This document discusses the psychoanalytic concept of latency in children ages 5-10. It describes how during early latency from ages 5-8, children experience strong conflicts between their ego, id, and newly formed superego. Symptoms like anxiety and insomnia can arise as defenses form. Children are aware of emotional distress and may be open to analysis. In later latency from 8-10, conflicts decrease as sexual demands lessen and the superego becomes less rigid. Children consolidate defenses and focus more on reality. They resist analysis due to a fear of disrupting their fragile equilibrium.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes)
279 views
Bornstein, B. - On Latency
This document discusses the psychoanalytic concept of latency in children ages 5-10. It describes how during early latency from ages 5-8, children experience strong conflicts between their ego, id, and newly formed superego. Symptoms like anxiety and insomnia can arise as defenses form. Children are aware of emotional distress and may be open to analysis. In later latency from 8-10, conflicts decrease as sexual demands lessen and the superego becomes less rigid. Children consolidate defenses and focus more on reality. They resist analysis due to a fear of disrupting their fragile equilibrium.
To cite this article: Berta Bornstein (1951) On Latency, The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 6:1, 279-285, DOI: 10.1080/00797308.1952.11822916
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00797308.1952.11822916
Published online: 13 Feb 2017.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 1
View related articles
Citing articles: 17 View citing articles
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=upsc20 ON LATENCyl By BERTA BORNSTEIN (New York)
From the standpoint of the intellectual ability of the child in
latency, we could expect the child to associate freely. The factors re- sponsible for the child's failure to do so create a general limitation of child analysis. There are several reasons for his inability to associate. In addition to those well known to us, I will mention only one which has not been: stressed yet: Free association is experienced by the child as a particular threat to his ego organization. The use of free association is actually a regression to the primary process. In early latency the child is still close to the period when his mind was mainly dominated by the pleasure-principle and conscious and unconscious contents were not yet strictly separated. It is only with great difficulty that the child learns that contradictions exclude each other and that contradictory thoughts must be kept apart in conversa- tion. For the sake of adults, the child behaves as though he were living by the rules of the secondary process. However, listening to children five to eight years old, when they are engaged in conversations with each other, convinces us that conscious production does not necessarily mean constant utilization of the secondary process. Their conscious . thought processes (on the surface similar to ours) can during the latency period still easily dip into the primary process. Any newly acquired ac- complishment can easily be undone by regressive processes and the child senses that this would occur were he to attempt to express what- ever comes into his mind. Therefore the child must fight against free association more than the adult. A particularly strong anticathexis is needed to safeguard the hardly achieved intactness of ego functioning. Free association is possible only after children have gradually developed the capacity for introspection. This hardly ever occurs before pre- puberty when the superego approaches a state of consolidation.
Before we speak about latency and the technique applied during
that period, let me review briefly the main factors which precede latency. 1. Paper read at the Panel on Child Analysis. held at the Annual Meeting of the American psychoanalytic Association in Cincinnati on May 5, 1951. 279 280 BERTA BORNSTEIN The ego as a mediator between the inner and outer world adopts at an early point defensive measures against painful stimuli from within and without. Under the influence of reality, the ego is enabled gradually to tolerate greater amounts of tension. The open pursuit of the child's gratifications is hindered by the parents' opposition. The growing functions of intellect and judgment assist the child further to postpone gratifications and to block impulses from direct discharge. A partial resolution of the oedipus complex leads, via the identification with the objects of the oedipus complex, to the establishment of the superego. From now on the ego has to observe not only the demands of instinctual drives and of the outside world, but also the demands coming from the superego. This means that certain demands which originally were only complied with under the pressure of the parents or their substitutes, are now complied with even if there is no threat of external danger. With the resolution of the oedipus complex and with the estab- lishment of the superego, the latency period is introduced. Although it is common practice to refer to the latency period as if it were uniform, at least two major divisions within it can be discerned; the first from five and one half to eight years, and the second from eight until about ten years. There are, of course, more than chronological differences be- tween them. The element common to both is the strictness of the super- ego in its evaluation of incestuous wishes-a strictness which finds ex- pression in the child's struggle against masturbation. Let me now describe the characteristics of the first period of latency. The ego, still buffeted by the surging impulses, is threatened by the new superego which is not only harsh and rigid but still a foreign body. This first phase of latency is complicated because of the intermingling of two different sets of defenses: the defense against genital and the de- fense against pregenital impulses. As a defense against genital impulses a temporary regression to pregenitality is adopted by the ego. First these pregenital drives appear as less dangerous than the genital ones. Still they are threatening enough for the child to have to evolve new de- fenses against the pregenital impulses. Reaction formations, developed as defense against the pregenital impulses, mark the first character changes in early latency. The result of the conflicts between the superego and the drives can be observed in a heightened ambivalence. This increased ambivalence is a regular feature of early latency, even if the child is not in the process of developing an obsessive neurosis. The ambivalence is ex- pressed in the child's behavior by an alternation between obedience and rebellion: and rebellion is usually followed by self-reproach. However, ON LATENCY 281 at this time of life, the child can tolerate his own feelings of guilt as little as he can tolerate criticism from the outside, nor is his behavior modified right away by either. Anna Freud has described what happens at "this intermediate stage of superego development": The attempt to internalize the criticism from the outside sometimes does not lead further than to an identification with the aggressor, "often supple- mented by another defensive measure, namely the projection of guilt." Both defenses in turn thrust the child into greater inner and outer conflicts. It appears to me that the statement frequently made that infantile neurosis decreases during latency requires some modification. It is cor- rect as far as the second period is concerned, but it does not correspond to my own experience as far as the first period is concerned. When the ego is faced by conflicts it cannot overcome during the first period behavior difficulties arise and neurotic symptoms manifest themselves in new ways. To give a few examples: Early animal phobias are re- placed by a new wave of separation anxiety and open castration fear is substituted by fear of death. The symptom of insomnia occurs more frequently during that period than is generally known. Some children in early latency give the appearance of being in an emergency situation; they are conscious of their emotional distress and under such conditions they are ready to accept the analyst as a potential helper. Though they usually expect instantaneous relief and become disappointed and distrustful if this does not occur, they can be very co-operative during treatment. Due to the facts that the child is aware of his suffering, that the ego is in rebellion against both id and super- ego, that the libido is still in a fluid state and the superego still open to modification, and thus that the ego is not yet completely crippled by neurotic defenses, therapeutic chances seem to be better in early latency than at any other time. In the second period of latency the situation is different: The ego is exposed to less severe conflicts by virtue of the facts that, on the one hand, the sexual demands have become less exerting and, on the other, the superego has become less rigid. The ego now can devote itself to a greater extent to coping with reality. The average eight-year-old is ready to be influenced by the children around him and by adults other than his parents. As he is able to compare them with other adults, his belief in the omnipotence of his parents subsides. Coinciding with a partial degradation of parents, there is a parallel change in the attitude of the ego toward the superego. Even if this period is not quite as smooth as described, even if 282 BEllT A BORNSTEIN children manifest character disturbances, ego restrictions or slight ob- sessive symptoms, these symptoms are ego-syntonic. During the second period, the temptation to masturbate is not completely overcome but the child is so sincerely opposed to the temp- tation as well as to the occasional break-throughs, that he must deny or repress both. His concern to forego the masturbatory temptations is accompanied by the desire that defenses should not be upset by any interference. Since he is further along in the process of consolidation of ego defenses than the younger latency child, and is more oriented to coping with problems of the outside world, and since he has more gratifications in reality, the older latency child is less aware of his suf- fering. He fears nothing more than the upsetting of his precarious equilib- rium. The fear of upsetting this equilibrium becomes the decisive force in his resistance toward analysis. The child's distrust of the enmity toward the analyst is thus often a displacement of his enmity and dis- trust of his instinctual impulses. We repeat: during both periods of latency, neurotic children see as a principle task the warding off of incestuous fantasies and mastur- batory temptations. They accomplish this task by means of partial regression. The ego during this period is engaged in deflecting the sexual energy from its pregenital aims and is utilizing it for sublima- tion and reaction formation. But in neither periods do they fully suc- ceed and a close-up of this period shows the ego in a ceaseless, though quickly repressed, battle against the temptation to masturbate. It ap- pears to us that adult patients give a distorted picture of their latency. They are inclined to remember this period as one in which they had in reality attained what Freudt described as the ideal of latency: the suc- cessful warding off of instinctual demands. The impression retained of latency period is understandable when one takes into account with what amazing rapidity (even in analysis) children repress or deny the occasional breakthroughs of their masturbatory activities. The child's behavior during the latency period might be described as one of per- sistent denial of the struggle against the breakthrough of instinctual impulses, a denial which extends into adulthood as a partial amnesia for this period. This may be one of the reasons why one learns relatively little about latency from the analysis of adults.s The form of analytic technique with latency children must be in 2. "Three Contributions to the Theory of Sex," in The Basic Writings of Sig. mund Freud, Modern Library. New YO!"k, 1938. 3. We find exceptions in those adults whose latency was disturbed by a severe obsessive neurosis. ON LATENCY 285 accordance with the specific characteristics of the psychic structure we have just described. Because the child battles against his impulses and needs to keep up his defenses, we must be particularly careful to respect his resistance, and to work through his defenses before we approach the material which is warded off. We know how difficult it is for a child in latency to tell us anything about his inner life. We have learned to utilize substitutes for free associations, such as play, drawings and stories, which enable us to draw conclusions about id contents, but unlike the adult's free association, these substitute media do not, to the same extent, furnish material on defenses or their genesis. Whatever these media represent, we do not use them for the interpretation of id contents, but as a source of knowledge of the child, and as a stepping stone toward the analysis of defense and of affects. Since free association is not applicable in latency, defense analysis is more complicated in the analysis of children than in that of an adult. We are forced to search for defenses by microscopic observation of the total behavior of the child.
So far my remarks were theoretical. Let me now turn to an example, Il-
lustrating the technique of defense analysis as it was employed in the handling of a daydream a ten-year-old boy told during his treatment. The patient's char- acter disturbance found expression in his complicated relationship with his father and his brothers with whom he competed and toward whom he harbored strong passive desires. Like most children he was apt to forget painful experi- ences in reality. His daydreams were important for us not only because they represented a superstructure of a masturbatory fantasy. but because their appearance in the analytic situation signaled a current humiliating experience and permitted the reconstruction of his reaction to such experiences. After a period of analysis in which we had worked on his desire to com- pete with his father and his older brothers which at times had not led further than to an identification with their gestures. he made a conscious effort to com- bat his display of competition. At the beginning of his analysis, he openly played at being important men like generals and admirals; now, in his daydreams, he revealed a modification of these wishes. Motivated by the desire to please the analyst, he made a strong point of telling me that in his daydream he himself was a ten-year-old boy and that in reality he no longer sought to compete with his father but wanted to remain a boy of his own age. In his daydreams, however, young as he was, he was a famous brain surgeon, and had also discovered a cure for cancer. He attended school during the day, of course, but nevertheless General Eisenhower had heard that our patient was a famous brain surgeon and ordered the boy night after night to the battle field to perform his famous operations on outstanding generals. The brains of those generals were shattered by bullets or their lives were endangered by brain 284 BERTA BORNSTEIN tumors. It was through his restoring the generals' mental capacity that the United States won the war. Up to this point his daydream emphasized that nobody at school knew about his fame as a brain surgeon. One day a variation of the daydream oc- curred. A policeman entered the class room and asked about a car that was not parked correctly. It turned out to be our patient's car; in this way everyone at school suddenly learned that he not only had, as he said, a "doctor's certificate," but in recognition of his outstanding services, he had also been granted a driver's license. Our knowledge of his tendencies to react to slight narcissistic injuries with ideas of greatness made us inquire about a defeat at school. We asked whether anything had happened at school, whether anyone had offended him, etc. He told us,' though not immediately, that a man teacher had com- mented on his continuous yawning during class and that it was at this moment that his exhibitionism broke through in his daydream.
As we said before, our interest in a daydream is not aimed at im-
mediately reaching the masturbatory fantasy which it elaborates. What we take out of it, is a knowledge of the typical defenses and the reac- tion to affects. Although this would be true of almost any production of the child, I have selected the daydream precisely because it is so close to the unconscious; yet it should be used to deal with defense and affect rather than with the instinctual impulses. Since defense and affect are closer to the ego than the impulse, we are able, through them, to make interpretations which the child can recognize and accept without undue resistance. Once a defense appears, we can assume that it is typical for particular situations, and that the identical affect or impulse is present whenever the defense reappears. This being so, whenever a defense is noticed, one can bring to the child's attention the event and the affect to which he had reacted. What has been said about defenses applies in a general way to affects. Wherever we observe an inappropriate expression of affect, we can assume that the ego has intervened. As Anna Freud' said: from the transformations which the affect has undergone, we can deduce the specific defenses used against them and we can also assume that the same defenses are used against the instinctual impulses which originally gave rise to these affects. Let us return to the daydream and scrutinize it from the standpoint of defense and affect. Our young daydreamer, we have seen, reacted to a painful reality situation by denial in fantasy. He was not the little boy whom a teacher could scold, but an important surgeon who wielded the power of life and death over the commanders of many men. Another element in his reaction to the reprimand was a feeling of shame. The shame could not be consciously admitted, because of its •. The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defense. Int. Univ. Press, New York, 1946. ON LATENCY 285 associative connection with his passivity. In his daydream, he reverses the shame into glory. The shame was secret, the glory was public. When- ever our patient used the mechanism of reversal, we were sure that be- hind it was an instinctual impulse which clamored for discharge, and against which the boy rebelled. For instance, the daydream in which our patient performs nocturnal brain surgery on generals becomes mean- ingful if understood as representing its opposite. You will not be sur- prised to hear that the daydream took form in a period in which the boy fought against his identification with women. His surgical activity, and his removal of foreign bodies from the head, was the opposite of his unconscious wish to be a woman who gives birth to a child,s There were other affects involved which had to be examined. In a school situation, in which a teacher reprimanded a pupil, we would expect the child to experience some anger. This the child did not. No anger had appeared. It was obvious that he consciously could not toler- ate any aggression against men. In order to prevent the impulse from appearing, the appropriate affect had to be repressed. Asked about how he had really felt, the child answered good-humoredly: "I really was 'not angry, I had fun with my daydream. I would really like to become a brain surgeon." Some time after the boy spoke scornfully about his daydream: "Gee, I'm really a fool, here I am talking about curing the generals from brain tumors, and 1 don't even know anything about the brain, how the nerves are working and the blood cells, and what makes the heart beat, and what makes a man's muscles hard as bones." His thirst for knowledge, the desire to learn about physiology and anatomy, remained untouched by our analytic interpretation, although they were rooted in the same conflicts that showed up in the daydream. In concluding I should like to emphasize that I have discussed pri- marily the neurotic child and his latency. All children in latency, how- ever, not only the neurotic ones, use their free energies for character development. Therefore it is particularly important during latency not to interfere with healthy character formation. The utmost care has to be exercised in the analysis of latency to strengthen weak structures and to modify those which interfere with normal development. The selec- tion of material for interpretation and the form of interpretation itself must be geared to these ends. 5. This unconscious wish was interpreted in other connections.
The Groups Book Psychoanalytic Group Therapy Principles Practice With The Groups Manual A Treatment Manual With Clinical Vignettes 1st Edition Caroline Garland All Chapters Instant Download
(Suny Series, Transforming Subjects Psychoanalysis, Culture,) Deborah P. Britzman - A Psychoanalyst in the Classroom on the Human Condition in Education-State University of New York Press (2015)