Countertransference: The Emerging Common Ground Glen Gabbard
Countertransference: The Emerging Common Ground Glen Gabbard
, (76):475-485
Glen Gabbard
In his search for common ground, Wallerstein (1990) noted that there are fewer
differences in technique than in theory among the diverse psychoanalytic
traditions. He proposed that systematic attention to the core psychoanalytic
phenomena of transference and resistance shows that they are probably more
similar than different in the clinical setting, regardless of one's theoretical
perspective. In this communication I am proposing that recent psychoanalytic
writings suggest another area of emerging common ground—namely, the
understanding of countertransference. Abend (1989) recently acknowledged
that the notion that an analyst's countertransference can be a crucial source of
information about the patient has now become widely accepted. This has
occurred in parallel with a gradual recognition that contemporary Kleinians and
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
classical ego psychologists have a good deal in common (Richards & Richards,
in press).
475
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
1964; Spillius, 1992). Some contemporary American contributors to the
literature on projective identification (Ogden, 1979, 1982, 1994; Scharff, 1992)
have called attention to a footnote in Klein's paper in which she stressed that
she prefers to conceptualise the projected contents as going into rather than on
to the mother. This attempt at clarification by Klein may be viewed as possibly
signalling an interpersonal dimension to the process. This perspective is further
bolstered by the usage of projective identification in her subsequent 1955
paper, ‘On identification’. Ogden (1994) pointed out that in the novella, If I Were
You, by Julian Green, which is the centrepiece of Klein's paper, there is a clear
implication that the target of the projection is transformed by the process.
On the other hand, Spillius (1992) did not interpret this usage to imply a change
in the external object as an integral part of projective identification. To the
extent that the analyst was influenced by the patient's behaviour, Klein
understood it to reflect countertransference in the narrow Freudian sense,
implying that the analyst needed further analysis. Spillius (1992) has argued
that Klein was not enthusiastic about the broadening of the concept to include
the analyst's emotional response to the patient's provocative behaviour, as
Heimann (1950) suggested. She was concerned that such a connotation might
allow analysts to blame their patients for their own countertransference
difficulties.
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
in the analytic situation, the analyst actually feels coerced by the patient into
playing a role in the patient's fantasy.
476
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
self-object-affect constellations in the internal world of the recipient, will
determine whether or not the projection is a good fit with the recipient. Even
when the countertransference response is experienced by analysts as an alien
force sweeping over them, what is actually happening is that a repressed self-
or object-representation has been activated by the interpersonal pressure of
the patient. Hence, the analyst's usual sense of a familiar, continuous self has
been disrupted by the emergence of these repressed aspects of the self.
Symington (1990) has described this process as one in which the patient ‘bullies’
the analyst into thinking the patient's thoughts rather than the analyst's own
thoughts.
477
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
Some critics (Kernberg, 1987; Porder, 1987; Sandler, 1987) of the broadened
conceptualisation of projective identification have felt that the original Kleinian
notion has been extended too far and distorted in the process. Kernberg (1987)
argued for a narrower definition that includes projecting intolerable aspects of
intrapsychic experience on to the analyst, maintaining empathy with the
projected contents, attempting to control the analyst in the service of defensive
efforts, and unconsciously inducing feelings in the object that correspond to
what has been projected in the here-and-now interaction with the analyst. He
felt that extending it to include the analyst's intrapsychic elaboration of the
projected contents and the return of what has been projected in the form of an
interpretation is unwarranted.
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
a role derived from the patient's intrapsychic world. Spillius (1992) has
characterised Sandler's concept of actualisation as a colloquial term for the
same process described by Joseph (1989), in which the patient unconsciously
induces feelings in the analyst and nudges the analyst into acting in concert
with the projection. Sandler (1993) regarded this form of identification with the
fantasised object as more or less the same as Racker's (1968) notion of
complementary countertransference. He distinguished it from a process of
primary identification, an automatic mirroring process that underlies analytic
empathy. Sandler underscored that any intense emotional reaction by the
analyst to the patient's words or behaviour is not projective identification
‘unless it is unconsciously intended to evoke such a reaction in the analyst’
(1993, p.1105). In this regard he sharpened the definition to avoid a tendency in
the literature to ascribe any intense countertransference feeling to a state that
is induced by the patient.
Countertransference enactment
478
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
When enactments are narrowed further to a focus on countertransference
enactments, the connection to projective identification becomes clear, as in
Chused's (1991) definition: ‘Enactments occur when an attempt to actualize a
transference fantasy elicits a countertransference reaction’ (pp. 629). Boesky
(Panel, 1992) noted the similarities between enactment and projective
identification, and he suggested that detailed study of enactments might allow
for a better understanding of how projective identification works. Chused
(Panel, 1992) stressed that implicit in the notion of projective identification is
that any analyst would respond in approximately the same manner to specific
behaviour or material in the patient. Countertransference enactments, on the
other hand, assume that the intrapsychic meaning of an interaction in the
analysis could be entirely different for different analysts, who might then
behave differently when presented with the same material by the same patient.
McLaughlin (1991; Panel, 1992) suggested that in projective identification the
analyst is viewed as virtually empty and is simply a receptacle or container for
what the patient is projecting.
The distinctions made by Chused and McLaughlin may be more apparent than
real. As noted previously, modern Kleinians such as Spillius (1992) and Joseph
(1989) share the same concern that it would be inappropriate to assume that all
of the analyst's feelings derive from the patient. They would agree with
Chused's perspective that individual variations in the analyst might result in
different countertransference enactments or different variants of projective
identification.
It is true that more classical analysts, when writing about enactments, often
focus to a greater extent on countertransference in the narrow sense, i.e.
experiences from their own past that are revived in the interaction with the
patient (Jacobs, 1986). However, most would agree with the Kleinian notion that
the analyst's countertransference may convey important information about the
patient (Abend, 1989). As Jacobs has noted,
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
Roughton also regarded countertransference enactments and projective
identification as strikingly similar. He made a distinction, however, between an
enactment, which simply involves putting an experience into behaviour, and
actualisation, which he sees as
479
Chused (1991) has noted the value of enacting certain impulses within the
analytic frame, only to catch oneself and retrospectively examine what
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
happened. She stressed, however, that the value for the analysis is not in the
enactment itself but rather in the observations and eventual understanding
that derive from those enactments. Jacobs (1993b) has taken a middle course in
which he has said that both experience and insight operate together and
cannot truly be separated from one another.
Relational theorists, such as Mitchell (1988, 1993), Aron (1991), Hirsch (1993,
1994), and Tansey (1994) have arrived at similar conclusions about the
inevitability and usefulness of countertransference enactments. Mitchell, for
example, in pointing out the similarities between his view and those of Sandler,
Gill, Racker, and Levinson, has made the following observation:
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
roles even if he desperately tries to stand outside the patient's system
and play no role at all (1988, p. 292).
He went on to emphasise that unless the analyst enters into the patient's
relational world, the analytic experience will not be optimal.
480
Intersubjectivity
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
analytic third. Projective identification negates the subjectivity of both
analysand and analyst while simultaneously reappropriating both subjectivities
to create a newly integrated ‘third’, a new ‘subject’ of the projective
identification process. A clear implication of this view is that a mutual projective
identification process is going on in both parties. Another implication is that the
portion of the analyst's psychic reality occupied by the countertransference is to
a large extent a new creation.
Discussion
481
scripted by the patient's internal world. The exact dimensions of this role,
however, will be coloured by the analyst's own subjectivity and the ‘goodness of
fit’ between the patient's projected contents and the analyst's internal
representational world.
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
The similarities between projective identification as used in contemporary
psychoanalytic writing, role-responsiveness, and countertransference
enactment have been observed by a number of authors (Gabbard, 1994a, b, c;
Mc-Laughlin, 1991; Roughton, 1993; Spillius, 1992). Even Kernberg, who has
objected to Ogden's view of projective identification nevertheless has
recognised the induction of countertransference responses by the patient:
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
Other analysts (Abend, 1989; Chused, 1992; McLaughlin, 1991; Renik, 1993)
have stressed that it is the interpretive working through of the enactment that
ultimately helps the patient to change. Still others (Cooper, 1992; Jacobs, 1993b;
Ogden, 1989; Pulver, 1992) have argued that it is not an either/or proposition.
Both the events occurring in the relationship and the interpretations resulting
from those events work synergistically to produce psychic change.
482
References
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
Bion, W.R. (1957). Differentiation of the psychotic from the non-psychotic
personalities. Int. J. Psychoanal., 38: 266-275; also in Second Thoughts.
London: Heinemann, 1967, pp. 43-64.
Bion, W.R. (1958). On arrogance. Int. J. Psychoanal., 39: 144- 146; and in Second
Thoughts. London: Heinemann, 1967, pp. 86-92.
Bion, W.R. (1959). Attacks on linking. Int. J. Psychoanal., 40: 308-315; and in
Second Thoughts. London: Heinemann, 1967, pp. 93-100.
483
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
Gabbard, G. O.( 1994b). On love and lust in erotic transference.J. Am. Psychoanal.
Assoc., 42: 385-403.
Joseph, B. (1989). Psychic Equilibrium and Psychic Change: Selected Papers of Betty
Joseph, ed. M. Feldman & E. B. Spillius. London & New York: Routledge.
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
Klein, M. (1946). Notes on some schizoid mechanisms. In Envy and Gratitude and
Other Works, 1946-1963. New York: Delacorte Press/Seymour Laurence, 1975,
pp. 1-24.
484
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
Renik, O. (1993). Analytic interaction: conceptualizing technique in light of the
analyst's irreducible subjectivity. Psychoanal. Q., 62: 553-571.
Scharff, J. S. ( 1992). Projective and Introjective Identification and the Use of the
Therapist's Self. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
Segal, H. ( 1964). An introduction to the Work of Melanie Klein. New York: Basic
Books.
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
Weiss, J., Sampson, H. the Mount Zion Psychotherapy Research Group ( 1986).
The Psychoanalytic Process: Theory, Clinical Observation and Empirical
Research. New York: Guilford Press.
Authorized Users
For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms
& conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).
PEP-Web Copyright
1. All copyright (electronic and other) of the text, images, and photographs of the
publications appearing on PEP-Web is retained by the original publishers of the Journals,
Books, and Videos. Saving the exceptions noted below, no portion of any of the text,
images, photographs, or videos may be reproduced or stored in any form without prior
permission of the Copyright owners.
2. Authorized Uses. Authorized Users may make all use of the Licensed Materials as is
consistent with the Fair Use Provisions of United States and international law. Nothing in
this Agreement is intended to limit in any way whatsoever any Authorized User's rights
under the Fair Use provisions of United States or international law to use the Licensed
Materials.
3. During the term of any subscription the Licensed Materials may be used for purposes of
research, education or other non-commercial use as follows:
1. Digitally Copy. Authorized Users may download and digitally copy a
reasonable portion of the Licensed Materials for their own use only.
2. Print Copy. Authorized Users may print (one copy per user) reasonable potions
of the Licensed Materials for their own use only.
Copyrighted Material. For use only by apm339. Reproduction prohibited. Usage subject to PEP terms & conditions (see terms.pep-web.org).