POWs) from the Yom Kippur War (1973).
As
al
COMBAT-INDUCED STRESS DISORDERS
ic
og
The most common and conspicuous psychological sequelae of combat are
ol
combat stress reaction, an acute response, and posttraumatic stress disorder, a
ch
chronic disorder.
y
Ps
which soldiers need on the battlefield. It is perhaps surprising that most com-
batants manage their anxiety fairly well and remain psychologically immune
e
diers, do not endanger themselves or their fellow comrades, and do not ask to
by
places and eras as combat stress reaction (CSR), shell shock, combat fatigue, or war
ig
neurosis, among other terms (Solomon, 1993). CSR occurs when a soldier is
yr
A)
symptoms include confusion, temporal and spatial disorientation, and impaired
attention, memory, judgment, and decision making. The main emotional
P
(A
symptoms are alternating states of paralyzing anxiety and deep depression.
Behavioral symptoms include manifestations of these emotions: great agitation,
n
on the one hand, and apathy and withdrawal, on the other. Some of the symp-
io
toms are quite bizarre, with soldiers tearing off their uniforms and running amok
at
toward the enemy or becoming frozen in their tracks, or clinging to a piece of
ci
clothing. These manifestations change as rapidly as the emotional state that
so
underlies them and can be quite perplexing to an observer (Solomon, 1993).
As
The reported prevalence rates of CSR vary considerably, both within and
among wars (Solomon, 1993). The reported rates among World War II’s
al
Allied soldiers ranged from 10% to 48% of injured soldiers. In the Vietnam
ic
War, rates were significantly lower, with the official figure being 1.2% of the
og
total number of American soldiers wounded on the battlefield. In the Yom
ol
Kippur War, the official count was 10% of Israeli soldiers wounded in action,
ch
although in some units it was as high as 70%. In the First Lebanon War, the
y
official figure was 23% of Israelis wounded in action, meaning that one of every
Ps
four war casualties was a psychiatric casualty. More recently, rates of only 3%
of CSR casualties were reported among British veterans evacuated from the
an
battle zone in Iraq (Turner et al., 2005). This huge variation seems to reflect
ic
ences in the amount and severity of destruction and atrocities to which sol-
Am
diers are exposed on the battlefield. However, despite this variability, the
reported rates clearly indicate that CSR is an inevitable and common conse-
e
quence of war.
th
by
CSR can be a transient state for some veterans, but for others it marks
20
ever, profound and prolonged psychological sequelae may remain and consol-
ht
A)
study commenced during the war itself, and the follow-up study is still con-
tinuing. Hundreds of veterans from both study groups have been interviewed
P
at four time points: 1, 2, 3, and 20 years after their active participation in the
(A
1982 war.
n
io
Chronic Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
at
ci
According to currently prevailing psychiatric formulations, PTSD is the
so
most common psychological injury of exposure to war violence (American
Psychiatric Association, 1994). Still, few empirical investigations have
As
assessed PTSD in identified CSR casualties. The results of our 20-year follow-
al
up study of all treated Israeli CSR casualties of the First Lebanon War
ic
(Solomon & Mikulincer, 2006) revealed that 64% of identified CSR casual-
og
ties suffered from PTSD symptoms 1 year after the war. At 2 years after the
war, the rate of PTSD casualties decreased to 59% and then further to 40%
ol
at 3 years. The most interesting and perhaps surprising finding, however, was
ch
that the rate of PTSD casualties increased again to 53% 20 years after the war.
y
In other words, the war does not end for many traumatized veterans; instead,
Ps
control group who were not identified as CSR casualties on the battlefield.
ic
Fourteen percent of them reported suffering from PTSD 1 year after the war,
er
22% after 2 years, 11% after 3 years, and 27% 20 years later. These figures
Am
point to a long-term detrimental effect of war violence even for soldiers who
survive the immediate stress of battlefield without any identified psychologi-
e
cal breakdown.
th
It is important to note that the afflicted soldiers in our control group did
by
not seek help. Many of them were probably unaware that they suffered from
CSR symptoms or believed that these symptoms were a natural and inevitable
11
their plight but were reluctant to seek help. It is likely that these veterans
with silent PTSD are a mere fraction of a much larger number of psychiatric
©
American veterans suffered from PTSD for years without seeking help.
yr
Although we realized that PTSD often goes untreated, it was still sur-
prising and troubling in the Israeli context. Had the PTSD casualties sought
op
help for their war-related disturbances at any IDF mental health clinic, they
C
would have averted the risk of being sent back to the front. In our view, this
disinclination to seek help can be explained in part by the finding that PTSD
among our control veterans was less severe and less distressing than that of
CSR veterans (Solomon, 1993). Earlier studies have shown that the propen-
A)
sity to seek treatment for psychiatric disorders and the propensity to adopt the
sick role are closely related to symptom severity (e.g., Nadler, 1983).
P
(A
Alternatively, it is possible that PTSD casualties in the control group
were highly motivated to continue serving in the army and were not interested
n
in obtaining any potential secondary gain. At the time of the study, masculine
io
identity was strongly associated with military service in Israel. Identifying one-
at
self as ill may be costly in terms of both self-esteem and social approval. Vet-
ci
erans who sustained a CSR had to contend with the implications of their
so
breakdown, with the shame and guilt of having let down their buddies and
As
having betrayed the trust placed in them by their family and nation. Consid-
ering the great importance attributed to the army in Israel, the silent PTSD
al
sufferers’ sense of failure and injured manhood must weigh heavily on them
ic
and perhaps contributes to their continuing PTSD.
og
The high rates of PTSD casualties in both of our study groups are
ol
intriguing; they may reflect the recurrent exposure to military and political
ch
violence in Israel. All of our study participants, like other Israeli men of their
y
age, have served in the IDF reserves and could have been recalled for active
Ps
military duty during the first and second intifadas (Palestinian rebellions), the
Second Lebanon War, and other military operations in Lebanese and Pales-
an
tinian territories. Like all other Israeli citizens, they have been repeatedly
ic
exposed to Palestinian terror attacks, suicidal bombers, and Iraqi missile attacks
er
during the last 20 years. We believe that such recurrent exposure to war may
Am
may have two main causes. First, 20 years after the war most of our participants
th
review one’s life (Solomon & Mikulincer, 2006). This transition may allow
20
carried out in the midst of the second intifada (2003), when the Israeli popu-
ht
lation was exposed to numerous terror attacks and sights of destruction, injuries,
ig
and violent death. This intense exposure of the previously traumatized veter-
yr
ans to harsh political violence may have reactivated the psychological break-
op
down they had experienced 20 years earlier, during or immediately after the
First Lebanon War.
C
A)
ture on combat stress has little to offer on this issue. However, studies of other
P
adverse experiences provide three alternative theoretical perspectives on the
(A
potential effects of recurrent exposure to war violence. First, the vulnerability
perspective (Coleman, Butcher, & Carson, 1980) considers repeated exposure
n
io
to stressful events to be a risk factor because it drains a person’s coping
at
resources. Second, the stress inoculation perspective (Epstein, 1983) holds that
ci
repeated exposure to stressful events serves as an “immunizer” because it fos-
so
ters the development of more effective coping strategies and thereby promotes
As
adaptation. Third, the stress resolution hypothesis (Block & Zautra, 1981) pro-
poses that what matters is not so much the person’s repeated exposure to a par-
al
ticular stressful event but how he or she coped with it. According to Block and
ic
Zautra (1981), successful coping leads to a sense of well-being and to increased
og
coping resources, whereas unsuccessful coping leads to increased vulnerability
and distress and an erosion of coping resources. ol
ch
To examine the effects of recurrent exposure to war violence, we asked
our sample of First Lebanon War veterans to indicate whether they had partic-
y
Ps
ipated as combatants in previous Israeli wars and whether they had sustained a
psychological breakdown in each of them (Solomon, Mikulincer, & Jacob,
an
1987). We found that the highest rate of CSR casualties in the First Lebanon
ic
previous Israeli war (66%); the lowest rate was among soldiers who had fought
Am
soldiers are better off than those who had collapsed in a previous war. Although
11
not every soldier who sustains CSR is doomed to a second breakdown under
similar circumstances, CSR leaves most casualties more vulnerable the second
20
time around (Coleman et al., 1980). We should note that only a minority of
©
bility, other CSR casualties who were not allowed to return to military duties
would presumably be even more vulnerable.
op
more apparent when the number of previous wars is taken into account. Among
soldiers with a prior psychological breakdown, CSR rates in the First Lebanon
War increased linearly with the number of prior war experiences: 57% after one
war, 67% after two, and 83% after three. Among soldiers who had fought with-
out a prior psychological breakdown, CSR rates in the First Lebanon War were
A)
curvilinear: Veterans who actively participated in either one or three previous
wars had higher CSR rates (50% and 44%, respectively) than did soldiers who
P
(A
participated in two (33%). Taken together, our research findings suggest that
exposure to military violence scars an individual and weakens his resistance
n
to future combat-related violence. They also suggest that whatever the pos-
io
sible inoculation benefits of successful stress resolution, repeated trauma may
at
eventually break even the hardiest souls.
ci
so
Reactivation of Traumatic Reactions
As
Reactivation of traumatic reactions is a well-known phenomenon. Sur-
al
vivors of many traumatic events, including the Holocaust, combat, and rape,
ic
among others, can experience a reactivation of their response to the original
og
trauma when they are reminded of it (Christenson, Walker, Ross, & Malthie,
1981; Lindemann, 1944). ol
ch
As we have noted, 66% of veterans who suffered from psychological break-
down in the Yom Kippur War showed a recurrent CSR episode in the First
y
Ps
Lebanon War. Studying these cases in a deeper and more detailed way, we
found that they represented two different types of trauma-related reactivation
an
(e.g., Solomon, Garb, Bleich, & Grupper, 1987). Twenty-three percent of these
ic
erans seemed to have completely recovered from their previous Yom Kippur
Am
left more salient scars, and veterans continued to suffer from PTSD symp-
toms of high (51%), medium (9%), and low severity (17%) between the Yom
20
Kippur War and the First Lebanon War. Symptoms became intensified during
©
reserve duty, and the call-up notice to Lebanon provoked considerable antic-
ipatory anxiety. Moreover, these men were so vulnerable to combat-related
ht
violence that their CSR during the First Lebanon War had been triggered by
ig
yr
an incident unrelated to their original war trauma and, in many cases, one that
did not pose a direct or immediate danger.
op
the 9 years between the Yom Kippur War and the First Lebanon War, and
they had generally been successful. While relying on coping mechanisms such
as repression, suppression, denial, and cognitive avoidance, most of them
married, started families, and held jobs—and some did well professionally.
None were hospitalized. All continued to serve in the reserves, despite the
A)
fact that their symptoms were intensified in the presence of military violence.
Many hid their symptoms from their friends, families, and army commanders.
P
(A
Their recurrent CSR during the First Lebanon War revealed the psy-
chological damage that the first breakdown had created and seemed to deepen
n
the damage. In general, there were more symptoms during the recurrent CSR
io
episode in the First Lebanon War than during the first CSR episode in the
at
Yom Kippur War, and the symptoms were more intense and debilitating. Fur-
ci
thermore, even though some soldiers with prior CSR in the Yom Kippur War
so
participated in the First Lebanon War without further breakdown, the detri-
As
mental effects of the earlier episode were still detectable a decade later
(Solomon, 1993).
al
ic
og
LONG-TERM IMPLICATIONS OF WAR CAPTIVITY:
LESSONS FROM THE YOM KIPPUR WAR ol
y ch
mental stress that has pathogenic effects. For one group of soldiers, combat is
but the first step in a traumatic journey. For these soldiers who fall into enemy
an
hands, the war continues even after the shooting stops. These POWs continue
ic
range with his enemies, and brutal force is typically used to deprive him of his
by
autonomy (e.g., Avnery, 1982). During captivity, the POW is often held in
unsanitary and uncomfortable conditions and may be deprived of sufficient
11
amounts of food and water (e.g., Hunter, 1993). The POW may even be sub-
20
ency on his captors. The lack of social support, denial of privacy, and continu-
ig
ous torture and humiliation may cripple a POW’s identity and potentially pave
yr
These stressors are added on to the extreme conditions and hazards that the
A)
trol and coercion are used to deprive the POWs of their sense of autonomy
and replace it with a sense of horror and helplessness.
P
(A
Captors use various techniques to break a captive’s spirit. During the Yom
Kippur War, Israeli POWs were repeatedly exposed to anti-Israeli propaganda,
n
misinformed of the death of Israel’s leaders, and falsely told about the triumph
io
of Arab states over Israel and the occupation of Israel by Arab armies. At times,
at
captives were informed that their homes had been destroyed and their loved
ci
ones and relatives killed (e.g., Avnery, 1982). These were deliberate efforts to
so
exacerbate the POWs’ feelings of loneliness, weakness, and isolation.
As
The trauma of captivity was often made worse by prisoners’ feelings that
they had failed to meet the heroic standards emphasized by Israeli culture.
al
This ethos requires that prisoners of war refuse to disclose any information,
ic
endure the unbearable pain of interrogation without cracking, and even take
og
their own lives if necessary. This code of conduct is entwined with the ideal
ol
image of the Israeli combatant who is expected to “fight until the end” and
ch
“maintain his honor” during interrogations (Gavriely, 2006). Thus, many
y
Israeli POWs were regarded not only as soldiers who failed in the important
Ps
may deserve the death penalty. Moreover, disclosure of secrets while in cap-
Am
is at the heart of what Avnery (1982) called the captive’s dilemma: staying
th
alive and sustaining criticism and condemnation versus complying with the
by
norm at the cost of losing one’s life. For many ex-POWs, this dilemma pro-
duced feelings of utter failure and unbearable weakness.
11
During the 1973 Yom Kippur War, several hundred Israeli soldiers were
20
captured on the Syrian and Egyptian fronts. POWs held in Egypt were released
relatively quickly, within 4 to 6 weeks. POWs in Syria were held for 8 months.
©
During captivity in Egypt, the prisoners were held in separate cells, whereas in
ht
Syria, after a rigorous interrogation period, POWs were held in groups, each
ig
in a large common room. In both states of captivity, Israeli soldiers were sub-
yr
the Egyptian or the Syrian front during the Yom Kippur War, and we compared
them with a control group of combat veterans who fought on the same fronts
as the ex-POWs during the same war but were not taken captive. Controls were
matched with the ex-POWs in terms of personal and military background. Par-
ticipants in the two groups (i.e., ex-POWs, controls) were interviewed twice:
A)
18 and 30 years after the war (see Solomon & Dekel, 2005, for details).
P
(A
Long-Term Psychological Sequelae of War Captivity
n
Findings from our study reveal that 3 decades after their release from cap-
io
tivity, 23% of former Israeli POWs still met symptom criteria from the Diag-
at
nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; American Psychiatric
ci
Association, 1994) for PTSD, as compared with 5% of non-POW controls
so
(Solomon & Dekel, 2005). This figure points to both the resilience of the 77%
As
of former POWs who did not meet PTSD criteria and to the long-lasting psy-
chological damage of captivity to the remaining 23%. The question is why the
al
psychological damage of captivity should be so much more enduring than that
ic
of combat, which is itself pathogenic.
og
Several explanations may be offered. The simplest is based on the spe-
ol
cial hardships of captivity: the torture, humiliation, and isolation that are part
ch
and parcel of war captivity (Mollica et al., 1990). In addition, these hardships
y
are personal (Herman, 1992). That is, the threat of combat to the life and
Ps
tive and his captors. The special torments of captivity are part of a planned
Am
physical survival.
th
sion and social support derived from comrades and commanders is a well-
documented sustaining force for combatants (e.g., Solomon, Mikulincer, &
©
Hobfoll, 1987). However, captivity renders the POW totally isolated and
ht
Still another possible explanation is that POWs adopt strategies that are
useful in captivity, such as suspiciousness and hyperalertness, and then apply
C
them in civilian life later on, where they are often counterproductive. Eberly,
Harkness, and Engdahl (1991) suggested that traumatized POWs can be viewed
as survivors who continue to exhibit patterns of thought, emotion, and behav-
ior that were adaptive during their period of traumatization.
A fourth possible explanation focuses on the compounding of traumatic
A)
experiences. For most POWs, the trauma of captivity follows on the heels of
the trauma of combat. Captivity thus extends the duration of the traumatic
P
(A
experience, further drawing on the soldier’s already depleted coping resources
(Ursano et al., 1996). As is well known, the longer a traumatic experience
n
lasts, the more severe the ensuing psychiatric disorders are likely to be (Hunter,
io
1993). The cumulative damage of multiple traumas is known to be more severe
at
than the damage of a single trauma (Herman, 1992).
ci
so
Trajectories of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Changes Over Time
As
The theoretical literature on the longitudinal effects of trauma offers three
al
alternative views on the temporal course of postcaptivity symptomatology. One
ic
view is that time is a healer and that ex-POWs will recover partially or com-
og
pletely with the passage of time (e.g., Engdahl, Speed, Eberly, & Schwartz,
ol
1991; Potts, 1994). A second, different view is that PTSD is a chronic ailment
ch
that will intensify over time because of a natural decline in resilience, perhaps
y
especially in midlife (e.g., Maercker, 1999). This view gains some support from
Ps
War II and the Korean War (Port, Engdahl, & Frazier, 2001). The third view
ic
Dickman, 1989). In this view, time can either heal or intensify the psycholog-
ical wounds of captivity, depending on an ex-POW’s personality and life expe-
e
riences after release. This view stresses the liability of PTSD symptomatology
th
and the possibility that life events and psychological changes or developments
by
can cause the symptoms to intensify or decline (Buffum & Wolfe, 1995).
Trauma researchers have not yet established which of these three views
11
best describes the long-term effects of war captivity. Too few studies tracing
20
the longitudinal effects of captivity have been carried out, and most have
assessed recovery and other changes in PTSD symptomatology only through
©
math of captivity, both between and within groups, is not well understood.
ig
the Yom Kippur War (Solomon & Dekel, 2005), we found that PTSD fol-
op
ments. Almost 20% of ex-POWs who did not meet PTSD criteria 18 years
after their release did meet them at the 30-year assessment. This deteriora-
tion occurred in less than 1% of combat controls. Given the study design,
it is impossible to know whether this 20% rise in ex-POWs’ PTSD rates
A)
reflects reactivation or delayed onset of PTSD. Previous studies reported
delayed-onset PTSD in 11% (Green et al., 1990) and 20% (Wolfe, Erickson,
P
(A
Sharkansky, King, & King, 1999) of various traumatized groups.
POWs also showed a statistically significant increase in endorsement of
n
each of the PTSD symptom clusters (i.e., intrusion, avoidance, hyperarousal).
io
Among controls, in contrast, there was no change in endorsement of the three
at
symptom clusters, along with a downward trend in the endorsement of most
ci
PTSD symptoms. These findings indicate that time exacerbates the detrimen-
so
tal effects of war captivity. This increase in PTSD among ex-POWs is consis-
As
tent with findings of increased PTSD rates and symptom levels over a 4-year
measurement interval among American ex-POWs (Port et al., 2001) but dif-
al
fers from findings of decreased PTSD symptoms over time (Engdahl et al.,
ic
1991). The differences are probably related to the times of measurement in the
og
research studies.
ol
A previous study (Port et al., 2001) found a U-shaped pattern, with high
ch
PTSD rates immediately after captivity, followed by a gradual decline, and
y
then, from midlife onward, a rise in PTSD rates. It is possible that our first
Ps
assessment, taken 18 years after prisoners’ release, fell within the lower part of
the curve, and our second assessment, 12 years later, reflected the rising rates
an
ilar to increases in PTSD rates observed among First Lebanon War veterans
er
(reported earlier in this chapter). In our view, both of them can be explained
Am
mention the resilience of the non-POW veterans, who had low rates of PTSD
th
both 18 and 30 years after the war (3.8% and 4.8%, respectively). Even though
by
all of them had seen combat and, as noted, most continued to serve in active
reserve and were exposed to the ongoing threat of terror, they did not show any
11
higher, but the majority did not meet PTSD criteria at either time of assess-
ment. This high level of resilience in both groups lends further support to the
©
contention that resilience in the face of trauma is more common than psycho-
ht
were helpless captives under control of their captors suggest that survivors
tend to develop not only PTSD but also a unique form of posttraumatic seque-
lae that penetrates and alters their personality, often referred to as complex
PTSD or disorder of extreme stress not otherwise specified (DESNOS; Herman,
1992; Terr, 1991; van der Kolk, 2002). Herman (1992) suggested that pro-
A)
longed captivity disrupts captives’ personal relationships, which may result in
long-lasting attachment injuries and may be manifested in either anxious or
P
(A
avoidant attachment. Whereas anxious attachment characterizes individuals
who are concerned that their significant others will not be available in times
n
of need and who wish intensely for proximity to and care from others, avoidant
io
attachment characterizes individuals who cannot trust others and who with-
at
draw from intimacy and interdependence (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007).
ci
To test Herman’s (1992) ideas, we assessed attachment orientations
so
among former POWs and controls 18 and 30 years after the Yom Kippur War,
As
and we examined reciprocal associations between PTSD and attachment ori-
entations over time (Solomon, Dekel, & Mikulincer, 2008). Paralleling the
al
increase in PTSD rates among ex-POWs between the two waves of assessment,
ic
there were increased levels of self-reported anxious and avoidant attachment
og
among ex-POWs during the same period. This trend was evident only in the
ol
ex-POW group; levels of anxious and avoidant attachment were quite stable
ch
in the control group. In addition, increases in both anxious and avoidant
y
These findings further highlight the pervasive impact of captivity. In line with
previous studies (van der Kolk, Roth, Pelcovitz, Sunday, & Spinazzola, 2005),
an
psychological injuries that are not captured by the PTSD diagnosis alone.
er
Similarly, harsh and dramatic experiences, such as war captivity, may alter a
th
person’s basic trust in others in a way that undermines the ability to maintain
by
secure attachments. Hence, even ex-POWs who have had secure attachments
may become more anxious and may defensively avoid interpersonal contact fol-
11
were found so many years after the war ended demonstrates the pervasive and
dramatic effects of intentional, human-inflicted victimization.
©
ht
Finally, our findings indicate that about twice as many ex-POWs as com-
op
bat controls felt that they needed psychotherapy, and about five times as many
ex-POWs as combat controls actually sought and obtained it. The observed
C
rates of seeking psychotherapy and being willing to admit a need for help
among the ex-POW group were high relative to norms in Israel. They are tes-
timony to the intensity of the distress from which ex-POWs suffer and to the
increasing acceptance in Israeli society of seeking help following traumatic
military experiences (Gavriely, 2006).
A)
The higher rates of both reported need for help and actual help seeking
among ex-POWs may be explained by their greater trauma-related and gen-
P
eral distress and their lower recovery rates. Even those who received treatment
(A
were less prone to recover than combat controls who were treated. The com-
n
plex and prolonged stressors to which they were exposed may have contributed
io
to their intensive, pervasive, and widespread distress (Herman, 1992). The
at
lower rate of recovery among treated POWs than among untreated controls is
ci
further evidence not only of the difficulties in treating trauma but also of the
so
fact that the more massive the trauma, the more damage it causes and the more
As
difficult it is to ameliorate with professional intervention.
al
ic
CONCLUSIONS
og
The studies discussed in this chapter were conducted among Israeli com-
ol
batants and ex-POWs. They were based on longitudinal designs, with assess-
ch
ments conducted 20 and 30 years after exposure to war-related violence.
y
Findings showed that for many combatants and former POWs, exposure to
Ps
war violence, atrocities, and massive acts of destruction can cause severe,
an
long-lasting psychological disorders. Although the war ended and the prison
doors opened years ago, many of the combatants and ex-POWs are still faced
ic
for many of these veterans, the picture has become even bleaker with time,
Am
sion, they also testify to the amazing resilience of many veterans. Further
th
war casualties and to devise more effective therapeutic interventions and pre-
ventive measures.
11
riences and ways to reduce their prevalence and severity. More thorough and
yr
their suffering.
REFERENCES
A)
Avnery, A. (1982). Coping and adjustment to war captivity (Unpublished master’s
thesis). Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel.
P
(A
Block, M., & Zautra, A. J. (1981). Satisfaction and distress in the community: A test
of the effects of life events. American Journal of Community Psychology, 9, 165–180.
n
doi:10.1007/BF00896365
io
Buffum, M. D., & Wolfe, N. S. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder and the World
at
War II veteran: Elderly patients who were in combat or were prisoners of war
ci
may have special health care needs that may not be obvious. Geriatric Nursing,
so
16, 264–270. doi:10.1016/S0197-4572(95)80006-9
As
Christenson, R. M., Walker, J. L., Ross, D. R., & Malthie, A. A. (1981). Reactivation
of traumatic conflicts. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 984–985.
al
Coleman, J. C., Butcher, J. N., & Carson, R. C. (1980). Abnormal psychology and
ic
modern life. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman.
og
Eberly, R. E., Harkness, A. R., & Engdahl, B. E. (1991). An adaptational view of
ol
trauma response as illustrated by the prisoner of war experience. Journal of
ch
Traumatic Stress, 4, 363–380. doi:10.1002/jts.2490040305
Engdahl, B. E., Speed, N., Eberly, R. E., & Schwartz, J. (1991). Comorbidity of psy-
y
Ps
00005053-199104000-00001
Epstein, S. (1983). Natural healing processes of the mind: II. Graded stress inoculation
ic
Stress prevention and management: A cognitive behavioral approach (pp. 39–66). New
Am
Kippur War ex-POWs. Armed Forces and Society, 33, 94–105. doi:10.1177/
th
0095327X05282531
by
Green, B. L., Lindy, J. D., Grace, M. C., Gleser, G. C., Leonard, A. C., Korol, M., &
Winget, C. (1990). Buffalo Creek survivors in the second decade: Stability of
11
h0079168
Herman, J. L. (1992). Trauma and recovery. New York, NY: Basic Books.
©
A)
nity Psychology, 13, 239–257. doi:10.1002/1520-6629(198507)13:3<239::AID-
JCOP2290130302>3.0.CO;2-T
P
(A
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics,
and change. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
n
io
Mollica, R. F., Wyshak, G., Lavelle, J., Truong, T., Tor, S., & Yang, T. (1990).
at
Assessing symptom change in Southeast Asian refugee survivors of mass vio-
lence and torture. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 83–88.
ci
so
Nadler, A. (1983). Personal characteristics and help-seeking. In J. D. Fisher,
A. Nadler, & B. M. DePaulo (Eds.), New directions in helping: Help-seeking (Vol. 2,
As
pp. 124–151). New York, NY: Academic Press.
al
Port, C. L., Engdahl, B., & Frazier, P. (2001). A longitudinal and retrospective study
ic
of PTSD among older prisoners of war. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158,
og
1474–1479. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1474
ol
Potts, M. K. (1994). Long-term effects of trauma: Posttraumatic stress among civilian
internees of the Japanese during WW2. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 681–698.
ch
doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199409)50:5<681::AID-JCLP2270500504>3.0.CO;2-3
y
Ps
Solomon, Z. (1993). Combat stress reaction: The enduring toll of war. New York, NY:
Plenum Press.
an
Solomon, Z., & Dekel, R. (2005). Posttraumatic stress disorder among Israeli ex-
ic
prisoners of war 18 and 30 years after release. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
er
Solomon, Z., Dekel, R., & Mikulincer, M. (2008). Complex trauma of war captivity:
A prospective study of attachment and PTSD. Psychological Medicine, 38,
1427–1434. doi:10.1017/S0033291708002808
e
th
Solomon, Z., Garb, R., Bleich, A., & Grupper, D. (1987). Reactivation of combat-
related posttraumatic stress disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 144,
by
51–55.
11
Solomon, Z., Mikulincer, M., & Hobfoll, S. (1987). Objective versus subjective
ht
006X.55.4.577
op
Solomon, Z., Mikulincer, M., & Jacob, B. R. (1987). Exposure to recurrent combat
stress: Combat stress reactions among Israeli soldiers in the Lebanon War. Psy-
C
Solomon, Z., & Prager, E. (1992). Elderly Israeli Holocaust survivors during the
Persian Gulf War: A study of psychological distress. The American Journal of
Psychiatry, 149, 1707–1710.
Terr, L. C. (1991). Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. The American Jour-
A)
nal of Psychiatry, 148, 10–20.
Turner, M. A., Kiernan, M. D., McKechanie, A. G., Finch, P. J. C., McManus, F. B.,
P
(A
& Neal, L. A. (2005). Acute military psychiatric casualties from the war in Iraq.
The British Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 476–479. doi:10.1192/bjp.186.6.476
n
Ursano, R. J., Rundell, J. R., Fragala, M. R., Larson, S. G., Jaccard, J. T., Wain,
io
H. J., . . . Beach, B. L. (1996). The prisoner of war. In R. J. Ursano & A. E.
at
Norwood (Eds.), Emotional aftermath of the Persian Gulf War (pp. 443–476).
ci
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
so
van der Kolk, B. A. (2002). The assessment and treatment of complex PTSD. In
As
R. Yehuda (Ed.), Treating trauma survivors with PTSD (pp. 127–156). Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Press.
al
van der Kolk, B. A., Roth, S. H., Pelcovitz, D., Sunday, S., & Spinazzola, J. (2005).
ic
Disorders of extreme stress: The empirical foundation of a complex adaptation
og
to trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18, 389–399. doi:10.1002/jts.20047
ol
Wolfe, J., Erickson, D. J., Sharkansky, E. J., King, D. W., & King, L. A. (1999). Course
ch
and predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder among Gulf War veterans: A
y
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.67.4.520
Zeiss, R. A., & Dickman, H. R. (1989). PTSD 40 years later: Incidence and person-
an
doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198901)45:1<80::AID-JCLP2270450112>3.0.CO;2-V
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
12:01 PM
Am
er
ic
Page 402
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 403
INDEX
A)
Aberrant individuals, 25 across life span, 124–126
P
Abnormal populations, 107 and attachment, 246–249
(A
Abu-Aish, Az-a-Din, 306 behavior genetic studies of, 146–147
Abu Ghraib prison, 15 cognitive factors in, 18–19
n
Abuse, in childhood. See Child continuity of. See Continuity of
io
maltreatment aggression
at
Abusive personalities, 262–265 defined, 3–4, 18, 35, 211
ci
Action tendencies, 316 development of, 156–157
displaced, 224–225
so
Activation, of power system, 76–79
Adaptation, 58–60 forms of, 151–153
As
Adaptationist program, 53 between groups of people, 26–27
Additive genetic influences, 149 heritability of, 176–177
al
ADHD (attention-deficit hyperactivity influence of government actions on,
ic
disorder), 157 27–29
og
Adolescents, 125–126, 169–170 operationalizations of, 144
aggression prevention/intervention power vs., 75
strategies for, 212
ol
reduction of, 29
ch
reinterpretation of, 373–377
externalizing spectrum in, 93–95
and revenge, 221–222
y
Aggression-prone individuals, 29
violent intimate relationships of, 175
er
Aggression Questionnaire, 78
Adoption studies, 144
Aggressive action, 61
Am
Age periods, critical. See Critical age American Theocracy (K. Phillips), 268
periods Amygdala activation, 172, 175, 176
403
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 404
A)
and attachment, 244–246, 260 Arguments, 62, 65
Armageddon, 268
P
behavioral responses to, 59–60,
Arousal, 21
(A
62–63
brain regions for, 109–114 Arousal routes, 36
Arriaga, X. B., 376
n
cognitive model of, 57
io
control of, 262 Assertion behavioral system. See Power
system
at
and cost imposition, 60–61, 63
Assertiveness Questionnaire, 78
ci
and evolutionary biology of conflict,
Attachment, 241–254, 259–270
so
54–57
and abusive personalities, 264–265
and face, 64
in adulthood, 243–244
As
features of, 60–64 and aggression, 246–249
functional, 245 and anger, 244–246
al
and hatred, 322–324 in approach/inhibition theory of
ic
insults as cause of, 62, 64–65 power, 250–251
og
and intent, 61, 63–64 attachment threat, 266–269
in intergroup aggression, 315–316, in children’s psychological
320–326 ol development, 355–356
ch
neurophysiological locality of, 63 dyadic processes in, 265–266
y
109–110 260–261
er
in adolescents, 125–126
activation of, 73
aggressive, 153–155
in combat, 267
11
404 INDEX
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 405
A)
and memory, 357–358 Bartholomew, K., 265
Battlefield violence, 386
P
with primary caregivers, 355–356
Baumeister, R. F., 204, 207
(A
severe and early, 265
Attachment orientation, 396–397 Behavior(s)
aggressive. See Aggressive behavior
n
Attachment relationships, 355–356
io
Attachment security antisocial. See Antisocial behavior
autonomy-inhibiting, 285
at
and attachment styles, 242–243
changes in, 343–344
ci
broaden-and-build cycle in, 243
conditioning of, 132–133
so
and parent–child respectfulness,
disrespectful, 289
283–285
effects of combat stress reaction on,
As
and power, 75–76 387
Attachment style externalizing. See Externalizing
al
in adults, 262–263 behaviors
ic
and attachment theory, 242–243 functional definitions of, 223–224
og
in intimate relationships, 265–266 hyperactivated power-oriented, 77–78
and mortality salience, 269 measurement of, 210
power in, 251 ol
normal ranges of, 107–108
ch
security in, 242–243 of parents, 131–137
y
Bias
Awareness, of inequality, 338, 340, 341 of advantaged group members, 340
C
INDEX 405
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 406
A)
Boden, J. M., 206 and transmission of aggression,
Boivin, M., 151 126–127
P
Boomsma, D. I., 156 Child maltreatment
(A
Borderline personality, 264–265 in children’s psychological develop-
Boundaries, group, 339 ment, 352–358
n
io
Bowlby, J., 71–73, 76, 241–242, and coping strategies, 358
244–245, 260 and externalizing/internalizing
at
Brain activity behaviors, 354
ci
with anger, 63 operationalization of, 359
so
effects of environmental variables and posttraumatic stress disorder,
on, 176 356–357
As
in social information processing, 172 and resilience, 351
Brain anatomy, 106 sexual abuse, 357–358
al
Brain lesions, 107 and social information processing,
ic
Brain regions, 105, 109–114. See also 173–174
og
Prefrontal cortex (PFC) Children
Branden, Nathaniel, 204 aggression continuity in, 125
Brendgen, M., 151, 152
ol
aggression prevention/intervention
ch
Broaden-and-build cycle, 243 strategies for, 212
y
406 INDEX
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 407
A)
Collectivistic cultures, 214 130–131
Columbia County Longitudinal Study, in intergenerational transmission of
P
124, 133 aggression, 133–137
(A
Combat, 385, 390–391 in self-views, 213–214
n
Combat fatigue, 386 Cross-generational continuity, of
io
Combat-induced disorders, 386–392 aggression. See Continuity of
at
Combat stress, 267 aggression
Combat stress reaction (CSR), 386–392 Cross-group friendships, 338–339
ci
Combat veterans. See Veterans CSR (combat stress reaction), 386–392
so
Commitment level, 371–373, 376–377 C-system
As
Commonality-focused contact, in angry rumination, 109–110
338–339, 344 and brain regions, 113
al
Compassionate teachings, 310 control and emotion regulation
Complex posttraumatic stress disorder, mechanisms of, 114
ic
397 in social neuroscience perspective,
og
Compulsive self-reliance, 244 107–108
Conditioning, 132–133 ol
Cues, 55–56
ch
Conduct problems, 169 Cultural approach, 187–188
Conflict resolution, 333 Cultural context, 196–198
y
Contingent respect, 278–282, 289 honor, dignity, and face in, 189–191
20
INDEX 407
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 408
A)
Death Displaced aggression, 224–225
mental structures for denial of, Dispositional influences, 89–99
P
298–301 environmental and situational
(A
reminders of, 304 factors in, 95–97
n
and terror management, 268–269 and externalizing spectrum, 90–95
io
Decentration, 279 genetic research on externalizing
at
Decision making, 114, 305 phenomena, 97–98
Defensive exclusion, 357 trait approach to, 89–90
ci
Delinquents, 248 Disrespectful behavior, 289
so
Democratic societies Disruption, of attachment, 266
As
public support for aggressive Dissociation, 261–262
retaliation in, 318 Dissolution of relationships, 377–378
al
support for military action in, 302 Distributive justice, 281
Dependence, emotional, 379–380 Dixon, J., 342
ic
Deprivation, 337 Dodge, K. A., 354
og
Desensitization, 112 Domestic violence. See Intimate partner
DESNOS (disorder of extreme stress ol violence; Partner aggression
ch
not otherwise specified), 397 Dominance hierarchies, 56–57
Destructive aggression, 246–249 Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex
y
Deterrence (dACC)
Ps
408 INDEX
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 409
A)
individual and group-based, 320 in, 97–98
in intergroup aggression, 316–319 heritability of, 98
P
in intergroup conflict, 315–316 and personality traits, 94–96
(A
negative, 111 psychobiological factors in, 96
and respect, 288–289
n
Externalizing spectrum
io
in support for aggressive retaliation, dispositional influences in, 90–95
317–321
at
genetic research on, 97–98
Emotional decision making, 305 Extraversion, 90
ci
Emotional dependence, 379–380
so
Emotional sentiment, 318–319 Face
Emotion regulation
As
cultures of, 190–194
angry rumination as form of, 110 in CuPS approach, 189–191
in children’s psychological develop-
al
as feature of anger, 64
ment, 353–355
ic
restoration of, 61–62
C-system mechanisms of, 114 Facial expressions, 353–354
og
observational learning processes in, Factor structure (PBSS instrument),
132
in social neuroscience perspective,
ol 79–80
ch
Fairness, 338, 341
114–116
Familial influences, 143–144
y
INDEX 409
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 410
A)
Functional anger, 245 Genetic polymorphisms, 177–178
Goal(s)
P
Functional magnetic resonance imaging
of behavioral systems, 73–74
(A
(fMRI), 108
Functional theories, 66 power as, 75–77, 250–251
n
Fundamental attribution error, 23–24 regulatory, 244
io
Goal selection, 167
at
GABA (gamma-aminobutyric acid), Government actions, 27–29
Greenberg, M., 212
ci
178
Group-based emotions, 320
so
GABRA 2, 178
Gächter, S., 229 Group-based hierarchy
As
Gage, Phineas, 107 advantaged and disadvantaged
Galinsky, A. D., 250 groups in, 335–336
al
GAM. See General aggression model stability of, 338
Group boundaries, 339
ic
Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), 178
og
Gaza War, 306
Gene-by-environment interaction Haberstick, B. C., 156
effects, 178–180 ol
Halperin, E., 324
ch
Handbook of Attachment (J. Cassidy and
General aggression model (GAM), 4,
P. R. Shaver), 265
y
15–30
Harmony, 191, 333–334, 343
Ps
410 INDEX
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 411
A)
Hudziak, J. J., 156 Insults, 62, 64–65
Integrity, 194–196
P
Huesmann, L. R., 128
Intent
(A
Humiliation, 208
Humility, 191 of adversary, 308–309
n
Hyperactivated power-oriented behavior, as feature of anger, 63–64
io
77–78 and forgiveness, 233
at
Hyperactivating strategies, 73, 244 interpretation of, 167
and likelihood of anger, 61
ci
Hyperactivation (power system), 79–84
Intentional action, 61
so
I3 Theory, 35–48 Interaction effects, 43–46
As
components of, 35–36 Interdependence theory, 372–373
future research, directions for, 46–48 Intergenerational aggression, 126–127.
al
impelling forces in, 39–40 See also Continuity of aggression
Intergroup aggression, 315–327
ic
inhibiting forces in, 40–41
anger and hatred in, 315–316,
og
instigating triggers in, 37–39
interaction effects of, 43–46 320–326
main effects in, 41–43 ol
appraisal-based model for, 319–320
ch
attachment insecurity in, 248–249
risk factors, 46–47
emotions and sentiments in,
y
Iacono, W. G., 98
316–319
Ps
INDEX 411
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 412
A)
aggression Knafo, A., 287
abusive men in, 260 Knowledge structures, 19
P
adolescents in, 175 Krueger, R. F., 98
(A
attachment anxiety in, 246 Kusché, C., 212
n
attachment styles in, 262–263
io
expectations in, 373–375 Laboratory studies, 210
at
perceptions of aggression in, Landau, M. J., 302
370–371 Lansford, J. E., 174
ci
respect in, 288 Learning processes, observational,
so
in young adulthood, 170–171 131–132
As
Intrinsic welfare trade ratios, 58 Lebanon Wars
Inventory of Personal Problems, 78–79 First, 388, 390–392
al
IPV. See Intimate partner violence Second, 309, 389
IRT (item response theory) model, Legrand, L. N., 98
ic
91–92 Lerner, J. S., 320
og
Israel Defense Forces (IDF), 385–386 Levinson, A., 248
Israeli experience ol
Lieberman, M. D., 108
ch
attachment orientation of soldiers Life course, aggression across. See
in, 397 Continuity of aggression
y
Love, 288–289
Jacobson, K. C., 152 Lower order traits, 94–95
11
(JUPI), 304–305
yr
412 INDEX
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 413
A)
McGue, M., 98 Narcissism
Medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), 106, in children, 208
P
110, 113 developmental origins of, 213–214
(A
Mediational approach, 188 and ego threat, 43–44
n
Media violence as self-view, 207–209
io
in social neuroscience perspective, Narcissistic Personality Inventory
at
111–112 (NPI), 207
and top-down control mechanisms, Natural selection, 54, 66. See also
ci
115–116 Evolutionary perspective
so
Memory, 351, 357–358 Neale, M. C., 156
As
Men Negative affect, 266, 354
abusive, 260, 264–265 Negative emotional states, 111
al
abusive personality in, 262–263 Negative outcome expectancies, 45
attempts to save face by, 61–62 Neoassociation theory, 4, 111–112
ic
socialization of, 125 Neural alarm system, 115
og
Mental disorders, 90–91 Neural processes, 171–173
Mental processes, 105 ol
Neurobiology, 261–262
ch
Mental representation, 167 Neurophysiology, 63, 261
Middle East conflict, 311 Neuroticism, 90
y
INDEX 413
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 414
A)
Overt narcissists, 208 Personal inputs, 20, 22
Personality traits
P
Palestine Liberation Organization on aggressive behavior, 89–90
(A
(PLO), 298 and externalizing behaviors, 95–96
n
Palestinian rebellion, 16, 297, 389. in externalizing behaviors, 94–95
io
See also Israeli–Palestinian conflict facets of, 90
at
Parameters (power system), 74–77 stability of, 124
Parent–child relationship Person inputs, 36
ci
disorganized attachment in, 261–262 Pérusse, D., 151
so
power of parent in, 284 PFC. See Prefrontal cortex
As
respect in, 282–285 Phenotypes, behavior, 129–130
and teacher–student relationship, Phillips, K., 268
al
286–287 Physical abuse, 133–137, 353–354
transmission of aggression in, Physical aggression
ic
126–127, 133–137 interpretation of, 372
og
Parents and norms, 369–370
aggressive behavior of, 133–137 ol
psychological aggression vs.,
ch
and dysfunctional parenting, 370–371
213–214 tolerance of, in intimate relation-
y
Scale 298–300
PCC (posterior cingulate cortex), war captivity, 392–398
©
414 INDEX
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 415
A)
defined, 75 and victim well-being, 379
Psychological breakdown, 390–391
P
as goal, 250–251
Psychological control, 284
(A
and objectification, 252–253
in parent–child relationship, 284 Psychological development in children,
n
sense of, 75 351–360
io
Power behavioral system. See Power attachment in, 355–356
at
system emotion regulation in, 353–355
and maltreatment, 352–358
ci
Power Behavioral System Scale (PBSS),
memory in, 357–358
so
79–84
Powerless individuals, 78 methodological considerations with,
As
Power-oriented behavior, 77–78 358–359
Power system, 71–85 in pathogenic relational environ-
al
activation and functioning of, ments, 352
ic
77–79 and psychopathology, 356–357
Psychological processes, 128–133
og
and behavioral systems perspective,
Psychological sequelae, 394–395
72–74
hyperactivation and deactivation of, ol
Psychology of revenge, 223
ch
Psychopathology, 91, 356–357
79–84
Psychophysiology, 171–173
y
Public support
Prefrontal cortex (PFC) for aggressive retaliation, 316–317,
Am
INDEX 415
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 416
A)
Reconciliation, 230–232 in I3 theory categories, 46–47
RED (response evaluation and decision), impelling forces as, 39–40
P
167–168 inhibiting forces as, 40–41
(A
Reflective processes, 109–110 instigating triggers as, 37–39
Romantic relationships. See Intimate
n
Regulatory goals, 244
io
Relatedness-inhibiting behaviors, 285 relationships
Rosenberg, S. E., 78
at
Relational aggression, 150–153
Relationships Rumination, angry, 109–110
ci
commitment in, 45, 380 Rusbult, C. E., 376
so
dissolution of, 377–378
Saguy, T., 340, 341
As
expected value of, 233–234
intimate. See Intimate relationships Savin, N. E., 227
Schmitz, S., 156
al
security in, 310–311
Relationship specificity, 170–171 Schools, respect in, 285–288
ic
Relationship value, 231 Schore, A. N., 261
og
Relative deprivation, 337 Scripts
aggressive, 111–112
Religion, 267–268, 310 ol
behavioral, 111
ch
Repeated trauma, 397
in information-processing model, 128
Reputation, 190, 228
y
74–75
Sefton, M., 227
er
416 INDEX
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 417
A)
213–215 Social dominance theory, 336
and instigating triggers, 43–44 Social equality, 334–339
P
low self-esteem in adults, 204–205 Social factors, 125, 206–207
(A
low self-esteem in children, 206–207 Social inequality
n
narcissism, 207–209 and intergroup relations, 334–339
io
Sensitization, 353 research on, 338–343
at
Sentiments Social information processing, 165–180
emotions vs., 318–319 characteristic styles of, 165–166
ci
in intergroup aggression, 316–319 and child maltreatment, 173–174
so
September 11th attacks, 267, 306–307 environmental factors in, 176–179
As
Sequential assessment game, 56 genetic factors in, 176–179
Sequential prisoner’s dilemma game, 226 mechanisms of, 166–168, 179–180
al
Serotonin, 106, 158 neural and psychophysiological
Sex differences, 152 processes in, 171–173
ic
Sexual abuse, 357–358 patterns of, 168–171
og
Shameful experiences, 208 and peer social rejection, 174–175
Shaver, P. R., 243, 248–249, 265 ol
Social interaction theory, 4
ch
Shell shock, 386 Social learning, 4, 16, 111–112
Shields, A., 354–355 Social neuroscience perspective,
y
phisms), 97 390–391
Social attention-holding, 76 Solomon, Z., 388
op
INDEX 417
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 418
A)
Status, 64 Type D (disorganized/disoriented)
Steinberg, L., 282 attachment, 261–262, 355
P
Strength model of self-regulation, 42
(A
Stress, 386–392 Ubeida, Marwan Abu, 15
n
Stress inoculation perspective, 390 Ultimatum game (UG), 227
io
Stressors, 392–394 Unconditional respect, 278–282
at
Stress resolution hypothesis, 390 Universalism, 287
Structural inequality, 335–339 Universality, of anger, 66
ci
Substance-dependence syndromes, 97 Ureno, G., 78
so
Suicidal terrorism, 15, 302 U.S. War on Terrorism, 28
As
Suicide, 266 Utility motives, 304–305, 309
Support, public. See Public support
al
Symbolic immortality, 269 Vagal tone, 246
Syndromes (CuPS approach), 188–189 van Beijsterveldt, C. E., 156
ic
van der Valk, J. C., 156
og
Tausch, N., 340 Vengeance, 232–234
Taylor, S. P., 113 ol
Ventral prefrontal cortex, 106
ch
Teachers, 286–287 Verhulst, F. C., 156
Tedeschi, J., 61 Veterans, 388, 389, 395–396
y
418 INDEX
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 419
A)
in societies, 17 Welfare tradeoff ratios (WTR), 57–60
support for extreme forms of, Well-being, 378–380
P
308–309 Werther, A., 279
(A
in war captivity, 392–393 White, T. L., 250
n
Violence escalation cycle, 23–25 Withholding of benefits, 228–229
io
Violent video games, 112 Women, 125, 263
at
Vitaro, F., 151 Wright, S. C., 342
Vulnerability, 308–309, 390 WTR. See Welfare tradeoff ratios
ci
so
War X- and C-systems model, 107–108
As
aftermath of, 387–392 X-system, 108, 109, 112–114
dichotomizing effects of, 301
al
effects of, on citizens, 27–29 Yamagishi, T., 229
prevalence rates of combat stress Yom Kippur War, 391, 393–394
ic
reaction in, 387 Young, S. E., 156
og
psychological wounds of, 385
public support for, 301–302 ol
Zautra, A. J., 390
ch
undermining of consensus on, 306 Zigler, E., 351
y
Ps
an
ic
er
Am
e
th
by
11
20
©
ht
ig
yr
op
C
INDEX 419
C
op
yr
ig
ht
©
12051-23_Index-rev.qxd
20
11
5/25/10
by
th
e
12:02 PM
Am
er
ic
Page 420
an
Ps
y ch
ol
og
ic
al
As
so
ci
at
io
n
(A
P A)
12051-24_Editor-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 421
PA)
(A
n
io
at
ci
so
As
al
ic
og
ol
y ch
Ps
an
and Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change, and has published
over 200 scholarly journal articles and book chapters. Dr. Shaver’s research
11
and Emotion, and has served on grant review panels for the National Insti-
ig
tutes of Health and the National Science Foundation. He has been executive
yr
both the American Psychological Association and the Association for Psy-
C
421
12051-24_Editor-rev.qxd 5/25/10 12:02 PM Page 422
A)
School of Psychology at the Interdisciplinary Center in Herzliya, Israel. He
has published three books—Human Helplessness: A Coping Perspective;
P
(A
Dynamics of Romantic Love: Attachment, Caregiving, and Sex; and Attachment
in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change—and over 280 scholarly journal
n
articles and book chapters. Dr. Mikulincer’s main research interests are attach-
io
ment theory, terror management theory, personality processes in interpersonal
at
relationships, coping with stress and trauma, grief-related processes, and pro-
ci
social motives and behavior. He is a member of the editorial boards of several
so
scientific journals, including the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
As
Psychological Inquiry, and Personality and Social Psychology Review, and has served
as associate editor of two journals, the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
al
and Personal Relationships. Recently, he was elected to serve as chief editor of
ic
the Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. He is a fellow of the Society
og
for Personality and Social Psychology and the Association for Psychological
ol
Sciences. He received the EMET Prize in Social Science for his contributions
ch
to psychology and the Berscheid–Hatfield Award for Distinguished Mid-Career
y