2002 Tarrier Formulation
2002 Tarrier Formulation
Clinical Section
University of Manchester, UK
Abstract. The historical origins of the case formulation approach to assessment and treat-
ment are described and its role in clinical practice and research discussed. It is argued that
treatment based on individual case formulations should not be precluded from clinical trials.
The empirical evidence for the reliability and efficacy of case formulation is reviewed. The
evidence that an idiosyncratic case formulation approach to treatment has any advantage
over a standard protocol is equivocal; however, the studies that have been carried out are
under powered and potentially suffer from a Type II error. The standard procedure for case
formulation is briefly described and the argument advanced that this method of clinical
assessment should be soundly based upon empirical evidence and hypothesis testing and not
on speculation. Three modifications are suggested to the current practice of case formulation.
These are: 1) the conceptualization of dysfunctional systems in the maintenance of clinical
problems; 2) the historical background of a clinical problem should be described in terms
of vulnerabilities and epidemiological evidence-base; and 3) the pivotal role of social behavi-
our and context should be emphasized and accommodated in a formulation. The advantages
of a case formulation, in providing an understanding of the maintenance of clinical problems,
in providing an integrated approach to intervention are outlined.
Keywords: Cognitive-behavioural, case formulation, social context.
Introduction
Case formulation is one of the core skills of the clinical psychologist (DCP, 2001, p. 2) and
is a central process in the role of the scientific practitioner.1 It involves the elicitation of
appropriate information and the application and integration of a body of theoretical psycho-
Reprint requests to Nicholas Tarrier, Academic Division of Clinical Psychology, Education and Research Building,
Wythenshawe Hospital, Southmoor Road, Manchester M23 8LT, UK. E-mail: [email protected]
1
Case formulation has been developed in a number of therapeutic schools (see Eells, 1997), but the purpose of
this paper is to focus on CBT.
logical knowledge to a specific clinical problem in order to understand the origins, develop-
ment and maintenance of that problem. Its purpose is both to provide an accurate overview
and explanation of the patient’s problems that is open to verification through hypothesis
testing, and to arrive collaboratively with the patient at a useful understanding of their
problem that is meaningful to them. The latter has been termed the ‘‘treatment utility’’ of
case formulation (Hayes, Nelson, & Jarrett, 1987). The case formulation is then used to
inform treatment or intervention by identifying key targets for change. The purpose of this
paper is to describe the process and development of cognitive-behavioural case formulation,
integrating the epidemiological, systemic and social aspects of clinical problems.
Empirical basis
The factor that has distinguished the broad school of cognitive-behaviour therapies from
other schools of psychotherapy has been a commitment to empirical validation both in
terms of its basic theoretical premises and treatment outcomes. The issues surrounding case
formulation should be no different and should be resolved by reference to empirical findings
and not speculation. This is especially true with respect to historical factors that may influ-
ence the genesis and development of a clinical disorder.
research projects will be efficacious with those cases that have been excluded from the
original efficacy trial. Secondly, the use of strict protocols limits the clinical freedom of
qualified and competent practitioners and reduces the possibility of innovation and develop-
ment. Lastly, there is the suspicion that the use of strict protocols is being promoted not by
specific evidence but by managed health care, especially in the USA. This is, of course, not
to argue that practice should not be informed by research and that treatments need not be
validated by outcome trials, but rather that the conclusions drawn from research need to be
accurate and that we need to be aware of the social and economic pressures that can also
be exerted on practice. For example, one should compare the actual scientific evidence
currently available to support some treatment approaches with their heavy marketing (e.g.
Rosen, Lohr, McNally, & Herbert, 1999).
over-responsible, which were identified by a median of 93.4% (range: 100% to 67.4%), but
were poor at identifying social isolation, with only 13% identifying this problem. Inter Class
Correlations for inter-rater reliability of five clinicians for identifying underlying cognitive
mechanism varied between .27 and .92 with a median of .82. Dysfunctional attitudes were
the most difficult to reach agreement on, whereas views of the world had good consensus
with an ICC of .92. In a second study of 38 clinicians, 67% of patients’ overt problems
were identified but reliability coefficients for single clinicians were low (mean 0.37, range
0.13–0.66) for schema ratings (Persons & Bertagnolli, 1999).
advantage in the use of an individualized treatment over a standard package. This result is
not surprising given the sample sizes of these studies. Standard treatment programmes are
effective for a wide range of psychological disorders. Even if an individualized treatment
was superior, the difference in effect sizes between the two approaches would most probably
be small. Thus, the sample size required to significantly demonstrate such a difference would
necessarily be large. Therefore, the studies that have been carried out are significantly under
powered. To substantiate this we have estimated the sample sizes required to show signific-
ant differences with 80% power and .05 significance level based on the data provided in the
published reports of two such studies. On the basis of Emmelkamp et al.’s (1994) data the
numbers in each group required to show a significant difference for the various measures
used in the study would be 25 for the Rational Behaviour Inventory, 560 for the Symptom
Check List-90-R, 800 for the Self-rating Depression Scale and Inventory of Interpersonal
Symptoms, 4,000 for the Maudsley Obsessional-Compulsive Inventory and in excess of
15,000 for measures of anxiety/discomfort. Similarly, on the basis of data published by
Hickling and Blanchard (1997) for the protocol treatment of PTSD following a road traffic
accident, to detect a 10% improvement on this from a case formulation treatment would
require group sizes of 75 for State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, 130 for the PTSD checklist and
480 for the Beck Depression Inventory. Therefore, the feasibility of carrying out comparison
studies between protocol-based and case formulation-based treatments, let alone obtaining
funding to do so, is limited.
Dysfunctional systems
Dysfunctional systems arise from failure in corrective feedback or homeostasis. Instead
there is a tendency for feedback to destabilize and so amplify or maintain feedback processes
Case formulation: an integrative approach 317
that mitigate against self-correction. Response patterns often become entrenched and cyc-
lical, and take on numerous functions. Therefore, because the chain of events often becomes
circular rather than linear, it is easier to think of a dysfunctional system that is activated
and in which the various components have interacting relationships, which are strengthened
through activation of the feedback system (see Figure 1). Such systemic approaches can be
seen in models of psychopathology, for example in the ‘‘vicious circle’’ model of panic
disorder (Clarke, 1986), in the coping feedback model of psychotic symptom maintenance
in schizophrenia (Haddock & Tarrier, 1998; Tarrier & Haddock, 2002) and in the social
ecological model of adolescent delinquent behaviour (Henggeler et al., 1991, 1992, 1997).
Thus the initial process of case formulation is to identify the dysfunctional interactions
that are currently operating in the patient’s life and serve to maintain the problem. The
relationships of the various components within the dysfunctional system should be open to
empirical verification through further assessment and hypothesis testing. The aim is to deter-
mine which set of factors is maintaining the problem and preventing some type of natural
restitution and return to normal homeostasis. As can be seen from Figure 1 the dysfunctional
system can be a complex set of interactions that involve both intrapersonal and interpersonal
factors.
method of using a patient’s retrospective recall of the historical development of their prob-
lem as evidence of its aetiology is unsatisfactory. We suggest a probabilistic model in
which individual characteristics of the patient’s life and experience are matched to known
vulnerability and risk factors drawn from the research literature to suggest possible pathways
to the origins of the current problem. Thus historical aspects of case formulation need to be
founded on epidemiological data on risk factors associated with the development of any
subsequent disorder.
Vulnerability-stress models
The occurrence of disorder is postulated to be the product of vulnerability and stress. That
some common characteristics render individuals more at risk to develop a specific disorder
is seen as evidence of vulnerability. Increased vulnerability results in increased risk but does
not inevitably result in the occurrence of disorder; some further destabilization or stress is
required to precipitate the disorder and also trigger help-seeking behaviour and progress
into and through the health care system (see Goldberg & Huxley, 1992). Some vulnerability
represents both inherited and acquired biological vulnerability; for example, it is highly
likely that there is a genetic component in the cause of schizophrenia and bipolar affective
disorder. However, other vulnerability may well be acquired through exposure to specific
environments. There are many published studies investigating vulnerability and the later
development of psychological disorders but a few examples will suffice. In a famous series
of studies Brown and Harris (1978) found that in working class women the lack of a confid-
ing relationship, the loss of their mother before the age of 11, unemployment and three
young children at home greatly increased the risk of depression. Thus exposure to a number
of vulnerability factors, some distal and some proximal, substantially increases the probabil-
ity of developing depression in the face of provoking agents. However, it was not inevitable,
and other more proximal factors, such as the occurrence of a life event or crisis, serve to
explain the occurrence and maintenance of this disorder in any individual.
A second, but related example, is the long established finding that maternal depression is
associated with emotional and behavioural problems in young children (Cox, Puckering,
Pound, & Mills, 1987), and that, untreated, these persist even when the mother’s depression
remits. Emotional and behavioural problems in childhood show linkages with a range of
problems in adolescence and beyond, including mental health problems (Robins, 1991),
reduced educational and occupational attainment (Caspi, Elder, & Bem, 1987; Fergusson &
Horwood, 1998), criminal and antisocial behaviours, and suicide (Fergusson & Lynskey,
1998). The importance of consideration of historical factors in case formulation therefore
becomes the identification of general vulnerability factors, (i.e. those that pertain to a
population) and specific predispositions (those that pertain to that individual alone), which
have resulted in increased risk of disorder should other necessary conditions prevail.
There is an important point here in how this is explained to the patient. For example, poor
parenting and adverse childhood experience increase vulnerability to developing anxiety and
depression in adult life (Goldberg & Huxley, 1992, p. 87). It can be explained that exposure
to these factors had meant the person’s personal risk has increased. This is very different
from using the patient’s retrospective account of their parents’ behaviour and unhappy child-
hood to explain why they later became depressed.
To take the example of sexual abuse, Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans and Herison
Case formulation: an integrative approach 319
(1996) were able to demonstrate in a retrospective study of a large community sample that,
while sexual abuse carries specific risks for later psychological disturbance, many of the
apparent adverse long-term effects associated with sexual abuse arise in part from the
broader context of childhood disadvantage and family conflict within which abusive events
occur. They warned that an exclusive or exaggerated focus on prior sexual abuse might
obscure other, equally relevant vulnerability factors.
A useful distinction can be made between distal and proximal vulnerabilities. Distal vul-
nerability is conferred by events that occurred in the past, such as being the victim of
childhood abuse, whereas proximal vulnerability is conferred by vulnerability that is recent
or still active or occurring, such as low self-esteem or lack of a confiding relationship in
women at risk of depression. This is an important point pragmatically as well as conceptu-
ally, as there is probably little that can be done to change distal vulnerability but on-going
proximal vulnerability may be possible to change. So, for example, Runtz and Schallow
(1997) showed that, for students reporting previous abusive experiences, current psycholo-
gical adjustment was mediated by social support and coping strategies. Findings such as
these indicate the value of individual formulation in identifying current interpersonal pro-
cesses and cognitions that contribute to vulnerability or promote resilience. The stress-
vulnerability model is described diagrammatically in Figure 2.
Social factors
Social context of the clinical problem
Traditionally, behavioural analysis has examined behaviour outside of its social context
except for viewing it as a reinforcement or punishment delivery system. Cognitive models
have further distanced their analysis from the social context by focusing on internal pro-
cesses. We view meaningful social interactions and the interpersonal environment as central
to human behaviour and clinical problems, and therefore their analysis needs to take centre
stage in case formulation. There is good reason to do this; human beings have evolved into
complex social animals with very complex social behaviours and goals. Baumeister and
Leary (1995), in their review of the need for meaningful social interactions, concluded that:
1) people naturally seek and form relationships with others; 2) efforts to dissolve relation-
Case formulation: an integrative approach 321
ships are strongly resisted; 3) information about meaningful relationships and relationship
partners are more thoroughly processed than information about other people; 4) the quality
and intensity of relationships are directly associated with mood; and 5) intermittent or super-
ficial social bonds do not result in the same mental health benefits as meaningful bonds.
Humans have evolved as a species as social animals for whom socialization and all that
goes with it is highly influential in their lives. It is also highly probable that mechanisms to
elicit, maintain and regulate social interactions have also evolved and are operational in
every aspect of a person’s functioning. Aspects of problems seen in the clinic may well have
their origins in this social evolution (Gilbert, 2001). For example, Gilbert (2000) provides an
explanation of the role of submissive behaviours in depression and their evolution as a form
of social defence, and similarly accounts for the function of self-esteem within the role of
social attention-holding capacity in social comparisons as a mechanism in forming and
maintaining social hierarchy (Gilbert, Price, & Allan, 1995; Price, 2000). Interpersonal
environments thus have the potential to generate powerful psychobiological responses that
precipitate action that may be persistent or difficult to control. Such action may in itself
constitute or lead to psychopathology or disruptive emotion reactions.
There is considerable empirical evidence that good social support has a positive effect on
health in general (Uchino, Uno, & Holt-Lunstad, 1999). Social support has been hypothes-
ized to impact on mental health in two ways: by buffering against stress (Cohen & McKay,
1984) or through social cognition (Cohen & Willis, 1985; Rhodes & Lakey, 1999). In the
former, perceived social support may impact on the appraisal of stress by providing the
individual with the belief that additional resources, in the form of others, are available. In
the latter, the positive role of social support may be to have access to others who can provide
information about positive health related behaviour and positive beliefs and conceptualiz-
ation of situations and self-perceptions (Penn, Mueser, Tarrier, Gloege, Serrano, 2001).
Negative interpersonal environments will also have a powerful effect on both the develop-
ment and outcome of psychological disorders. Rhodes and Lakey (1999) cite over 1000
studies reporting an association between low social support and poor mental health. A
related research area is the interpersonal environment in the home, which has been investig-
ated by the measure of Expressed Emotion. Expressed Emotion (EE) is assessed from an
audio-taped semi-structured interview with a respondent, usually a close relative, about the
index patient. From this interview it is possible to reliably measure dimensions of criticism,
hostility, emotional over-involvement and warmth and classify the relative as high or low
on EE (Leff & Vaughn, 1985). Living in a household that has high EE members has been
shown to have a detrimental effect on the outcome of various psychological disorders
(Kavanagh, 1992; Butzlaff & Hooley, 1998; Wearden, Tarrier, Barrowclough, Zastowny, &
Rahill, 2000). These include schizophrenia, eating disorders, depression, bipolar disorder, a
variety of medical conditions and childhood behavioural disorders. Furthermore, EE levels
of significant others may also affect outcome of cognitive behavioural treatments, for
example with PTSD (Tarrier, Sommerfield, & Pilgrim, 1999). Various models of how the
interpersonal environment, as measured by EE, impact upon the patient have been advanced
(Wearden et al., 2000; Tarrier, Barrowclough, Ward, Donaldson, & Burns, in press). Beliefs
or social cognitions held by those within the patient’s social environment may well deter-
mine their behaviour towards the patient (Barrowclough, Johnston, & Tarrier, 1994;
Wearden et al., 2000) and also potentially the physiological reactions of the patient
(Tarrier & Turpin, 1992). Measurement of EE is time consuming and does not lend itself
322 N. Tarrier and R. Calam
easily to clinical practice. However, there are other briefer methods such as the perceived
and expressed criticism scales developed by Hooley with depression (Hooley & Teasdale,
1989) that could be used as proxy measures of the interpersonal environment. Although
clinical investigations of adult disorders rarely routinely assess the person’s social environ-
ment, in some clinical settings, such as child and adolescent services, interviewing the
parents and other family members is the norm and combines more easily with this type of
approach.
Investigations of family environments have led to family interventions in schizophrenia
aimed at reducing EE levels and their putative stressful effects. Many of these interventions
have used cognitive-behavioural assessments and interventions (e.g. Falloon, Boyd, &
McGill, 1984; Barrowclough & Tarrier, 1992). These family interventions have been found
consistently to be successful at reducing relapse rates (Pitschel-Walz, Leucht, Bauml,
Kissling, & Engel, 2001). Similar efficacy has also been suggested with family interventions
in depression (Leff et al., 2000).
Social support is unlikely to be simply a passive substrate against which psychopathology
develops. Seeking out the co-operation of others and the utilization of social resources may
well be primary bio-social goals (Gilbert, 2001) that can moderate against adversity. Sim-
ilarly, social context has been shown to mitigate against the effects of abuse in childhood.
The literature on maltreatment, for example, suggests that good social support may have a
major buffering effect (Runtz & Schallow, 1997), and studies of maltreated children have
shown that good peer relationships are associated with better mental health throughout child-
hood (Bolger, Patterson, & Kupersmidt, 1998). However, this longitudinal study of abused
children showed that chronicity and type of maltreatment appear to influence peer accept-
ance, and severity and timing of maltreatment are important. So, for example, children who
are maltreated over a long period of time are less popular with their peers. Initial effects
are apparent from early school age, and persist through childhood (Bolger & Patterson,
2001). This is likely in turn to limit the capacity to elicit or benefit from social resources
for support and learning, and lead to increased vulnerability.
The interactive nature of the relationship between the person and their social environment
can be seen from a study that investigated factors that predicted subsequent PTSD in victims
of road traffic accidents (Holeva, Tarrier, & Wells, 2001). The use of social engagement as
a method of coping after a road traffic accident was generally a positive coping strategy
that appeared to moderate the effects of trauma. However, if the interpersonal environment
was itself negative, or perceived as such by the subject, then social engagement became a
liability and the risk of later developing PTSD greatly increased by a factor of eight. Sim-
ilarly, in a longitudinal study investigating the onset of seasonal depression, poor social
support, especially in the context of low self esteem, measured during summer when the
subjects’ mood was normal, resulted in faster onset and longer duration of seasonal depres-
sion with the progression into winter (McCarthy, Tarrier, & Gregg, 2002).
The positive effects of a strong social support system could be to neutralize a person’s
irrational and biased interpretation of events, whereas a critical or rejecting relative or signi-
ficant other could reinforce negative thinking (Beck et al., 1979). In patients suffering obses-
sional-compulsive disorder it has been suggested that concealment from others of cata-
strophic fears is an additional manifestation of avoidance and neutralizing behaviour
characteristic of this disorder. Concealment, resulting primarily from a fear of the negative
Case formulation: an integrative approach 323
Integration of intervention
Case formulation provides the capacity to list and prioritize the patient or family’s problems
and to generate testable hypotheses as to why they occur, which allows a strategic approach
to treatment. Rather than prescribing a specific treatment for a particular disorder, case
formulation allows problems to be translated into treatment goals. Treatment techniques can
then be implemented so as to achieve these goals. This approach encourages the use of
evidence-based practice through the selection of empirically validated treatment techniques
to achieve the treatment goals guided by the case formulation. In this way assessment and
treatment are integrated. There are indications that interpersonal behaviour is being given
more importance in modified versions of CBT. For example, the ability to identify and
modify cognitive and behavioural patterns that underlie interpersonal difficulties has been
the aim of a modified form of CBT known as cognitive behavioural-analysis. This method
has been shown to be particularly effective in treating chronic depression when used in
combination with nefazodone in a large well conducted clinical trial of 662 patients (Keller
et al., 2000). Similarly, Henggeler and colleagues (Henggeler et al., 1992, 1993, 1997) have
found that multisystemic therapy (MST), based upon a conceptual system in which the
problems of juvenile offenders and their families are viewed as embedded in multiple sys-
tems, is successful in improving psychiatric symptoms and reducing criminal activity. How-
ever, the latter only occurred when the adherence to the treatment protocol was good
(Henggeler et al., 1997). It is anticipated that such attempts to modify interpersonal and
social context will be developed in an increasing range of psychological disorders.
Summary
The advantages of the case formulation approach are that it allows a flexible and idiosyn-
cratic understanding of each patient’s individual problems irrespective of their diagnostic
classification. It is sufficiently flexible to be accommodated in clinical research in terms of
either efficacy or effectiveness trials. It can include epidemiological information on vulner-
ability and risk, a systemic approach to problem maintenance and the patient’s interpersonal
and social context. It is further advantageous in that a targeted and individualized treat-
ment can be produced from the formulation that is specific to the needs of that individual.
It is suggested that this is particularly advantageous in complex cases (Tarrier, Wells, &
324 N. Tarrier and R. Calam
Haddock, 1998). Further research on the reliability and validity of case formulation is
required, although this will impose methodological and logistical challenges. Case formula-
tion can also be informed by other disciplines such as social psychology, psychiatric epi-
demiology and social psychiatry. Further developments on how best to incorporate the social
context, perhaps through detailed assessment of the patient’s social cognition or interper-
sonal environment, are required.
Acknowledgement
The authors are very grateful to Professor Paul Gilbert and Dr Irene Oestrich and to the
anonymous reviewers for their very helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.
References
AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(3rd ed. rev.). Washington, DC: APA.
AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(4th ed.). Washington, DC: APA.
BARROWCLOUGH, C., JOHNSTON, C., & TARRIER, N. (1994). Attribution, expressed emotion and patient
relapse: An attributional model of relatives’ response to schizophrenic illness. Behavior Therapy,
25, 67–88.
BARROWCLOUGH, C., & TARRIER, N. (1992). Families of schizophrenic patients: Cognitive behavioural
intervention, London: Chapman & Hall.
BARROWCLOUGH, C., TARRIER, N., SELLWOOD, W., QUINN, J., MAINWARING, J., & LEWIS, S. (1999). A
randomised controlled effectiveness trial of a needs based psychosocial intervention service for
carers of schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 506–511.
BAUMEISTER, R. F., & LEARY, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments
as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
BECK, A. T., RUSH, A. J., SHAW, B. F., & EMERY, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy in depression. New
York: Guilford.
BEGELMAN, D. A. (1976). Behavioral classification. In M. Hersen & A. S. Bellack (Eds.), Behavioral
assessment: A practical handbook. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
BISSETT, R. T., & HAYES, S. C. (1999). The likely success of functional analysis tied to the DSM.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 379–383.
BOLGER, K. E., & PATTERSON, C. J. (2001). Developmental pathways from child maltreatment to peer
rejection. Child Development, 72, 549–568.
BOLGER, K. E., PATTERSON, C. J., & KUPERSMIDT, J. B. (1998). Peer relationships and self-esteem
among children who have been maltreated. Child Development, 69, 1171–1197.
BROWN, G. W., & HARRIS, T. (1978). Social origins of depression. London: Tavistock.
BRUCH, M. (1998). The development of case formulation approaches. In M. Bruch & F. W. Bond
(Eds.), Beyond diagnosis: Case formulation approaches in CBT. Chichester: Wiley.
BUTZLAFF, R. L., & HOOLEY, J. M. (1998). Expressed emotion and psychiatric relapse: A meta analysis.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 547–552.
CASPI, A., ELDER, G. H., & BEM, D. J. (1987). Moving against the world: Life course patterns of
explosive children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 308–313.
CLARK, D. M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24,
461–470.
Case formulation: an integrative approach 325
CLARK, D. M., & WELLS, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. Heimberg, M. Leibow-
itz, D. A. Hope & F. R. Schnier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment and treatment. New
York: Guilford Press.
COHEN, S., & MCKAY, G. (1984). Social support, stress and the buffering hypothesis: A theoretical
analysis. In A. Baum, S. E. Taylor & J. E. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and health (pp.
253–267). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
COHEN, S., & WILLIS, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological
Bulletin, 98, 310–357.
COX, A. D., PUCKERING, C., POUND, A., & MILLS, M. (1987). The impact of maternal depression in
young children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 917–928.
DIVISION OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY (2001). The core purpose and philosophy of the profession. Leices-
ter: The British Psychological Society.
EMMELKAMP, P. M. G., BOUMAN, T. K., & BLAAUW, E. (1994). Individualised versus standardised
therapy: A comparative evaluation with obsessive-compulsive patients. Clinical Psychology and
Psychotherapy, 1, 95–100.
EIFERT, G. H. (1996). More theory-driven and less diagnosis-based behavior therapy. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 27, 75–86.
EELLS, T. D. (1997). Handbook of psychotherapy case formulation. New York: Guilford.
FALLOON, I. R. H., BOYD, J. L., & MCGILL, C. W. (1984). Family care of schizophrenia. New York:
Guilford Press.
FARMER, R. F., & NELSON-GRAY, R. O. (1999). Functional analysis and response covariation in the
assessment of personality disorders: A reply to Staats and to Bisset and Hayes. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 37, 385–394.
FERGUSSON, D. M., & HORWOOD, L. J. (1998). Early conduct problems and later life opportunities.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 1097–1108.
FERGUSSON, D. M., & LYNSKEY, M. T. (1998). Conduct problems in childhood and psychosocial
outcomes in young adulthood: A prospective study. Journal of Emotional and Behavioural Dis-
orders, 6, 2–18.
FOLLETTE, W. C., HOUTS, A. C., & HAYES, S. (1992). Behavior therapy and the new medical model.
Behavior Assessment, 14, 323–343.
GAVONO, A., GODOY, A., RODRIGUEZ-NARANJO, C., & EIFERT, G. H. (1996). How can behavior therapy
treat the same disorder with different techniques and different disorders with the same technique.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 27, 107–117.
GILBERT, P. (2000). Varieties of submissive behavior as forms of social defence: Their evolution and
role in depression. In L. Sloman & P. Gilbert (Eds.), Subordination and defeat: An evolutionary
approach to mood disorders and their therapy. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.
GILBERT, P. (2001). Evolutionary approaches to psychopathology: The role of natural defences. Aus-
tralian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 35, 17–27.
GILBERT, P., PRICE, J., & ALLAN, S. (1995). Social comparison, social attractiveness and evolution:
How might they be related? New Ideas in Psychology: An International Journal of Innovative
Theory in Psychology, 13, 149–165.
GOLDBERG, D., & HUXLEY, P. (1992). Common mental disorders: A bio-social model. London: Tavis-
tock/Routledge.
HADDOCK, G., & TARRIER, N. (1998). Assessment and formulation in the cognitive behavioural treat-
ment of psychosis. In N. Tarrier, A. Wells & G. Haddock (Eds.), Cognitive behaviour therapy for
complex cases: An advanced guidebook for the practitioner. Chichester: Wiley.
HAWTON, K., SALKOVSKIS, P., KIRK, J., & CLARK, D. M. (1989). Cognitive behaviour therapy for
psychiatric problems: A practical guide. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
HAYES, S. C., & HAYES, L. J. (1992). Some clinical implications of contextualistic behaviorism: The
example of cognition. Behavior Therapy, 23, 225–249.
326 N. Tarrier and R. Calam
HAYES, S. C., KAHOLOKULA, J. K., & NELSON, K. (1999). The idiographic application of nomothetic
empirically based treatments. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6, 436–451.
HAYES, S. C., NELSON, R. O., & JARRETT, R. B. (1987). The treatment utility of assessment: A func-
tional approach to evaluating assessment quality. American Psychologist, 42, 963–974.
HENGGELER, S. W., BORDUIN, C. M., MELTON, G. B., MANN, B. J., SMITH, L., HALL, J. A., CONE, L., &
FUCCI, B. R. (1991). Effects of multisystemic therapy on drug use and abuse in serious juvenile
offenders: A progress report from two outcome studies. Family Dynamics of Addiction Quarterly,
1, 40–51.
HENGGELER, S. W., MELTON, G. B., BRONDINO, M. J., SCHERER, D. G., & HANLEY, J. H. (1997).
Multisystemic therapy with violent and chronic juvenile offenders and their families: The role of
treatment fidelity in successful dissemination. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65,
821–833.
HENGGELER, S. W., MELTON, G. B., & SMITH, L. A. (1992). Family preservation using multisystemic
therapy: An effective alternative to incarcerating serious juvenile offenders. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 60, 953–961.
HENGGELER, S. W., MELTON, G. B., SMITH, L. A., SCHOENWALD, S. K., & HANLEY, J. H. (1993). Family
preservation using multisystemic therapy: Long term follow-up to a clinical trial with serious juven-
ile offenders. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 2, 283–293.
HICKLING, E. J., & BLANCHARD, E. B. (1997). The private practice psychologist and manual based
treatments: Post-traumatic stress disorder secondary to motor vehicle accidents. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 35, 191–203.
HOLEVA, V., TARRIER, N., & WELLS, A. (2001). Prevalence and predictors of acute stress disorder and
PTSD following road traffic accidents: Thought control strategies and social support. Behavior
Therapy, 32, 65–84.
HOOLEY, J. M., & TEASDALE, J. D. (1989). Predictors of relapse in unipolar depressives: Expressed
emotion, marital distress and perceived criticism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 229–235.
JACOBSON, N. S., SCHMALING, K. B., HOLTZWORTH-MUNROE, A., KATT, J. L., WOOD, L. F., & FOLLETTE,
V. M. (1989). Research-structured vs clinically flexible versions of social learning-based marital
therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 173–180.
JOHNSON, A. (1998). Clinical trials in psychiatry: background and statistical perspective. Statistical
Methods in Medical Research, 7, 209–234.
KANFER, F. H., & SASLOW, G. (1965). Behavioral analysis: An alternative to diagnostic classification.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 529–538.
KAVANAGH, D. (1992). Recent developments in expressed emotion and schizophrenia. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 160, 601–620.
KELLER, M. B., MCCULLOUGH, J. P., KLEIN, D. N., ARNOW, B., DUNNER, D. L., GELENBERG, A. J.,
MARKOWITZ, J. C., NEMEROFF, C. B., RUSSELL, J. M., THASE, M. E., TRIVEDI, M. H., & ZAJECKA, J.
(2000). A comparison of nefazodone, the cognitive behavioural-analysis system of psychotherapy,
and their combination for the treatment of chronic depression. New England Journal of Medicine,
342, 1462–1470.
KESSLER, R. C., SONNEGA, A., BROMET, E., HUGHES, M., & NELSON, C. B. (1995). Posttraumatic stress
disorder in the National Comorbidity survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 1048–1060.
KINDERMAN, P., & BENTALL, R. (1997). Attributional therapy for paranoid delusions: A case study.
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 25, 269–280.
LANG, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512.
LEFF, J. P., & VAUGHAN, C. E. (1985). Expressed emotion in families. New York: Guilford Press.
LEFF, J. P., VEARNALS, S., BREWIN, C. R., WOLFF, G., ALEXANDER, B., ASEN, E., DAYSON, D., JONES,
E., CHISHOLM, D., & EVERITT, B. (2000). The London Depression Intervention Trial: A randomised
controlled trial of antidepressants v couple therapy of people with depression living with a partner:
Clinical outcomes and costs. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 95–100.
Case formulation: an integrative approach 327
MCCARTHY, E., TARRIER, N., & GREGG, L. (2002). The nature and timing of seasonal affective symp-
toms and the influence of self-esteem and social support: A longitudinal prospective study. Manu-
script submitted for publication.
MEICHENBAUM, D. H. (1977). Cognitive-behavior modification: An intergrative approach. New York:
Plenum.
MULLEN, P. E., MARTIN, J. C., ANDERSON, J. C., ROMANS, S. E., & HERBISON, G. P. (1996). The long
term impact of the physical, emotional and sexual abuse of children: A community study. Child
Abuse and Neglect, 20, 7–21.
NELSON-GRAY, R. O., & FARMER, R. F. (1999). Behavioral assessment of personality disorders. Behavi-
our Research and Therapy, 37, 347–368.
NEWTH, S., & RACHMAN, S. (2001). The concealment of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
39, 457–464.
NUECHTERLEIN, K. H. (1987). Vulnerability models for schizophrenia: State of the art. In H. Hafner,
W. F. Gattaz, & W. Janzarik (Eds.), Search for the cause of schizophrenia (pp. 297–316).
Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
OSTROM, T. M. (1984). The sovereignty of social cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.),
Handbook of social cognition (vol. 1, pp. 1–37). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
OWENS, R. C., & ASHCROFT, J. B. (1982). Functional analysis in applied psychology. British Journal
of Clinical Psychology, 21, 181–189.
PENN, D. L., COMBS, D., & MOHAMED, S. (in press). Social cognition and social functioning in schizo-
phrenia. In P. W. Corrigan & D. L. Penn (Eds.), Social cognition in schizophrenia. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
PENN, D. L., MUESER, K. T., TARRIER, N., GLOEGE, A., & SERRANO, D. (2001). Supportive therapy for
schizophrenia: A closer look at the evidence. Manuscript submitted for publication.
PERSONS, J. B. (1986). The advantages of studying psychological phenomena rather than psychiatric
diagnoses. American Psychologist, 41, 1252–1260.
PERSONS, J. B. (1989). Cognitive therapy in practice: A case formulation approach. New York:
Norton.
PERSONS, J. B. (1991). Psychotherapy outcome studies do not accurately represent current models of
psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 46, 99–106.
PERSONS, J. B. (1992). A case formulation approach to cognitive-behavior therapy: Application to
panic disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 22, 470–473.
PERSONS, J. B., & BERTAGNOLLI, A. (1999). Inter-rater reliability of cognitive-behavioral case formula-
tion of depression: A replication. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23, 271–284.
PERSONS, J. B., CURTIS, J. T., & SILBERSCHATZ, G. (1991). Psychodynamic and cognitive-behavioral
formulations of a single case. Psychotherapy, 28, 608–617.
PERSONS, J. B., MOONEY, K. A., & PADESKY, C. A. (1995). Inter-rater reliability of cognitive-behavioral
case formulations. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 21–34.
PERSONS, J. B., & TOMPKIN, M. A. (1997). Cognitive-behavior case formulation. In T. D. Eells (Ed.),
Handbook of psychotherapy case formulation. New York: Guilford Press.
PITSCHEL-WALZ, G., LEUCHT, S., BAUML, J., KISSLING, W., & ENGEL, R. R. (2001). The effect of family
interventions on relapse and rehospitalisation in schizophrenia – meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bul-
letin, 27, 73–92.
PRICE, J. (2000). Subordination, self-esteem and depression. In L. Sloman & P. Gilbert (Eds.), Subor-
dination and defeat: An evolutionary approach to mood disorders and their therapy. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
RHODES, G. L., & LAKEY, B. (1999). Social support and psychological disorder: Insights from social
psychology. In, R. M. Kowalski & M. R. Leary (Eds.), The social psychology of emotional and
behavioural problems: Interfaces of social and clinical psychology (pp. 281–309). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
328 N. Tarrier and R. Calam
ROBINS, L. N. (1991). Conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 193–214.
ROSEN, G. M., LOHR, J. M., MCNALLY, R. J., & HERBERT, J. D. (1999). Power therapies: Evidence or
miraculous claims. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 27, 9–12.
RUNTZ, M. G., & SCHALLOW, J. R. (1997). Social support and coping strategies as mediators of adult
adjustment following childhood maltreatment. Child Abuse and Neglect, 21, 211–226.
SCHULTE, D. (1996). Tailor-made and standardized therapy: Complementary task in behavior therapy:
a contrarian view. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 27, 119–126.
SCHULTE, D., KUNZEL, R., PEPPING, G., & SCHULTE-BAHRENBERG, T. (1992). Tailor-made versus stand-
ardised therapy of phobic patients. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 67–92.
TARRIER, N. (2001). What can be learned from clinical trials? Reply to Devilly and Foa (2001).
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 117–118.
TARRIER, N., BARROWCLOUGH, C., WARD, J., DONALDSON, C., & BURNS, A. (in press). Expressed
emotion and attributions in the carers of patients with Alzheimer’s disease: The effect on carer
burden. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.
TARRIER, N., & HADDOCK, G. (2002). Cognitive-behavior therapy for the treatment of schizophrenia:
A case formulation approach. In S. Hofman & M. Tompson (Eds.), Handbook of psychological
treatments for severe mental disorders. New York: Guilford Press.
TARRIER, N., SOMMERFIELD, C., & PILGRIM, H. (1999). The effect of the relatives’ level of expressed
emotion (EE) on the outcome of psychological treatment of PTSD patients. Psychological Medicine,
29, 801–812.
TARRIER, N., & TURPIN, G. (1992). Psychosocial factors, arousal and schizophrenic relapse: A review
of the psychophysiological data. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 3–11.
TARRIER, N., WELLS, A., & HADDOCK, G. (1998). Treating complex cases: The cognitive behavioural
therapy approach. Chichester: Wiley.
TARRIER, N., & WYKES, T. (2002). Measuring outcome in cognitive behaviour therapy trials: The
problem of comparison. In S. Day, S. Green & D. Machin (Eds.), Text book of clinical trials.
Chichester: Wiley.
TARRIER, N., YUSOPOFF, L., KINNEY, C., MCCARTHY, E., GLEDHILL, A., HADDOCK, G., & MORRIS, J.
(1988). Randomised controlled trial of intensive cognitive behaviour therapy for patients with
chronic schizophrenia. British Medical Journal, 317, 303–307.
TURKAT, D. (1985). Behavioral case formulation. New York: Plenum.
TURKAT, D. (1990). The personality disorders: A psychological approach to clinical management.
New York: Pergamon.
TURKINGTON, D., & SIDDLE, R. (2000). Improving understanding and coping in people with schizophre-
nia by changing attitudes. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Skills, 4, 300–320.
UCHINO, B. N., UNO, D., & HOLD-LUNSTAD, J. (1999). Social support, physiological processes and
health. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 145–148.
WEARDEN, A. J., TARRIER, N., BARROWCLOUGH, C., ZASTOWNY, T. R., & RAHILL, A. A. (2000). A
review of expressed emotion research in health care. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 633–666.
WELLS, A. (1997). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: A practical manual and conceptual guide.
Chichester: Wiley.
WILSON, G. T. (1978). Cognitive behavior therapy: Paradigm shift or passing phase. In J. Foreyt &
D. Rathjen (Eds.), Cognitive behavior therapy. New York: Plenum Press.
WORLD HEALTH ORGANISATION (1994). International classification of mental and behavioural dis-
orders (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.