Folkins 1981
Folkins 1981
Mental Health
CARLYLE H. FOLKINS University of California, Davis
WESLEY E. SIME University of Nebraska-Lincoln
Arnheim & Sinclair Personality, self- Elementary age ? (motor skills improved) California Test of Personality, Improved
(1974)° awareness, visual children Frostig Movement Skills Test,
percept Draw-A-Person Test, Read-
ing Eye Camera III
Barry et al. (1966)" Cognition Geriatric Yes Raven's Progressive Matrices, No change
short-term retention, simple
addition, ambiguous stimuli
Fretz et al. (1969)a Cognition, percep- Children No (8-wk physical devel- WISC, Frostig, Bender-Gestalt Improved
tual-motor opment program) Test
Gutin (1966)b Cognition College males No (12 wks, 2 X wk fitness Employee Aptitude Survey No change
training)
Gutin & DiGennaro Cognition Students in a "con- Not fitness oriented (8 wks, Arithmetic tasks after step-up Some improved
(1968)' ditioning" class step-ups) exercises (posttest only)
Ismail (1967)" Cognition Fifth and sixth grad- No (1 yr, 30 min PE, Otis IQ No change
ers 3 X wk) Stanford Academic Achieve- Improved
ment
Johnson & Fretz (1967)c Perceptual-motor Children No (6-wk physical devel- Tachistoscope measure, mirror Improved
opment program) drawing task
s
M
O'Conner (1969)" Perception, cogni- First graders ? (Kephart program, mo- Perceptual Forms Test, Met- No change
tion tor skills improved) ropolitan Readiness and
O
Achievement Tests
Z Powell (1974)b Cognition Geriatric mental pa- No (12 wks, 1 hr exercise, Wechsler Memory Scale, Improved
Tf tients 5 X wk) Raven's Progressive Matri-
C/J ces, Memory-for-Designs
n Behavior Several geriatric behavior scales No change
3
O Stamford et al. (1974)b Cognition, behavior Geriatric mental pa- Yesd Draw-A-Person, WAIS, behav- Some improved
o
o
tients ior scales
55 Weingarten (1973)" Cognition Highway patrol Yes" Raven's Progressive Matrices Improved
H trainees
* Young (1979)° Cognition Male and female Yesd • WAIS, Trail-Making Test, Improved
adults Crossing-Off Test, Wechsler
^T
2 Memory Scale subtests
i —» Note. WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; wk(s) = week(s); yr = year; min = minutes; PE = physical education; hr = hour.
CD
OO " Approximates nonequivalent control group quasi-experimental design, but random assignment assumption is not met; Ismail (1967) and Weingarten (1973) used matching procedures.
1—•
'' Experimental design.
• *' Preexperimental design.
CO '' Cardiovascular fitness.
-4
-a
about the effects of fitness training on perception ter on discrimination of interpersonal skills but not
are premature because, as Layman points out, most on communication, as compared with the interper-
experimental programs have used motor training sonal functioning training group. Both groups were
activities rather than cardiovascular training. Ac- superior to the no-training control group.
cording to Layman, some improvement in visual The research relating fitness training to behav-
perception follows from activity training, espe- ioral variables suggests a positive relationship to
cially when the training is intensive and individ- work-related variables, but little has been done on
ualized, but cardiovascular training has no effect other relevant behaviors, such as sleep and social
on auditory perception. Three studies serve as a behavior. Fitness enthusiasts have also reported
representative sample of these activity-oriented that sexual behavior is enhanced by fitness training,
programs (Arnheim & Sinclair, 1974; Fretz, John- but no research in this area has been attempted
son, & Johnson, 1969; Johnson & Fretz, 1967). to date.
More detailed information regarding the studies
that have attempted to evaluate the effects of phys- AFFECT
ical fitness training on cognition and perception
is presented in Table 1. Avid exercisers and theorists in physical education
(e.g., Scott, 1960) report an improved sense of well-
BEHAVIOR being associated with fitness, and several research-
ers have corroborated this relationship (Carter,
Work. Research on the relationship between 1977; Snyder & Spreitzer, 1974). Similarly, Morris
physical fitness training and work behavior has and Husman (1978) found that a sense of improved
been reviewed by Donoghue (1977) and so does "life quality" resulted from a fitness training ex-
not receive a study-by-study critique here. Ac- perience for college students.
cording to Donoghue, fitness training has been Much of the research relating fitness training to
positively associated with reduced absenteeism, an various affects has focused on what one might call
effect that has been related to reduced risk of heart the stress emotions, especially anxiety. Most of
disease following fitness training. Moreover, ex- these studies have been summarized elsewhere
ercise breaks on the job appear to reduce errors (Folkins & Amsterdam, 1977), and as can be seen
and improve output. Workers who participate in from Table 2, almost all outcomes have been pos-
exercise programs generally report that their work itive. That is, fitness training is associated with im-
performance and attitudes improve, provement in mood states, an effect which is more
Sleep. Physical exercise appears to be impor- pronounced with subjects who are more distressed
tant to sleep behavior (Baekeland, 1970; Baekeland or physically unfit at the outset (e.g., deVries, 1968;
& Lasky, 1966; Walker et al., 1978), but only one Folkins et al., 1972; McPherson et al., 1967; Mor-
study has related physical fitness training to sleep. gan, Roberts, Brand, & Feinerman, 1970).
Folkins, Lynch, and Gardner (1972) found that Unfortunately, only one of these studies is a true
college females who showed improvements in experimental design (D. S. Hanson, 1971), and that
aerobic fitness also reported improvements in sleep study with 4-year-olds used movement training
behavior. Males, who were more fit than females and no measures of fitness change. The majority
at the outset, noted no change in sleep. Since so- of the remaining studies are attempts to create a
phisticated instrumentation exists for monitoring nonequivalent control group design by recruiting
sleep behavior, a true experimental study of the a convenient nonexercise group or class.
effects of physical fitness training on sleep behavior Too often, experimental fitness training is of-
appears feasible and is sorely needed. fered to specially recruited subjects who seek out
Social behavior. A large number of intact group exposure to the training. This selection bias is a
comparison studies have related fitness and sports serious problem in any attempts at comparison
activity to popularity, social adjustment, and the with controls. Furthermore, the experimental sub-
like (Stevenson, 1975), but little research is avail- jects know themselves to be "experimentals," and
able relative to the effects of fitness training. their moods may improve from this attention fac-
Greenberg (1976) randomly assigned 60 students tor. Nevertheless, the consistently positive findings
to one of three conditions: fitness training plus in- of these studies are impressive. Continued exper-
terpersonal functioning training, interpersonal func- imental work is needed, however, with more at-
tioning training, or a no-training control. On Car- tention given to controls on factors threatening in-
khuff's Communication and Discrimination Indices, ternal validity.
the fitness plus interpersonal training group did bet- Several researchers have demonstrated that brief
R. S. Brown et al. (1978, Depression High school and No (10 wks, jogging) Zung Self-Rating Depression Improved
Phase 1)" university athletes Scale
deVries (1968)" Tension Middle-aged males Yes Electromyogram Improved
Folkins (1976)" Moods Middle-aged males Yes^ Multiple Affect " Adjective Improved (anxi-
at risk of CHD Check List (MAACL) ety)
Folkins et al. (1972)" Personality, moods, College males and Yes" MAACL, rating scales Improved (fe-
work, sleep females males)
D. S. Hanson (1971)b Anxiety 4-year-olds No (10 wks, 30 min move- Holtzman Inkblot Test, teacher Improved
ment training, 5 X wk) rating
Karbe (1966)c Anxiety College females No (15 wks, 40 min swim- Institute for Personality and Improved
ming, 2 X wk) Ability Testing (IPAT) Anx-
iety scale, Swimming Anxi-
ety and Fear Check List
Kowal et al. (1978)" Moods, self-con- Male and female re- Yesd (males only) Spielberger State-Trait Anxi- Mood improved
cept, personality cruits ,. ety Inventory (STAI), Profile (males)
of Mood States, Eysenck Per-
sonality Inventory
> Lynch et al. (Note 2)° Moods Middle-aged males No (university exercise MAACL Improved
PI class, jogging)
2
o McPherson et al. (1967)° Moods Postinfarct and nor- No (24 wks, graduated Semantic differentials (moods) Improved
z> mal adult males exercise, 2 X wk)
T3 Morgan et al. (1970)" Depression Adult males No . Self-Rating Depression Scale No change
on
«; Morris and Husman Well-being College students Yes Pflaum Life Quality Inventory Improved
o (1978)a
o Popejoy (1968)° Anxiety Adult females No (20 wks, 4 X wk, fit- IPAT Anxiety scale, Neuroti- Improved
o ness training) cism Scale Questionnaire
o
35 Tredway (Note 3)e Moods Older adults No STAI, Self-Rating Depression Improved
H
Scale, Mood State Inventory
> Young (1979)° Well-being, anxiety Male and female Yesd Life Satisfaction and Health Improved
^3 adults Rating Scales, MAACL
2
r Note. CHD = coronary heart disease; wk(s) = week(s); min = minutes.
CO "b Approximates nonequivalent control group quasi-experimental design, but random assignment assumption is not met; McPherson et al. (1967) used matching procedures.
00 Experimental design.
1
•
Preexperimental design.
d
Cardiovascular fitness.
CO
-J
CD
or single doses of exercise can be used to relieve the brevity of training programs, one must con-
electromyographic tension (deVries, 1968; deVries sider whether the 16 PF is sufficiently sensitive.
& Adams, 1972; Sime, 1977; Wood, 1977) and state Since few of the 16 personality factors are theo-
anxiety (Morgan & Horstman, 1976). DeVries and retically expected to change following short-term
Adams (1972) found that exercise "has a signifi- intervention (cf. Cattell et al., 1970), one won-
cantly greater effect upon resting musculature, ders why so many researchers persist in using this
without any undesirable side effects, than does instrument to assess personality changes following
meprobamate" (p. 140) and recommended exer- fitness training. Another study, employing the
cise as a reasonable treatment modality when a Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory
tranquilizer effect is desired. (MMPI) with postinfarct patients who completed
In an intact group comparison study, Cantor, a reconditioning program, failed to demonstrate
Zillman, and Day (1978) found that low-fit indi- improvement on the clinical scales of the MMPI
viduals had greater sympathetic-arousal responses (Naughton, Bruhn, & Lategola, 1968).
to stress films than did high-fit individuals. Could It appears that there is no evidence to support
fitness training produce such an effect? Since ex- a claim that global changes on personality tests
ercise is touted as a prophylactic for stress, a film follow from fitness training. This is not surprising
stress test of fitness training effects would be an in light of the fact that outcome research for psy-
important area for future research. chotherapy effects has been plagued with largely
disappointing results using this approach. One di-
PERSONALITY rection for future efforts might be to focus on a
target variable that is expected to shift. For ex-
Research relating physical fitness to personality has ample, Folkins et al. (1972) assumed that changes
not produced a consensus of opinion primarily in fitness would most likely affect present adjust-
because, until recently, investigators have persisted ment. As another example, Duke, Johnson, and
in simple attempts to compare athletes and non- Nowicki (1977) focused on locus of control in their
athletes on personality variables. A good number study of the effects of a sports fitness camp. The
of preexperimental and quasi-experimental efforts children in their preexperimental study moved
to assess the effects of fitness training on person- from an external to an internal locus of control,
ality among relative normals have emerged over although the researchers did not control for peer
the past 10 years, however. or leader effects.
The majority of the researchers in this area have Self-concept. The personality research with the
favored Cattell's Sixteen Personality Factor Ques- highest payoff has been that which focuses on self-
tionnaire (16 PF; Cattell, Eber, & Tatsuoka, 1970) concept variables. Researchers in this area gener-
for measuring personality variables. Ismail and his ally assume a self-as-object meaning for self, that
colleagues found rather modest gains or no change is, persons' attitudes and evaluations of aspects of
in the 16 PF variables following fitness training their self—body traits, abilities, and the like.
(Ismail & Trachtman, 1973; Ismail & Young, 1973, Changes in the body as a result of fitness training
1977; Young & Ismail, 1976). Similarly, Buccola might reasonably be expected to alter one's body
and Stone (1975) failed to find much effect fol- image, which is highly correlated with and might
lowing a 14-week program for older males. This be expected to radiate to self-concept (Zion, 1965)
lack of effect on 16 PF variables has been attrib- and affect (Goldberg & Folkins, 1974).
uted to the shortness of the training programs. Is- In a review of this literature, Layman (1974)
mail and Young (1977) assert that personality observes that "of seven studies involving tests of
changes would result from more lengthy training, self-concept before and after a physical develop-
which would create significant alterations in body ment program, four reported improvement in self-
chemistry and, consequently, changes in person- concept or body image . . . and three reported no
ality variables associated with body chemistry. One significant change" (p. 43). As can be seen from
problem with Ismail's research is that all his sub- Table 3, research since then has generally con-
jects were drawn from the middle-aged male staff firmed the assumption that fitness training im-
and faculty at Purdue University, many of whom proves self-concept. Of the two studies with ele-
were already fit before entering the program. mentary-school-age children, both of which
Other studies with the 16 PF on different popu- employed movement skills training rather than
lations are equally discouraging (Mayo, 1975; Till- cardiovascularly oriented training, the true exper-
man, 1965; Werner & Gottheil, 1966). Aside from imental study found positive changes in self-con-
Self-concept
Bruya (1977)° Self -concept Fourth graders No (4 wks, 30-rnin move- Piers-Harris Children's Self- No change
ment session, 2 X wk) Concept Scale
Colling wood (1972)b Body and self atti- Adult male rehabil- Yesd Body Attitude Scale, semantic Improved
tudes itation clients differentials, Bills Index of
Adjustment and Values
Colhngwood & Willett Body and self atti- Obese male teen- Yes'1 Same as above Improved
(1971)- tudes agers
J. S. Hanson & Nedde Self-concept Adult females Yes'1 Tennessee Self-Concept Scale Improved
(1974)" (TSCS)
gain in IQ suggested in these studies can be at-
.c tributed to a fitness training effect. Aside from the
•a -a W)
S 0) Hawthorne effect, receptive language stimulation
II
y S
a (e.g., reinforcement for appropriate responses to
1 1
jjj e O
1 1 verbal instructions) in perceptual-motor, motor
S
development, and conditioning programs for the
s retarded (Morrison & Pothier, 1978) could also
,
"S § 3
,
1
c
>,
^s account for IQ gains.
Research has convincingly shown that physical
s
03
'O O .s a
development programs for retarded children result
X2
q
& .=
in more positive body image (Chasey, Swartz, &
S o jij 'n ^§
o **« 1 Chasey, 1974; Maloney, Ball, & Edgar, 1970). This
1
(1972) used
^ improvement appears to remain stable over an
1* U I
extended period of time (Maloney & Payne, 1970).
CJ
1!
n f
§
S3 -O U "!.
s
X
Exercise programs also appear important for the
development of social skills among retarded chil-
H 2 C/3 H J ;
dren (B. J. Brown, 1977; Nunley, 1965). Studies
C
with retarded adults (Hussey, Maurer, & Schofield,
1
u 3 1976; Leighton, Cupp, Prince, Phillabaum, &
McLarren, 1966) have yielded no interpretable
|1 results because of serious design deficiencies.
t, Cardiologists have long recognized the impor-
1 rt
tance of exercise at both the physical and the psy-
chological levels in the treatment of postinfarct
1*8 £• 1
"B coronary patients (e.g., Hellerstein, 1965; Martic,
1
1976; Naughton, 1967). Quasi-experimental re-
£
0
>-
$
>•
Is i 'c
_o search (Folkins, 1976; McPherson et al., 1967, Ta-
g ble 2) has shown that gains in physical fitness
among males with coronary heart disease (CHD)
| "c
T: J
and among males likely to develop CHD are ac-
53 ec companied by improvement on affect variables.
rt
4) 4) 5P
J so
rt 8P 4)
13
u i Other "physical" problems that have been linked
I x to psychodynamic variables may also be responsive
Seventh-gra
Elementary
children
s
S 1 1 to physical exercise programs. Marley (1977) re-
o ~a
W) c
1 £2 I 2
i II
S
< 1) "3
11
1 c'
views the literature evaluating the effects of ex-
ercise on asthma and concludes that there is en-
< II_ 'S
"0
couraging evidence that 'exercise, especially
IB 2 swimming, reduces the incidence of asthma at-
rt
s?
£ | tacks. Since all the studies reviewed were preex-
§ S"
11 ||
perimental, such a conclusion does not seem war-
ranted. Some preexperimental data suggest that
o,
I
c
8
d
4)
O
e
O If If headaches may be controlled by brief, vigorous
8 0
S exercise (Atkinson, 1977; Ekbom & Lindahl, 1970),
o
! u x| 1 i"
*"d)
c>5
'T;
w "dj
« ^ i
^
'T3 >i wi S?
"g S
but no controlled studies with longer fitness train-
ing programs are available. Similarly, preexperi-
s^ •g 2
0,
mental data suggest that dysmenorrhea (painful
pi
S c
II
menstruation) may be relieved by regular exercise
t< ^ fl M
•^
CO
^?t^. > '«
" 5 13 a-s ^ c
— >> '^ cr_j
ss • (Lundquist, 1947), although much of the effect
s 2
o s "s s ~° c "*5 £ .£?
U | c
o "c 5 S
E aj ^ T3
may be placebo related (Hubbell, 1949).
In summary, physical fitness training has been
1 15 1
^rt sill advanced as a treatment for a variety of clinical
Experimei
Approxim;
cGowan el
Quasi-expt
Cardiovast
!il
1 ^ 4J
co syndromes. For the most part, however, the data
1
3 S>
SX —i 3 § ^
from these studies are merely suggestive. "Im-
rt rt •—•
1 1" provement" can often be attributed to a Haw-
d
l>
r
c
£ s S
TABLE 4
Effects of Physical Fitness Training on Clinical Syndromes
SB
r Demonstrated fitness
Study Primary focus Subjects effects? Psychological measures and tests Outcome
CD
OO
i—'
Clark et al. (1975)° Psychotic behavior Geriatric mental pa- Nod Self -care, total daily activity No change
tients
S Dodson & Mullens Psychotic behavior Selected VA patients Yesd (S wks, 1-mile jog, MMPI, Personal Orientation Improved
R
53 (1969)b 5 X wk) Inventory, body image,
O Draw-A-Person (DAP), se-
Z mantic differentials, Auto-
T) kinetic Index
K Kramer & Bauer (1955)" Psychotic behavior VA patients No (10 days, swimming Ratings of hyperactive behav- No change
O activities) ior (mood im-
O proved)
0 Powell (1974)" Cognition Geriatic mental pa- No (12 wks, 1 hr exercise, Raven's Progressive Matrices, Improved
O tients 5 X wk) Memory-for-Designs
H Smith &Figetakis(1970)c Schizophrenia VA patients Yes (isometrics) MMPI, DAP, Cornell-Cox No change
Health Inventory
Timmerman (1954)° Psychotic behavior VA patients No (hydrogymnastics) Behavior control treatments, Improved
e.g., seclusion, shock
R. S. Brown et al. (1978)c Depression University students No Zung Self-Rating Depression Improved
Scale, Activation-Deactiva-
tion Check List, Lorr Mood
Check List
Greist et al. (1979)" Depression Male and female No (10 wks, 30-45 min Symptom Check List-90 Improved
outpatients running, 3 X wk) (NIMH)
Kavanagh et al. (1977)'' Depression Postcoronary "most Yesd MMPI (Depression scale) Improved
depressed" males
Gary & Guthrie (1972)" Alcoholism Hospitalized males Yesd ACL, Body-Cathexis and Self- Some improved
Cathexis Scales, sleep behav-
ior
B. J. Brown (1977)" Mentally retarded 12-year-old males Yes (isometrics) Stanford-Binet IQ, Vineland Improved
Social Maturity Scale
Chasey et al. (1974)" Mentally retarded 10-19-year-olds No (5 wks, daily PE) Holtzman Inkblot Test Improved
Corder (1966)" Mentally retarded 12-16-year-old Yes (motor proficiency) WISC Improved
males
Hussey et al. (1976)c Mentally retarded Adults Yes (motor proficiency) Ratings of attention seeking ?
and social behavior
Leighton et al. (1966)° Mentally retarded Adult males Yes Peabody Picture Vocabulary ?
Test, WAIS, DAP, TAT, Sen-
tence Completion Test
Maloney et al. (1970)" Mentally retarded Males with mean age Yes (motor proficiency) Personal Orientation Inven- Improved
of 14 tory; Eye, Hand, and Ear
Test (body image measures)
s
PI
IS Nunley (1965)'' Mentally retarded 9-14-year-olds Yes (motor proficiency) Observations of adjustment, Improved
n "social behavior"
z Oliver (1958)' Mentally retarded 13-15-year-old Yes (motor proficiency) Terman-Merrill IQ, Porteus Improved
« males Maze Test, Raven's Progres-
o sive Matrices, DAP, God-
o
1— 1
dard's Form Board
o
n Note. VA = Veterans Administration; wk(s) = week(s); hr = hour; MMPI = Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory; PE = physical education; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WAIS =
3! Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; TAT = Thematic Apperception Test:; ACL = Adjective Check List.
H " Experimental design.
* k
Quasi-experimental design (time series).
p> 'Approximates nonequivalent control group quasi-experimental design, but random assignment assumption is not met; Hussey et al. (1976) and Oliver (1958) used matching procedures.
3 Cardiovascular fitness.
' Preexperimental design (one-group pretest-posttest).
r
CD
00
1—'
oo
on
thorne effect (see D. L. Hanson, 1967) or to In this review we have focused on problems with
"regression toward the mean." Furthermore, very internal validity, but threats to external validity
few studies have employed rigorous cardiovascu- (e.g., the interaction effect of selection bias and
larly oriented fitness training. On the other hand, the treatment variable, exercise) must also be ad-
there are several true experimental studies with dressed. Self-selected, motivated volunteers may
psychotics and the mentally retarded. Studies with demonstrate improvement in psychological func-
the mentally retarded, especially those with chil- tioning simply because they are motivated for
dren, have been reasonably well designed, but overall self-improvement. It is therefore necessary
none have used cardiovascularly oriented training to arrange for control groups that have time ex-
programs. It appears valid to conclude nevertheless posure equal to that of trainees (e.g., B. J. Brown,
that young retardates benefit psychologically from 1977), as well as equal and justified expectations
fitness training and that this effect is most dramatic for benefit (e.g., Corder, 1966).
for intellectual functioning and body image. Pop- Aside from design problems, variations in du-
ulation groups that tend toward very sedentary ration and intensity of exercise programs make
life-styles, such as the retarded and the aged, ap- results difficult to interpret. We have suggested
pear to derive cognitive benefits from relatively that documentation of cardiovascular functioning
simple but regular exercise programs. Some min- is one approach to greater standardization of train-
imal amount of gross muscular movement may be ing effects.
needed to provide a stimulating effect on brain This new field of study needs further develop-
functioning (cf. Powell, 1974). ment at the theoretical level as well. Research
would appeal to a wider audience and have a wider
application if it were embedded in a broader
Conclusions framework of psychological functioning. Lazarus's
(1975) emotion and adaptation theory, with its ty-
In general, studies of physical fitness effects on pology of self-regulatory processes, seems to us to
psychological health are poorly designed, Only offer a promising context for this research.
about 15% of the studies reviewed here qualified Finally, future research on fitness training as a
as true experiments, and most of these were studies treatment strategy may need to pay more attention
on clinical populations. The true experiments with to individual differences. Individuals probably dif-
normals were mostly done with children (D. S. fer greatly in their ability to profit psychologically
Hanson, 1971; McGowan et al, 1974; Martinek et from fitness training because of differences in be-
al., 1978), and only one of these employed mea- liefs, expectations, preferred coping styles, and the
sures of cardiovascular fitness (McGowan et al., like. For example, exercise seems beneficial for
1974). Almost half of the studies used preexperi- those who experience anxiety at the somatic level,
mental designs'—most often the one-group pretest- while meditation may be a better strategy for those
posttest design. This design was used in all the who are overburdened by cognitive anxiety
research on psychosomatic problems. The most (Schwartz et al., 1978).
common design (60% of the studies) used by re- While all areas reviewed need more true ex-
searchers working with normal groups was an ap- perimental research, fitness programs for almost
proximation of the nonequivalent control group all of the clinical syndromes have yet to be care-
design. Because of selection bias and regression fully evaluated. Fitness training and cognitive
effects, these studies often have problems with in- functioning among the aged and fitness training
ternal validity. In future research, once a group effects on sleep and sex behavior also need more
of motivated subjects has been identified, a random attention. Additionally, theoretical speculation
assignment procedure is imperative. With a candid suggests that fitness training should help people
appeal for scientific rigor, normal adults may con- cope with physical and psychological stresses. Lab-
sent to serve in reasonable control conditions. Sub- oratory experiments with psychologically stressful
jects could be asked to wait for fitness training and conditions (e.g., stress films, exams) could yield im-
serve as waiting-list controls. Alternatively, subjects portant evidence relevant to this hypothesis.
might serve as trainers' assistants (e.g., Corder,
1966) and thereby serve as controls for attention REFERENCE NOTES
and mastery aspects. Still others might be ran-
1. Sime, W. E. Acute relief of emotional stress. In Proceedings
domly assigned to a reasonable substitute such as of the American Association for the Advancement of Ten-
karate training. sion Control, Louisville, Kentucky, 1978.