Jakobson Metaphoric and Metonymic Poles
Jakobson Metaphoric and Metonymic Poles
Roman Jakobson
Abstract
Roman Jakobson is probably the last homo universalis in the human sciences, who
both developed a theory of the mind and applied it to a panoply of disciplines.
Jakobson sees the metaphoric and the metonymic poles as the two basic modes or
ways of thought reflected in general human behaviour and in language. The meta-
phoric is based upon substitution and similarity, the metonymic upon predication,
contexture and contiguity. These two ways of thought are linked, though not in this
paper, but in several other papers of his collected works, to the paradigmatic and
the syntagmatic axes of linguistic expressions. The metaphoric and the metonymic
poles do not only underlie metaphor and metonymy in language, but, in alternative
ways, phenomena in all possible fields. such as language impairments, especially
aphasia, child language acquisition, literature (similarity in poetry, contiguity in
the novel), Freud's psycho-analysis, literary and art schools, the history of painting
and art movements, folklore such as folk tales and wedding songs. In fact, Jakob-
son holds out a research challenge not only to linguistics, but to all areas of semi-
otics. [R.D.]
The varieties of aphasia are numerous and diverse, but all of them lie
between the two polar types just described [i.e. similarity and conti-
guity disorders]. Every form of aphasic disturbance consists in some
impairment, more or less severe, either of the faculty for substitution
or for combination, and, contexture. The former affliction involves a
deterioration of metalinguistic operations, while the latter damages
1. I ventured a few sketchy remarks on the metonymical turn in verbal art ("Pro
realizm u mystectvi," Vaplite, Kharkov, 1927, No.2; "Randbemerkungen zur
Prosa des Dichters Pasternak" Slavische Rundschau, VII, 1935), in painting
("'Futurizm" Iskusstvo, Moscow, Aug. 2, 1919), and in motion pictures
(Upadek filmu," Listy pro umeni a kritiku, I, Prague, 1933), but the crucial
problem of the two polar processes awaits a detailed investigation.
2. Cf. his striking essay "Dickens, Griffith, and We": S. Eisenstein, Izbrannye
stat'i (Moscow, 1950), 153 ff.
3. Cf. B. Balazs, Theory ofthe Film (London, 1952).
4. For the psychological and sociological aspects of this dichotomy, see Bateson's
views on "progressional" and "selective integration" and Parsons' on the
"conjunction-disjunction dichotomy" in child development: J. Ruesch and o.
Bateson, Communication, the Social Matrix of Psychiatry (New York, 1951),
the two poles is still neglected, despite its wide scope and importance
for the study of any symbolic behaviour, especially verbal, and of its
impairments. What is the main reason for this neglect?
Similarity in meaning connects the symbols of a metalanguage
with the symbols of the language referred to. Similarity connects a
metaphorical term with the tenn for which it is substituted. Conse-
quently, when constructing a metalanguage to interpret tropes, the
researcher possesses more homogeneous means to handle metaphor,
whereas metonymy, based on a different principle, easily defies in-
terpretation. Therefore nothing comparable to the rich literature on
metaphor8 can be cited for the theory of metonymy. For the same
reason, it is generally realised that romanticism is closely linked with
metaphor, whereas the equally intimate ties of realism with meton-
ymy usually remain unnoticed. Not only the tool of the observer but
also the object of observation is responsible for the preponderance of
metaphor over metonymy in scholarship. Since poetry is focused
upon the sign, and pragmatical prose primarily upon the referent,
tropes and figures were studied mainly as poetic devices. The princi-
ple of similarity underlies poetry; the metrical parallelism of lines, or
the phonic equivalence of rhyming words prompts the question of
semantic similarity and contrast; there exist, for instance, grammati-
cal and anti-grammatical but never agrammatical rhymes. Prose, on
the contrary, is forwarded essentially by contiguity. Thus, for poetry,
metaphor, and for prose, metonymy is the line of least resistance and,
consequently, the study of poetical tropes is directed chiefly toward
metaphor. The actual bipolarity has been artificially replaced in these
studies by an amputated, unipolar scheme which, strikingly enough,
coincides with one of the two aphasic patterns, namely with the con-
tiguity disorder.