Garb2005clinical Judgment and Decision Making
Garb2005clinical Judgment and Decision Making
CONTENTS
CLINICAL JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67
METHODOLOGICAL ADVANCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
COGNITIVE PROCESSES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
Cognitive Heuristics and Biases . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70
Diagnosis and Psychology of Categorization . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70
VALIDITY OF JUDGMENTS AND UTILITY OF DECISIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
Description of Personality and Psychopathology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
Diagnosis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
Case Formulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76
Behavioral Prediction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
Treatment Decisions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
THE ROMANTIC AND EMPIRICIST TRADITIONS IN CLINICAL
PSYCHOLOGY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82
∗
The U.S. Government has the right to retain a nonexclusive, royalty-free license in and to
any copyright covering this paper.
1548-5943/05/0427-0067$14.00 67
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
68 GARB
Clinical psychologists are likely to have one of two reactions to the claims made
by Caldwell, depending on whether they are influenced by romantic or empiricist
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
ideas (Wood et al. 2003, pp. 92–94). Romantics are likely to be greatly impressed
and inspired by Caldwell’s accomplishments. Those from the empiricist tradition
are likely to have a more skeptical attitude. Empiricists insist that grand claims be
scientifically tested. Their point of view is exemplified by a new journal, The Scien-
tific Review of Mental Health Practice, which is devoted to the objective investiga-
tion of controversial and unorthodox claims in clinical psychology, psychiatry, and
social work. The distinction between romantics and empiricists seems especially
apt, given the title of Caldwell’s address: “My Love Affair with an Instrument.”
In this article, research on clinical judgment and its relationship to the romantic
and empiricist traditions in clinical psychology is examined. First, the following
topics are covered: (a) methodological advances in studying the validity of descrip-
tions of personality and psychopathology, (b) the cognitive processes of clinicians,
and (c) the validity of clinical judgments and the utility of treatment decisions.
METHODOLOGICAL ADVANCES
Important research has been conducted on clinical judgment and decision making,
but it has focused on issues that are relatively easy to address. For example, some
studies have described interrater reliability. Other studies have been conducted to
determine if judgments based on a small amount of information (e.g., results from
a single test) agree with results from a large set of information (e.g., judgments
based on an interview and history information).
Some important questions seldom have been addressed. For example, when a
clinical psychologist describes a client’s personality, it is difficult to know how
to determine if the psychologist is correct. The description can be compared to
other fallible indicators (e.g., self-report ratings, peer ratings), but it is also dif-
ficult to determine the validity of these indicators. Similarly, when assessing a
child, a clinician can systematically collect information from the child, parents,
teachers, and peers. However, what if the information from these different sources
is contradictory, as is often the case?
Two new approaches to evaluating the validity of descriptions of personality
and psychopathology can be described. One approach is to have clients make rat-
ings every day. For example, in a superb study, Wu & Clark (2003) constructed
a behavior record form to measure daily activities. Participants were instructed to
indicate whether they had performed a particular behavior on a given day (e.g.,
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
got into an argument, lost their temper). This particular measure was developed to
provide measurements of aggression, impulsivity, and exhibitionism. The advan-
tage of having participants make self-ratings every day is that they do not have to
rely on memory. Also, by making ratings for specific behaviors, they do not have
to make judgments that require more than a low degree of inference. If we were
interested in learning whether psychologists in clinical practice can report whether
their clients are aggressive, impulsive, or exhibitionistic, then it would be of value
to compare their judgments to results from this behavior record.
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
more than one source. For example, children can make self-ratings and can be
described by parents, teachers, and peers. When these descriptions are not in
agreement, it may be unclear which description is valid.
According to Kraemer et al. (2003), an informant’s report reflects the influences
of (a) the actual characteristics that we want to measure (e.g., traits, symptoms,
competencies), (b) the context in which the subject is observed, (c) the perspec-
tive of the informant, and (d) the error of measurement. Context refers to the
setting/place and circumstance in which the rater knows the person being rated
(e.g., home versus outside of home). Perspective refers to characteristics of the
informant that influence his or her assessment of the trait (e.g., someone who is
making self-ratings will have a different perspective than will other people). Given
this framework, our goal is to obtain a measure of a trait that is relatively free from
variance attributable to context (or setting) and perspective (biases of the infor-
mants). To do this, one must gather ratings from multiple informants who know
the client in different contexts and who have differing perspectives. For example,
ratings in the context of school could be made by a teacher and a child. Ratings in
the context of home could be made by a parent and the child. Perspective would
vary as a function of self (child) and other (teacher, parent). All of the trait ratings
could then be analyzed using a principal components analysis. If one is successful,
the principal components analysis will yield separate factors for trait(s), context,
and perspective. If this occurs, then one will have obtained a measure of trait(s)
that is closer to the gold standard than the individual ratings made by the infor-
mants because one will have removed variance due to context and perspective. This
would represent a significant advancement in being able to evaluate the validity of
a clinician’s ratings of traits and symptoms.
COGNITIVE PROCESSES
Describing the cognitive processes of clinical psychologists could prove beneficial
for improving training and clinical practice. It would be helpful if we could compare
the cognitive processes of trainees with those of expert clinicians. This would allow
supervisors to give empirically guided feedback to the students and trainees, e.g.,
on what information to attend to and what questions to ask. Remarkably, this rarely
has been done. In fact, little is know about individual differences among clinicians.
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
70 GARB
five outstanding but complex experiments, Kim & Ahn (2002) had psychologists
and psychology graduate students make ratings for a series of tasks. Some of
the ratings took from 2.5 to 6 hours to complete. For one of the tasks, the DSM
criteria and the associated symptoms listed in the DSM manual were presented
to judges. Participants were to draw arrows indicating cause-and-effect relations
between any of the symptoms that they felt were causally connected. They were also
instructed to describe the strength of the postulated causal relations. Psychologists
and psychology graduate students provided detailed causal explanations. Most of
these causal theories were quite complex. A composite of the drawings made by
psychologists and psychology graduate students for a specific phobia is presented
in Figure 1. Notice that in this example, the DSM criterion “Person recognizes
fear as excessive” was not perceived as being part of a causal relation with other
symptoms. Kim & Ahn then gave the judges additional tasks. They were able to
determine that the criteria and symptoms that had been described as forming part
of a causal relation were weighed more heavily than were other criteria when they
made diagnoses. For example, for the diagnosis of specific phobia, the criterion
“Person recognizes fear as excessive” was given a mean diagnostic importance
rating of only 75.5; ratings for the other DSM criteria ranged from 87.3 to 96.3.
This is of interest because when using the DSM, clinicians are supposed to weigh
each criterion equally.
Other results reported by Kim & Ahn (2002) are also of interest. For one
of the tasks, psychologists and psychology graduate students were instructed to
read a set of case studies. When they were later asked about the clients, they
recalled causally central symptoms more often than causally peripheral symptoms
and isolated symptoms. In fact, the results revealed a bias to falsely recognize
symptoms that were causally central to clinicians’ theories of different disorders.
72 GARB
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
In reviewing the literature, it is clear that questions about the validity of de-
scriptions of personality and psychopathology are usually sidestepped or answered
indirectly. Remarkably, in these studies, clinicians virtually never make their judg-
ments based on all of the information that is usually available in clinical practice.
This is not true of studies on diagnosis, prediction of behavior, and treatment
decision making. Important research has been conducted on the description of
personality and psychopathology, but it has focused on answering questions that
are easily answered.
A popular approach to studying clinical judgment in this area is to examine
interrater reliability: One can see if different clinicians make similar ratings when
evaluating the same set of clients. Overall, interrater reliability has varied widely
for describing personality traits, but has often been good for describing psychiatric
symptoms (Garb 1998, pp. 10–14).
Recent work on interrater reliability has focused on the description of defense
mechanisms. Overall, results from two studies suggest that psychologists should
not be describing defense mechanisms unless they receive extensive training. In
both studies, clinicians made ratings using the Defense Functioning Scale, a scale
that was introduced for further study in the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric As-
sociation 1994). Perry et al. (1998, p. 56) concluded, “Our findings indicate that
the defense axis is a feasible, acceptably reliable, and nonredundant addition to
DSM-IV,” but their results can also be interpreted as indicating that there is little
support for adding a defense mechanism axis to the next edition of the DSM. For
example, they reported that median kappa reliabilities were 0.42 for describing
individual defenses. A kappa value less than 0.40 typically indicates that interrater
reliability is poor. A median kappa value of 0.42 suggests that almost half of the
kappa reliabilities were below 0.40, so interrater reliability was presumably poor
in many cases. In the other study (Hilsenroth et al. 2003), positive results were
obtained: The mean intraclass correlation coefficient for ratings of six defense
levels was 0.59. However, the study was not conducted under typical clinical con-
ditions: ratings were made after a 2-hour semistructured clinical interview and a
1- to 1.5-hour interpretive/feedback interview.
Studies on validity have also been conducted. Typically, one indicator of a
construct has been related to another indicator of the construct (e.g., ratings based
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
74 GARB
ies, people have not been accurate at this task (DePaulo 1994). However, by coding
videotapes using the Facial Action Coding System (Ekman & Friesen 1978), one
can detect some individuals who are lying (Frank & Ekman 1997). Furthermore,
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
judges have been able to detect lying after viewing videotapes of interviews under
the following condition: when the experimenters select videotapes that differ in the
expression of emotion for subjects who lied and subjects who told the truth (Ekman
& O’Sullivan 1991, Ekman et al. 1999). Thus, by carefully selecting videotapes to
show clinicians, one can increase the likelihood that the clinicians’ ratings will be
valid. This result is of theoretical interest: When individuals show facial muscular
movement that indicates they are lying, clinicians’ ratings will have modest va-
lidity. However, the result is of limited clinical significance for two reasons. First,
individuals who lie do not always show facial muscular movement that is associ-
ated with lying. Second, considering that the chance level of accuracy was 50%, a
majority of the psychologists obtained only a modest level of accuracy. For exam-
ple, for “regular” clinical psychologists, 4% achieved 0%–30% levels of accuracy,
52% achieved 40%–60% accuracy, and 44% achieved 70%–100% accuracy.
Diagnosis
Acceptable levels of interrater reliability have been reported for psychodiagnoses in
field trials for both DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association 1980, pp. 470–471)
and for the tenth revision of the International Classification of Diseases (Sartorius
et al. 1993, 1995). However, it is likely that these results were obtained because
the clinicians participating in the field trials were familiar with and adhered to
diagnostic criteria. In clinical practice, many mental health professionals frequently
do not adhere to diagnostic criteria (e.g., Blashfield & Herkov 1996, Davis et al.
1993, Ford & Widiger 1989, Morey & Ochoa 1989); therefore, one might expect
that diagnoses made in routine clinical practice will be dissimilar to diagnoses
based on semistructured interviews. Semistructured interviews are used to ensure
that diagnoses are based on specific criteria and rules. The scientific literature
reveals that agreement is generally poor between diagnoses made in routine clinical
practice and diagnoses based on semistructured interviews (Shear et al. 2000,
Strakowski et al. 1997; also see Garb 1998, pp. 53–54).
Several factors indicate that semistructured interviews are more valid than diag-
noses made in clinical practice. First, clinicians do not always ask about important
symptoms. In one study (Miller et al. 2001), clinicians evaluated only about 50%
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
of the key criteria that were assessed using semistructured interviews. Of course,
some clinicians will evaluate all of the relevant criteria, but the research suggests
that many do not. Second, it can be noted that interrater reliability is better for
semistructured interview diagnoses than for clinical diagnoses. Finally, agreement
with diagnoses made by expert clinicians has been better for diagnoses based
on semistructured interviews than for diagnoses made in routine clinical practice
(Basco et al. 2000, Miller et al. 2001). In response to these findings, Widiger &
Samuel (2004) recommended that clinicians administer a self-report inventory to
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
76 GARB
the two sets of ratings are related to race, gender, or other variables, then one
can conclude that one set of diagnoses is biased. Bias has been observed for
diagnosis. For example, for diagnoses made in clinical practice and diagnoses
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Case Formulation
In addition to describing personality and psychopathology and making diagnoses,
psychologists also try to understand the causes of their clients’ behaviors and
symptoms. As already mentioned, even when they make other types of judgments
(e.g., diagnoses), they frequently consider causal factors (Kim & Ahn 2002). Thus,
case formulation is a key task for clinical psychologists—one that is likely to affect
how they perform on other tasks.
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
validity of case formulations. The research that has been conducted suggests
that the validity of case formulations is often poor (Garb 1998, p. 100).
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
1
However, interrater reliability was sometimes fair when conditions did not resemble clinical
practice, e.g., when ratings were averaged across clinicians.
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
78 GARB
whether they had engaged in behaviors related to conduct disorder during the
previous year. They completed the forms annually, starting in some instances at
age 12 and in other instances at age 14, and ending at age 15. Retrospective ratings
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
were made at age 19, at which time they were asked what criterion behaviors
they had engaged in prior to age 15. Participants who met antisocial criteria at
age 19 overreported having had behavior problems related to conduct disorder,
whereas participants who experienced a decline in antisocial behavior by age 19
underreported having had behavior problems related to conduct disorder in the
past. Thus, memories were distorted to conform to present behavior.
To increase the likelihood of making valid causal judgments, psychologists
should be more systematic in collecting history data. A well-replicated finding
is that many clinicians do not regularly ask clients about important events, e.g.,
if they have ever been sexually or physically abused (Garb 1998, pp. 88–89).
Similarly, Widiger & Clark (2000) implied that clinicians do not rigorously inquire
about the course of a client’s psychopathology. They recommended that DSM-V
require that clinicians systematically learn about the “life-span history of a patient’s
symptomatology . . . by recording, for example, age of onset, lifetime history of
disorders, and their longitudinal course” (p. 956). Finally, to decrease reliance
on retrospective reports of specific behaviors, clients can carry small handheld
electronic diaries that will allow for describing those behaviors in real-time and in
real-world contexts (Shiffman et al. 2002).
In summary, psychologists should be cautious when explaining behavior. Even
case formulations that seem reasonable may be incorrect. Psychologists should
also become more systematic and rigorous in collecting history data.
Behavioral Prediction
Behavioral prediction is one of the most exciting areas of clinical judgment. Al-
though progress has been slow for improving the validity of predictions for some
tasks (e.g., prediction of course of mental disorder, prediction of suicide), exciting
advances have been made for the prediction of violence and the prediction of re-
cidivism of sexual offenses. For these latter two tasks, new assessment instruments
are being adopted, and statistical prediction rules are having their greatest impact.
(Monahan 1981), results from subsequent studies indicate that predictions of vi-
olence made by clinicians are often valid (Mossman 1994). For example, in a
well-known study on the long-term (six-month) prediction of violence (Lidz et al.
1993), clinicians made correct predictions for 58% of the patients who did not
become violent and 60% of those who did.
Although mental health professionals are able to predict violence with moder-
ate validity, race bias and gender bias have been documented. Black psychiatric
inpatients and black prison inmates often are predicted to be more violent than
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
are white psychiatric inpatients and white prison inmates, even when race is not
significantly related to the occurrence of violence (Garb 1998, pp. 113–114). This
finding continues to be replicated. In a recent study (Hoptman et al. 1999), psy-
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
80 GARB
PREDICTION OF SEXUAL CRIME Advances also have occurred for the prediction
of sexual recidivism. Recidivism rates for criminals committing sex offenses range
from about 35% to 55% (Hanson et al. 2003). In recent years, research on the actu-
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
arial prediction of sexual recidivism has increased dramatically: Only one finding
was identified in a review of the literature published before 1996 (Hanson &
Bussiere 1998), whereas more than 50 findings have been subsequently described
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
(Hanson et al. 2003). Actuarial measures that have been used to predict sexual
recidivism include the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide (VRAG; Harris et al. 1993),
the Sex Offender Risk Appraisal Guide (SORAG; Quinsey et al. 1998), the Rapid
Risk Assessment for Sex Offender Recidivism (RRASOR; Hanson 1997), and the
Static-99 (Hanson & Thornton 1999). Positive results have been obtained (Hanson
et al. 2003). When results from different studies have been pooled, the accuracy
of actuarial predictions has been statistically significant, medium in effect size
(d = 0.68, 95% confidence interval of 0.62 to 0.42), and significantly more accu-
rate than unstructured clinical predictions (d = 0.28, 95% confidence interval of
0.14 to 0.42). Thus, compared to clinical prediction, the use of actuarial scales is
a major improvement for predicting sexual recidivism.
Treatment Decisions
Treatment decisions often are based on interviews, medical records, and, in some
cases, test results. Research topics include (a) the utility of assessment instruments,
(b) the utility of standardized versus tailored treatment plans, (c) the appropriate-
ness of evaluations of competency to consent to treatment, and (d) the use of
evidence-based recommendations for treatment.
Nelson-Gray (2003) described a method for studying the utility of assessment
instruments. In the following example, she related using the method to evaluate a
semistructured interview:
If treatment was more successful for individuals whose diagnosis had been
established by a set of assessment devices that included the semistructured
interview than by a set of assessment devices that excluded the semistructured
interview, then . . . the treatment utility of the semistructured interview would
be demonstrated (p. 524).
This method is rarely used, so little is directly known about the utility of as-
sessment instruments such as semistructured interviews and psychological tests
(Hayes et al. 1987, Meehl 1959, Nelson-Gray 2003).
A treatment plan that is formulated on an individual basis is not necessarily
better than a standardized one. In one study (Schulte et al. 1992), 120 phobic
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
clients were randomly assigned to one of three groups: (a) a control group, (b) a
standardized therapy group (exposure in vivo), and (c) a group receiving individ-
ually planned treatment. For the clients receiving individually planned treatment,
therapists were allowed to use all of the therapeutic methods commonly employed
in behavior therapy and cognitive therapy. Standardized treatment proved to be the
most successful, which suggests that in some instances the utility of standardized
treatment is greater than the utility of tailored treatment plans.
The most vigorous area of research on clinical decision making in the past six
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
to all of the necessary legal criteria when seeking civil commitments for psychiatric
hospitalizations (e.g., Bagby et al. 1991, Garb 1998), a recent study raises questions
about how they decide if a patient is competent to agree to electroconvulsive therapy
(Kitamura & Kitamura 2000). In this study, 176 members of the Japanese Society
of Psychiatry and Neurology read transcripts of competency interviews for five
patients who were recommended to undergo electroconvulsive therapy because
drugs had failed to improve their mental status. When indicating if a patient was
competent to make this treatment decision, interrater reliability among clinicians
was poor. The intraclass correlation coefficient was only 0.31.
The treatment of mental disorders may be transformed by the adoption of
evidence-based practice recommendations. For example, the Agency for Health
Care Policy and Research and the National Institute of Mental Health established
the Schizophrenia Patient Outcomes Research Team (PORT) to develop and dis-
seminate recommendations for treating schizophrenia based on scientific evidence.
Their recommendations addressed the use of antipsychotic agents as well as the use
of psychological, family, and other interventions (Lehman et al. 1998b). With the
publication of treatment recommendations, investigators have been able to evaluate
treatment decisions made by mental health professionals to learn if they are consis-
tent with evidence-based practice. In general, results indicate that implementation
of the treatment recommendations would transform clinical practice.
For the treatment of schizophrenia, mental health professionals frequently de-
viate from recommendations for evidence-based practice. For example, the PORT
(Lehman et al. 1998a) surveyed a stratified random sample of 719 individuals
diagnosed with schizophrenia in inpatient and outpatient settings. Treatment was
in conformance with the treatment recommendations less than half of the time,
although it was better for pharmacological than for psychosocial treatments. Treat-
ment recommendations were followed less often for minority patients. For exam-
ple, dosage of antipsychotic medication for an acute episode should be in the
range of 300–1000 chlorpromazine equivalents per day for a minimum of six
weeks. Lehman et al. (1998a) found that only 62.4% of inpatients received the
recommended dosage at discharge. Minority patients were more likely to be on
a high dose (>1000 chlorpromazine equivalents) than were Caucasian patients
(27.4% and 15.9%, respectively).
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
82 GARB
Since the publication of the PORT findings and recommendations, a large num-
ber of studies have been conducted to determine how well mental health profes-
sionals follow evidence-based practice when treating schizophrenia. In general,
half of the patients with schizophrenia frequently do not receive antipsychotic
medications within recommended dosages; black patients frequently receive ex-
cessively high dosages of antipsychotic medicine, and they are less likely to be
tried on atypical psychotics (Chen et al. 2000, Covell et al. 2002, Daumit et al.
2003, Dickey et al. 2003, DosReis et al. 2002, Herbeck et al. 2004, Kreyenbuhl
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
et al. 2003, Owen et al. 2001, Sohler et al. 2003, Valenstein et al. 2001, Valenti
et al. 2003, Walkup et al. 2000; also see Botts et al. 2003, Buchanan et al. 2002,
Remington et al. 2001, Weissman 2002, Woods et al. 2003). Conformance with
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
I even use psychological tests whose validity is in question. For example, I will
administer the Draw-A-Person Test (Machover 1949). Again, I make specific
predictions. Without so doing, I would not use this test because of the weak
support for its validity (Silver 2001, p. 1009).
Just as Caldwell (2004) referred to clinical experience to validate his use of a test,
Silver does the same. The problem with Silver’s approach is that a psychologist’s
prediction of test scores will sometimes be correct by chance, not because the
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
psychologist’s overall impression of the client is accurate. When this occurs, the
psychologist will be misled into believing that his impression of the client and his
use of the test have been validated.
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
84 GARB
LITERATURE CITED
Ambady N, Rosenthal R. 1992. Thin slices of chotic pharmacy in the ambulatory care set-
expressive behavior as predictors of interper- ting, 1993–2000. Psychiatr. Serv. 54:1086
sonal consequences: a meta-analysis. Psy- Buchanan RW, Kreyenbuhl J, Zito JM, Lehman
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
orders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. As- functional outcome. Schizophr. Bull. 28:63–
soc. 3rd ed. 73
American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diag- Burkett BG, Whitley G. 1998. Stolen Valor:
nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Dis- How the Vietnam Generation Was Robbed
orders. Washington, DC: Am. Psychiatr. As- of Its Heroes and Its History. Dallas, TX:
soc. 4th ed. Verity
Arkes HR. 1981. Impediments to accurate clin- Caldwell AB. 2004. My love affair with an in-
ical judgment and possible ways to mini- strument. J. Personal. Assess. 82:4–10
mize their impact. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. Cantor N, Smith EE, French R, Mezzich
49:323–30 J. 1980. Psychiatric diagnosis as proto-
Bagby RM, Thompson JS, Dickens SE, No- type categorization. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 89:
hara M. 1991. Decision making in psychiatric 181–93
civil commitment: an experimental analysis. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP. 1969. Illusory cor-
Am. J. Psychiatry 148:28–33 relation as an obstacle to the use of valid
Basco MR, Bostic JQ, Davies D, Rush AJ, psychodiagnostic signs. J. Abnorm. Psychol.
Witte B, et al. 2000. Methods to improve 74:271–80
diagnostic accuracy in a community mental Chen RS, Nadkarni PM, Levin FL, Miller
health setting. Am. J. Psychiatry 157:1599– PL, Erdos J, et al. 2000. Using a computer
605 database to monitor compliance with phar-
Baumeister RF, Campbell JD, Krueger JI, Vohs macotherapeutic guidelines for schizophre-
KD. 2003. Does high self-esteem cause bet- nia. Psychiatr. Serv. 51:791–94
ter performance, interpersonal success, hap- Cooper JE, Kendell RE, Gurland BJ, Sharpe L,
piness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychol. Sci. Copeland JRM, et al. 1972. Psychiatric Di-
Public Interest 4:1–44 agnosis in New York and London. London:
Blashfield RK, Herkov MJ. 1996. Investigating Oxford Univ. Press
clinician adherence to diagnosis by criteria: Costa PT, Widiger TA. 2002. Personality Dis-
a replication of Morey and Ochoa 1989. J. orders and the Five-Factor Model of Person-
Personal. Disord. 10:219–28 ality. Washington, DC: Am. Psychol. Assoc.
Blashfield RK, Sprock J, Haymaker D, Hodgin 2nd ed.
J. 1989. The family resemblance hypothesis Covell NH, Jackson CT, Evans AC, Essock SM.
applied to psychiatric classification. J. Nerv. 2002. Antipsychotic prescribing practices in
Ment. Dis. 177:492–97 Connecticut’s public mental health system:
Blashfield RK, Sprock J, Pinkson K, Hodgin rates of changing medications and prescrib-
J. 1985. Exemplar prototypes of personal- ing styles. Schizophr. Bull. 28:17–29
ity disorder diagnoses. Compr. Psychiatry Daleiden EL, Chorpita BF, Kollins SH, Drab-
26:11–21 man RS. 1999. Factors affecting the reliabil-
Botts S, Hines H, Littrell R. 2003. Antipsy- ity of clinical judgments about the function
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
Weighting criteria in the diagnosis of person- Garb HN. 1996. The representativeness and
ality disorder: a demonstration. J. Abnorm. past-behavior heuristics in clinical judgment.
Psychol. 102:319–22 Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 27:272–77
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Dawes RM. 1986. Representative thinking in Garb HN. 1998. Studying the Clinician: Judg-
clinical judgment. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 6:425– ment Research and Psychological Assess-
41 ment. Washington, DC: Am. Psychol. Assoc.
DePaulo BM. 1994. Spotting lies: Can humans Garb HN. 2000. Computers will become in-
learn to do better? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. creasingly important for psychological as-
3:83–86 sessment: not that there’s anything wrong
Dickey B, Normand S-LT, Hermann RC, Eisen with that? Psychol. Assess. 12:31–39
SV, Cortes DE, et al. 2003. Guideline rec- Garb HN. 2003. Incremental validity and the as-
ommendations for treatment of schizophre- sessment of psychopathology in adults. Psy-
nia: the impact of managed care. Arch. Gen. chol. Assess. 15:508–20
Psychiatry 60:340–48 Garb HN, Wood JM, Lilienfeld SO, Nezworski
DosReis S, Zito JM, Buchanan RW, Lehman MT. 2005. Roots of the Rorschach contro-
AF. 2002. Antipsychotic dosing and concur- versy. Clin. Psychol. Rev. In press
rent psychotropic treatments for Medicaid- Grove WM, Zald DH, Lebow BS, Snitz BE,
insured individuals with schizophrenia. Nelson C. 2000. Clinical versus mechanical
Schizophr. Bull. 28:607–17 prediction: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Assess.
Ekman P, Friesen WV. 1978. The Facial Ac- 12:19–30
tion Coding System. Palo Alto, CA: Consult. Hansen SS, Munk-Jorgensen P, Guldbaek B,
Psychol. Press Solgard T, Lauszus KS, et al. 2000. Psy-
Ekman P, O’Sullivan M. 1991. Who can catch choactive substance use diagnoses among
a liar? Am. Psychol. 46:913–20 psychiatric in-patients. Acta Psychiatr.
Ekman P, O’Sullivan M, Frank MG. 1999. Scand. 102:432–38
A few can catch a liar. Psychol. Sci. 10: Hanson RK. 1997. The Development of a Brief
263–66 Actuarial Scale for Sexual Offense Recidi-
Elbogen EB, Williams AL, Kim D, Tomkins vism. Ottawa, Ont.: Dept. Solicitor Gen.
AJ, Scalora MJ. 2001. Gender and per- Hanson RK, Bussiere MT. 1998. Predicting re-
ceptions of dangerousness in civil psychi- lapse: a meta-analysis of sexual offender re-
atric patients. Leg. Criminol. Psychol. 6: cidivism studies. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.
215–28 66:348–62
Evans DL, Herbert JD, Nelson-Gray RO, Gau- Hanson RK, Morton KE, Harris AJR. 2003.
diano BA. 2002. Determinants of diagnos- Sexual offender recidivism risk: what we
tic prototypicality judgments of the per- know and what we need to know. Ann. NY
sonality disorders. J. Personal. Disord. 16: Acad. Sci. 989:154–66
95–106 Hanson RK, Thornton D. 1999. Static-99: Im-
Ford MR, Widiger TA. 1989. Sex bias in the proving Actuarial Risk Assessments for Sex
diagnosis of histrionic and antisocial per- Offenders. Ottawa, Ont.: Dept. Solicitor Gen.
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
86 GARB
Hare RD. 1991. The Hare Psychopathy Check- A Synthesis, ed. LY Abramson, pp. 295–365.
list-Revised (PCL-R). Toronto, Ont.: Multi- New York: Guilford
Health Syst. Kihlstrom JF. 1999. The psychological uncon-
Harris GT, Rice ME, Quinsey VL. 1993. Vi- scious. In Handbook of Personality, ed. LR
olent recidivism of mentally disordered of- Pervin, O John, pp. 424–42. New York: Guil-
fenders: the development of a statistical ford. 2nd ed.
prediction instrument. Crim. Justice Behav. Kim NS, Ahn W. 2002. Clinical psychologists’
20:315–35 theory-based representations of mental dis-
Hathaway SR, McKinley JC. 1943. The Min- orders predict their diagnostic reasoning and
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
treatment utility of assessment: a functional stance use disorders among inpatients with
approach to evaluating assessment quality. schizophrenia. Psychiatr. Serv. 49:82–85
Am. Psychol. 42:963–74 Kitamura T, Kitamura F. 2000. Reliability
Herbeck DM, West JC, Ruditis I, Duffy FF, of clinical judgment of patients’ compe-
Fitek DJ, et al. 2004. Variations in use of tency to give informed consent: a case vi-
second-generation antipsychotic medication gnette study. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 54:
by race among adult psychiatric patients. 245–47
Psychiatr. Serv. 55:677–84 Kraemer HC, Measelle JR, Ablow JC, Essex
Hilsenroth MJ, Callahan KL, Eudell EM. 2003. MJ, Boyce WT, Kupfer DJ. 2003. A new
Further reliability, convergent and discrimi- approach to integrating data from multiple
nant validity of overall defensive function- informants in psychiatric assessment and re-
ing. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 191:730–37 search: mixing and matching contexts and
Hoptman MJ, Yates KF, Patalinjug MB, Wack perspectives. Am. J. Psychiatry 160:1566–
RC, Convit A. 1999. Clinical prediction of 77
assaultive behavior among male psychiatric Kreyenbuhl J, Zito JM, Buchanan RW, Soeken
patients at a maximum-security forensic fa- KL, Lehman AF. 2003. Racial dispar-
cility. Psychiatr. Serv. 50:1461–66 ity in the pharmacological management of
Hunsley J, Bailey JM. 1999. The clinical utility schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 29:183–93
of the Rorschach: unfulfilled promises and an Lally SJ. 2003. What tests are acceptable for use
uncertain future. Psychol. Assess. 11:266–77 in forensic evaluations? A survey of experts.
Jopp DA, Keys CB. 2001. Diagnostic over- Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract. 34:491–98
shadowing reviewed and reconsidered. Am. LeFever GB, Arcona AP, Antonuccio DO.
J. Ment. Retard. 106:416–33 2003. ADHD among schoolchildren:
Kahneman D. 2003. A perspective on judg- evidence of overdiagnosis and overuse of
ment and choice: mapping bounded rational- medication. Sci. Rev. Ment. Health Pract. 2:
ity. Am. Psychol. 58:697–720 49–60
Kamphaus RW. 1998. Intelligence test inter- Lehman AF, Steinwachs DM, Co-Investigators
pretation: acting in the absence of evidence. of the PORT Project. 1998a. Patterns of usual
In WISC-III Clinical Use and Interpreta- care for schizophrenia: initial results from the
tion: Scientist-Practitioner Perspectives, ed. Schizophrenia Patient Outcomes Research
A Prifitera, DH Saklofske, pp. 39–57. New Team (PORT) client survey. Schizophr. Bull.
York: Academic 24:11–20
Kayne NY, Alloy LB. 1988. Clinician and pa- Lehman AF, Steinwachs DM, Co-Investigators
tient as aberrant actuaries: expectation-based of the PORT Project. 1998b. Translating
distortions in assessment of covariation. In research into practice: the Schizophrenia
Social Cognition and Clinical Psychology: Patient Outcomes Research Team (PORT)
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
88 GARB
Sartorius N, Ustun TB, Korten A, Cooper JE, tion of specialization? Prof. Psychol. Res.
van Drimmelen J. 1995. Progress toward Pract. 34:225–32
achieving a common language in psychiatry. Turk DC, Salovey P, eds. 1988. Reasoning, In-
II: Results from the international field trials ference, and Judgment in Clinical Psychol-
of the ICD-10 diagnostic criteria for research ogy. New York: Free Press
for mental and behavioral disorders. Am. J. Tversky A, Kahneman D. 1974. Judgment un-
Psychiatry 152:1427–37 der uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Sci-
Schulte D, Kunzel R, Pepping G, Schulte- ence 185:1124–31
Bahrenberg T. 1992. Tailor-made versus Valenstein M, Copeland L, Owen R, Blow F,
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
standardized therapy of phobic patients. Adv. Visnic S. 2001. Delays in adopting evid-
Behav. Res. Ther. 14:67–92 ence-based dosages of conventional antipsy-
Shear MK, Greeno C, Kang J, Ludewig D, chotics. Psychiatr. Serv. 52:1242–44
Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005.1:67-89. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org
Frank E, et al. 2000. Diagnosis of nonpsy- Valenti AM, Narendran R, Pristach CA. 2003.
chotic patients in community clinics. Am. J. Who are patients on conventional antipsy-
Psychiatry 157:581–87 chotics? Schizophr. Bull. 29:195–99
Shedler J, Westen D. 2004. Refining person- Wakefield JC, Kirk SA, Pottick KJ, Hsieh D.
ality disorder diagnosis: integrating science 1999. Disorder attribution and clinical judg-
and practice. Am. J. Psychiatry 161:1350– ment in the assessment of adolescent antiso-
65 cial behavior. Soc. Work Res. 23:227–38
Shiffman S, Gwaltney CJ, Balabanis MH, Liu Walkup JT, McAlpine DD, Olfson M, Labay
KS, Paty JA, et al. 2002. Immediate an- LE, Boyer C, et al. 2000. Patients with
tecedents of cigarette smoking: an analysis schizophrenia at risk for excessive antipsy-
from ecological momentary assessment. J. chotic dosing. J. Clin. Psychiatry 61:344–
Abnorm. Psychol. 111:531–45 48
Silver RJ. 2001. Practicing professional psy- Watkins MW. 2003. IQ subtest analysis: clin-
chology. Am. Psychol. 56:1008–14 ical acumen or clinical illusion? Sci. Rev.
Slovic P, Finucane M, Peters E, MacGregor Ment. Health Pract. 2:118–41
DG. 2002. The affect heuristic. In Heuris- Webster CD, Douglas KS, Eaves D, Hart SD.
tics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive 1998. The HCR-20: Assessing Risk for Vio-
Judgment, ed. T Gilovich, D Griffin, D Kah- lence (Version 2). Odessa, FL: Psychol. As-
neman, pp. 397–420. Cambridge, UK: Cam- sess. Resourc.
bridge Univ. Press Wedding D, Faust D. 1989. Clinical judgment
Sohler NL, Walkup J, McAlpine D, Boyer and decision making in neuropsychology.
C, Olfson M. 2003. Antipsychotic dosage Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 4:233–65
at hospital discharge and outcomes among Weissman EM. 2002. Antipsychotic prescrib-
persons with schizophrenia. Psychiatr. Serv. ing practices in the Veterans Healthcare
54:1258–63 Administration–New York metropolitan re-
Spitzer RL. 1983. Psychiatric diagnosis: Are gion. Schizophr. Bull. 28:31–42
clinicians still necessary? Compr. Psychiatry Westen D. 1997. Divergences between clinical
24:399–411 and research methods for assessing person-
Strakowski SM, Hawkins JM, Keck PE, McEl- ality disorders: implications for research and
roy SL, West SA, et al. 1997. The effects of the evolution of Axis II. Am. J. Psychiatry
race and information variance on disagree- 154:895–903
ment between psychiatric emergency service Whaley AL. 2001. Cultural mistrust and the
and research diagnoses in first-episode psy- clinical diagnosis of paranoid schizophre-
chosis. J. Clin. Psychiatry 58:457–63 nia in African American patients. J. Psy-
Tolman AO, Mullendore KB. 2003. Risk evalu- chopathol. Behav. Assess. 23:93–100
ations for the courts: Is service quality a func- Widiger TA. 2002. Personality disorders. In
26 Feb 2005 19:22 AR AR240-CP01-03.tex XMLPublishSM (2004/02/24) P1: JRX
Handbook of Assessment and Treatment versial Inkblot Test. San Francisco: Jossey-
Planning for Psychological Disorders, ed. Bass
MM Antony, DH Barlow, pp. 453–80. New Woods SW, Sullivan MC, Neuse EC, Diaz E,
York: Guilford Baker CB, et al. 2003. Racial and ethnic ef-
Widiger TA, Clark LA. 2000. Toward DSM- fects on antipsychotic prescribing practices
V and the classification of psychopathology. in a community mental health center. Psy-
Psychol. Bull. 126:946–63 chiatr. Serv. 54:177–79
Widiger TA, Samuel DB. 2005. Evidence based Wu KD, Clark LA. 2003. Relations between
assessment of personality disorders. Psychol. personality traits and self-reports of daily be-
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
Rorschach? Science Confronts the Contro- being missed? Compr. Psychiatry 40:182–91
P1: JRX
February 26, 2005 19:35 Annual Reviews AR240-FM
CONTENTS
Access provided by University of California - San Francisco UCSF on 12/11/14. For personal use only.
vii
P1: JRX
February 26, 2005 19:35 Annual Reviews AR240-FM
viii CONTENTS
INDEX
Subject Index 653